Introduction

There is a consensus regarding the key role of the cerebellum in motor tasks, but its involvement in cognitive and affective functions is not well known1,2. Physiological and neuroimaging studies have shown cerebellar activation in non-motor tasks3,4,5,6,7, and also some cerebellar impairments have been related to different psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders, such as schizophrenia, autism spectrum disorders or Niemann-Pick disease8,9,10,11,12,13. The few animal studies so far performed indicate that the severity of the cerebellar abnormalities directly determine the extent to which cognition is affected14,15. Thus, due to the lack of time-course studies it is not clear how the nature of cerebellar defects influences the performance of non-motor behaviors16. Previous works have demonstrated the importance of both migration and morphogenesis of Purkinje cells (PCs) for correct cerebellar function17,18, but the repercussion of progressive alterations of the cerebellar circuitry on the development of non-motor behaviors is not well documented.

Microtubules are essential for normal neuronal development and function; they change their structure and dynamics during cellular development and fate. In the brain, this molecular plasticity is necessary for neuronal migration, but also enables neurons to modify their morphology during development and to maintain this morphology as mature neurons17,18,19. Microtubular alterations have been linked to different cognitive disorders, as well as to cerebellar defects13,20,21,22,23. In this sense, microtubule dynamics has been proposed to be the basis of the cerebellar destruction in different animal models, such as the Purkinje Cell Degeneration (PCD) mutant mouse24,25. However, it is not known how microtubule dynamics shape cerebellar structure and function along postnatal development.

In this context, the PCD mouse provides a good model to study the impact of cerebellar disorders on social and cognitive disorders. The PCD mouse suffers a mutation in the Nna1 gene, which encodes cytosolic carboxypeptidase 1 (CCP1), an enzyme responsible for the de-polyglutamylation of microtubules24. This mutation induces the degeneration of the Purkinje cells (PCs) of the cerebellum, but its effect on microtubule dynamics and structure remains to be determined. The progression of the death of PCs has been previously characterized and divided into two different stages: (1) an initial stage with cytoplasmic and nuclear alterations (from postnatal day 15, P15, to P18) and (2) a neurodegenerative process where all PCs die (P18–P45), leading to an aberrant cerebellum without the main projecting neurons26,27,28. Interestingly, the expression of Nna1 gene increases in the normal cerebellum from P15 to P2529, just when Purkinje cells dendritic arbors are re-modeling30,31. Thus, CCP1 seems to be closely related to Purkinje cells maturation. Furthermore, previous studies have demonstrated that no other neuronal populations are affected during cerebellar degeneration in the PCD mouse25,28,29, therefore making it a good model for studying the progression of alterations in the cerebellar structure and its influence on motor, cognitive and social behaviors. Also, since neuronal degeneration of the PCD mouse is known to be associated with microtubule defects32, this model allows us to understand, first, the effect of polyglutamylation on microtubules dynamics and structure and, second, how these alterations affect postnatal cognitive and social behaviors along the neurodegenerative process. Thus, in order to study the impact of the lack of the CCP1 enzyme on behavioral development and cerebellar histology we used this well-established model25,33,34. For the study of microtubules dynamics and morphology we generated a CCP1 KO mouse line that mimics the pcd mutation32.

The main goal of this work is to understand the influence of microtubule dynamics and structure on cerebellar development and function, especially in Purkinje cells. To do this, we first studied the effects the lack of a functional CCP1 enzyme has on microtubule dynamics and structure in a generated CCP1 KO mouse line. We then analyzed the impact of microtubule alterations on PCs morphology and on general cerebellar structure in the original PCD mouse, which has a spontaneous deletion of the CCP1 enzyme. Finally, we analyzed in these PCD mice the effect of progressive cerebellum degeneration on motor, cognitive, and social processes by means of an animal model lacking the CCP1 enzyme.

Results

Effect of pcd mutation on microtubule dynamics and structure

The lack of CCP1 induces an over-polyglutamylation of microtubules in the brain24, which may influence their interaction with different proteins including severing and motor proteins35,36. However, the effect on microtubule dynamics is poorly understood. To address this, we studied the microtubules of mouse embryonic fibroblasts (MEFs) lacking the CCP1 enzyme, which we named PCD microtubules (Fig. 1). Results showed (Table 1) that “growing rate” (p = 0.022) and “catastrophe frequency” (p = 0.012) were significantly increased in the PCD microtubules (Fig. 1A–C). Regarding microtubule curvature, PCD microtubules were more curved than the controls (p = 0.003; Fig. 1D–F). In parallel, the curvature of trajectories was also increased in PCD microtubules (p = 0.024; Fig. 1G–I). The lack of the CCP1 enzyme increased the instability of microtubules and influenced microtubule flexibility, thus affecting their trajectories.

Figure 1
figure 1

Effect of pcd mutation on microtubule dynamics and structure (mean ± SEM). (A and B) manual and automatic analyses of microtubules growth and shrinking rates; in both analysis PCD microtubules showed an increase in growing rate. (C) Catastrophe frequency analysis; an increase in the frequency of catastrophes in PCD microtubules is demonstrated. (DF) Analysis of WT (D) and PCD (E) microtubules curvature and its graphical representation (F); an increase in the curvature of PCD microtubules can be observed. (GI) Analysis of WT (G) and PCD (H) microtubule trajectory curvature and its graphical representation (I); an increase in the trajectory curvature of PCD microtubules can be observed. *p < 0.05.

Table 1 Quantification of WT and PCD microtubule dynamics and structure. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

Morphological changes in the main dendrite of PCs begin during the pre-neurodegeneration

Microtubules are essential for maintaining neuronal shape13,37. Therefore, to understand the impact of these microtubular defects, we analyzed the morphology of PCs (Fig. 2A; Table 2) during different stages of PCD postnatal development: (1) P7, where no defects have been previously described, (2) P15 and P17, to characterize the pre-neurodegenerative stage and (3) P22 and P30, to characterize the neurodegenerative stage, when PCs disappear. Results showed that morphological changes of the main dendrite length (P7, p = 0.483; P15, p = 0.393; P17, p = 0.028; P22, p = 0.043; P30, p = 0.027) and width (P7, p = 0.379; P15, p = 0.007; P17, p = 0.017; P22, p = 0.695; P30, p = 0.020) began during pre-neurodegeneration (Fig. 2B,C), both parameters decreased in PCD mice. By contrast, a reduction of the dendritic arbor length - molecular layer thickness - (P7, p = 0.361; P15, p = 0.064; P17, p = 0.704; P22, p = 0.041; P30, p = 0.030) and of the soma size (P7, p = 0.484; P15, p = 0.466; P17, p = 0.085; P22, p = 0.008; P30, p = 0.001) was observed later, during the neurodegenerative stage (Fig. 2D,E). Hence, microtubule instability may induce morphological alterations of the PCs throughout late postnatal stages of cerebellar development, before the beginning of neurodegeneration.

Figure 2
figure 2

Morphological analysis of the PCs during postnatal development (mean ± SEM). (A) Micrographs of the PCs at different ages as seen with anti-calbindin immunofluorescence (Cb-28k; red). (BE) Charts showing the quantification of the studied parameters; note that morphological alterations appear from P15 and onwards, with the soma area/size and the dendritic arbor length being the last to be significantly modified. n = 4 per experimental group. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

Table 2 Quantification of the PCs morphology of both WT and PCD mice. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

TUNEL analysis revealed general neuronal death in the cerebellum of PCD mice

The Nna1/Ccp1 gene mutation is known to affect all neuronal populations of the cerebellum24,25,27. However, apart from PC loss, the progression of cell death of other cerebellar neuronal populations of the PCD mouse has not been previously characterized. Therefore, we monitored the death of the three main neuronal populations of the cerebellum: the molecular layer cells, the PCs and the granular layer cells (Fig. 3). Results confirmed that the PCs began to die in the PCD mouse from P22 and onwards, resulting in a process of cerebellar degeneration (P7, p = 0.276; P15, p = 0.073; P17, p = 0.444; P22, p = 0.004; P30, p = 0.012; Fig. 3D. We also found an increase in cellular death during the same period in both the molecular (P7, p = 0.168; P15, p = 0.411; P17, p = 0.302; P22, p < 0.001; P30, p = 0.010; Fig. 3C) and granular layers (P7, p = 0.581; P15, p = 0.960; P17, p = 0.346; P22, p = 0.029; P30, p < 0.001; Fig. 3E). The neuronal nature of apoptotic cells in the three cerebellar layers was confirmed using TUNEL/NeuN and TUNEL/PV immunofluorescence colocalization (Fig. 3A,B). In summary, the initial symptoms of the breakdown of cerebellar structure are seen in the main dendrite of PCs. Following on, the morphological defects extend to the whole neuron, altering both the general dendritic arbor and soma. At the same time, there is a progressive and general neuronal loss in the other two layers resulting in a general breakdown of the cerebellum.

Figure 3
figure 3

Analysis of apoptosis along cerebellar postnatal development. (A) Micrograph of a Purkinje cell labeled with parvalbumin (PV, red) and TUNEL (green) in a PCD mouse. (B) Micrograph of an interneuron of the molecular layer labeled with NeuN (red) and TUNEL (green) in a PCD mouse. (CE) Charts showing the quantification of TUNEL-positive cells in the three cerebellar layers (mean ± SEM). An increase in the number of apoptotic cells can be observed from P22 and onwards in the three cerebellar layers of the PCD mice. n = 4 per experimental group. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

Motor coordination is only affected during the neurodegeneration

Data from the rota-rod test (Fig. 4A,B) showed that motor performance of the PCD mouse was not affected during pre-neurodegeneration (P15, p = 0.677 and P17, p = 0.398; Fig. 4B), but impaired during neurodegeneration; i.e. at P22 (p < 0.001) and P30 (p < 0.001; Fig. 4B). Thus, motor coordination is affected only during PC loss, but not prior to this.

Figure 4
figure 4

Analysis of motor coordination and home-cage behavior along postnatal development (mean ± SEM). (A,B) Representation and quantification of the rota-rod test. (CF) Representation and quantification of the parameters analyzed in the home-cage behavior test; i.e. time for grooming, time of environmental exploratory behavior and time displacing. Note the differences derived from the pcd mutation for all parameters, except for displacement. n = 8–9 per experimental group. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

Home-cage behavior is altered in PCD mice before PC death

Results of the home-cage behavior test showed no differences between genotypes regarding the time spent displacing, during any of the developmental stages, which indicated that the pcd mutation did not affect general movement (Fig. 4D). Concerning grooming time, no differences were found during pre-neurodegeneration (Fig. 4E), but it did decrease during neurodegeneration in the PCD mice (P15, p = 0.444; P17, p = 0.535; P22, p = 0.005; P30, p < 0.001). Interestingly, environmental exploratory behavior - rearings - was also reduced in PCD mice compared to the controls during both pre- (P15, p = 0.042; P17, p < 0.001) and neurodegeneration (P22, p = 0.985; P30, p = 0.005; Fig. 4F). Surprisingly, at P22 no changes were found in this exploratory behavior of PCD mice.

Long-term memory is only affected in late neurodegenerative stages

The results of the novel object recognition test showed that wild-type (WT) mice at all ages spent a large percentage of time exploring new rather than familiar objects (Fig. 5; P17, p = 0.016; P22, p = 0.004; P30, p = 0.001), indicating a normal memory recognition. Similarly, PCD mice also presented the same behavior at P17 (p = 0.016) and P22 (p = 0.010). However, at P30 no differences were detected in the PCD mice regarding the percentage of time spent exploring both types of objects (p = 0.497), suggesting a deficit in object memory recognition at this age.

Figure 5
figure 5

Analysis of the novel object recognition test along postnatal development. (A) Schematic representation of the test and the objects employed. (BD) Analysis of the percentage of time exploring familiar and novel objects at different ages for both WT and PCD mice (mean ± SEM); a lack of preference for the new object is seen in PCD mice at P30. (E) Quantification of the number of visits to both objects at P30; no differences were observed. n = 8–9 per experimental group. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

The number of visits to both objects at P30 was further analyzed to avoid possible biases derived from motor alterations in PCD mice. Results did not show differences between the genotypes (Fig. 5E), thus discarding any effects of ataxia in PCD mice, with respect to moving within the plastic box and exploring the objects, and validating the previous result.

Social preference is affected in PCD mice because of cerebellar pre-neurodegeneration

The results of the social preference test revealed that WT mice at all ages spent a larger percentage of time exploring the chamber containing other mouse rather than the chamber containing an object (P15, p = 0.028; P17, p = 0.042; P22, p = 0.001; P30, p < 0.001; Fig. 6), indicating a preference for social contact. However, PCD mice spent the same percentage of time exploring both chambers at all ages (P15, p = 0.305; P17, p = 0.328; P22, p = 0.272; P30, p = 0.812; Fig. 6). Thus, the pre-neurodegenerative changes observed prior to the breakdown of cerebellar structure seem to be enough to induce social deficits.

Figure 6
figure 6

Analysis of the social preference test during postnatal development. (A) Diagram of test performance. (BE) Charts showing quantification of the percentage of time spent exploring the chambers containing either the intruder animal or an object for both WT and PCD mice at different ages (mean ± SEM). Note that PCD mice, contrary to the WT, spent the same amount of time exploring both chambers. n = 8–9 per experimental group. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

In conclusion, the behavioral analysis of PCD mice strongly suggests a close relationship between cerebellar function and cognition. Moreover, the progression of the cerebellar alterations increasingly impaired the performance of cognitive and affective tasks.

Discussion

One of the main obstacles to establishing a relationship between the cerebellum and cognitive and social processes is the lack of studies analyzing the impact of progressive cerebellar defects on non-motor behavior. Also, this association has only been studied over short time frames. Here, to allow for a better understanding of this relationship, our results will be discussed chronologically based on their appearance throughout postnatal development in normal and PCD mice. The relevant order includes the first week of postnatal development (P7), then during the pre-neurodegenerative stage (P15–P17), and finally during the neurodegenerative stage (P22–P30). Thus, a temporal pattern is presented, characterizing the microtubule alterations observed in the PCD mouse, the progression of the cerebellar defects, and the influence of these alterations on motor and non-motor behaviors.

To understand the importance of the CCP1 enzyme in cerebellar development, the effect of the lack of this enzyme on microtubule dynamics and the structure of fibroblasts was initially studied, as the in vitro model available was able to provide insight into the structure and dynamics of this cellular component. While previous results have demonstrated that the lack of CCP1 enzyme increases microtubule polyglutamylation24, here it is shown that the lack of this de-polyglutamylase affects microtubule stability and flexibility. Microtubules are more unstable at the growth cones, which consequently increases their catastrophe frequency, an essential process for normal neuronal development38,39. Additionally, certain factors related to neuronal migration and development preferentially bind to curved microtubules40,41,42,43. Therefore, microtubule dynamics and curvature seem to have a different pattern during neuronal development and maturation, being more dynamic and curved during development and stable and rigid during maturation. The microtubules of PCD mice are likely very similar to those found in developing neurons, in terms of dynamics and structure. In WT mice, CCP1 expression increases at P15 in the cerebellum29, that is, at the beginning of the pre-neurodegeneration stage of the PCD mouse26,29. Precisely, during this short time frame, the growth of the dendritic arbor of PCs is completed and the process of maturation and remodeling takes place44,45. Thus, microtubule polyglutamylation seems to be essential during cerebellar development, and a reduction of this post-translational modification seems to be necessary at the end of neuronal growth.

Then, the microtubule over-polyglutamylation occurring in the PCD mouse triggers the pre-neurodegeneration and the loss of the PCs from P18 and onwards25,26,29. Conversely, at P7 no differences were observed in the morphology of the PCs between genotypes, the general structure of the cerebellum was apparently unaffected. Since neuronal migration and growth occur in the normal cerebellar cortex during this first postnatal week46, it appears that the pcd mutation (i.e., microtubule over-polyglutamylation) does not affect the initial perinatal stages of cerebellar cortex formation. CCP1 activity is extremely low during the first two postnatal weeks (it significantly increases at P15)29 thus, our data support the idea that microtubule polyglutamylation is essential during early cerebellar development, but deleterious for establishing a mature pattern. Therefore, the fine-tuning of microtubule polyglutamylation has a major role in cerebellar postnatal development.

In order to understand the influence of the cerebellum on cognitive and social behavior, different authors have analyzed the effects of different cerebellar alterations in animal models14,15. However, to our knowledge, there are no studies that have attempted to analyze progressive cerebellar alterations throughout postnatal development to understand the normal cerebellar function.

Morphological defects in the PCs of different psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders have been recently described47,48,49, but the direct effect of alterations in the morphology of PCs on non-motor behavior is not known. We demonstrate that prior to the breakdown of cerebellar structure (during pre-neurodegeneration, when no motor defects were detected), morphological alterations in the main dendrite of PCs and social and environmental exploratory behavioral disabilities occur simultaneously (both at P15 and P17). In psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders, such as autism or schizophrenia that can often exhibit severe symptoms of social disability, have been related to cerebellar alterations8,9,10,11,12,15. Furthermore, PC loss is the most common anatomopathological change found in autistic patients47, and even in those cases where no loss of PCs was observed, morphological defects were still found48. Also, in accordance with our results, environmental exploratory defects have been previously associated with cerebellar alterations50,51,52. A possible explanation for these impairments is that alterations in dendritic morphology influence the electrophysiological activity of neurons, including the PCs53,54. More precisely, defects in the PCs axial resistance, which directly depends on dendritic length and width, disrupt the synaptic integration in PCs53, as previously hypothesized55,56. Hence, it is plausible to relate the progressive extinction of Purkinje cells in PCD mice to the progressive alteration of the behaviors analyzed (see below).

During the cerebellar degeneration that occurs in PCD mice, neuronal death takes place throughout the three cerebellar layers. Previous studies assumed that in the PCD animal, the loss of neuronal populations of the granular and molecular layers is residual and delayed in time, in comparison to the death of PCs25; however, the progression of their death has never been characterized. Our results demonstrate that this neuronal loss progresses at the same time as the loss of PCs, indicating a simultaneous decline in all three layers in the cerebellum of the PCD mouse.

During neurodegeneration, the morphological alterations of the PCs worsen, with the most evident changes linked to the length and width of the main dendrite, the soma area and the dendritic arbor length. In parallel, the defects observed in social preference and environmental exploratory behavior also persist during this stage in the PCD mouse. Alterations in these behaviors have been described in both animal and human studies with cerebellar impairments8,9,10,57,58,59. It was observed that these alterations may be due both to neuronal loss and to PC malfunction. Interestingly, we saw that ataxia did not bias the results of the cognitive/social tasks performed by the PCD mice, and during neurodegeneration grooming was found to be reduced. In contrast, this behavior has been previously shown to increase in animal models of specific PC loss15, being more so in models presenting a general alteration of the cerebellum58. Accordingly, during grooming behavior there is an increase in the activity of both granule cells60 and PCs59. Consequently, grooming may be affected differently depending on whether the cerebellar alterations affect the PCs, granule cells or both. Thus, the severity of cerebellar defects helps to understand the relationship between the cerebellum and grooming. Although grooming is a very complex motor task, we cannot discard the possibility that the motor defects caused by cerebellar alterations have influenced the observed behavioral changes.

Interestingly, general changes in cerebellar structure in the PCD mouse, which disrupt recognition memory, only occurred at P30 when most of the PCs are lost. The cerebellum has been associated with procedural learning and memory61,62, and current neuroimaging studies have also described activation of the cerebellum during non-procedural memory tasks63,64,65. These works, however, did not study the impact of the progression of the breakdown of cerebellar structure on recognition memory. According to our results, cerebellar alterations lead to the impairment of recognition memory in PCD mice, and during neurodegeneration both the cerebellar and behavioral defects become worse. Therefore, these results suggest a correlation between the progression of the breakdown of the cerebellar structure and the severity of the defects related to performing cognitive and affective tasks. Then, a healthy, typical cerebellar structure is necessary to execute specific cognitive and affective tasks.

In conclusion, polyglutamylation affects microtubules dynamics and structure, and its modulation is essential throughout normal cerebellar development. During the first two postnatal weeks, when the PCs are still migrating and growing, microtubules instability is crucial. However, a pathologically extended instability of microtubules (triggered by the pcd mutation) affects the morphology and survival of PCs, which are vital for the correct functioning of the cerebellum. In this sense, cerebellum plays an important role in cognitive and affective functions, since the progressive breakdown of the cerebellar cortex in PCD mice is directly related to the increasing severity of cognitive and affective impairments.

Material and Methods

Animals

Both WT and PCD mice from the C57/DBA strain were used and purchased from The Jackson Laboratory, Maine, USA. For in vitro studies, MEFs were prepared from both WT and CCP1-KO embryos (see below). The embryos were separated into groups depending on their genotype (WT or PCD; n = 3 embryos per group). For in vivo studies, animals were separated into groups depending on their genotype and age at the time of analysis: P7, P15, P17, P22 and P30 (n = 4 per age and genotype) for the immunohistochemical analyses; and P15, P17, P22 and P30 (n = 8–9 per genotype) for the behavioral analyses.

Since PCD and CCP1-KO animals are not fertile, heterozygous mice were mated, and the embryos or newborn mice were genotyped (see below and28).

Animals were housed, handled and sacrificed per the guidelines established by European (2010/63/UE) and national legislations (Spanish RD53/2013 and Law 32/2007; French permit n°38 07 11). Every effort was made to minimize the number of animals used and their suffering. This study was approved by the Bioethics Committee of the University of Salamanca.

Generation of CCP1 KO mice

The conditional mutant mouse line for CCP1 (on exons 20 and 21) was established and generated at the Mouse Clinical Institute (MCI, Illkirch, France) to study microtubules dynamics and structure. The targeting vector was constructed as follows. The 5′ (4.5 kb), 3′ (1.4 kb) and inter-loxP (1.9) fragments were polymerase chain reaction (PCR) amplified and sequentially subcloned into an MCI proprietary vector containing the LoxP sites and a Neo cassette flanked by Flippase Recognition Target (FRT) sites (Fig. 7). The linearized construct was electroporated in 129S2/SvPas mouse embryonic stem (ES) cells. After selection, targeted clones were identified by PCR using external primers and further confirmed by Southern blot with 5′ and 3′ external probes. Two positive ES clones were injected into blastocysts, and derived male chimaeras gave germline transmission. The excision of the neomycin-resistance cassette was performed in vivo by breeding the chimeras with a Flp deleter line (C57BL/6 N genetic background FLP under ACTB promoter). The Flp transgene was segregated by breeding the first germ line mice with a wild type C57BL/6 N animal. For generation ccp1 KO, ccp1 floxed mice were crossed with transgenic mice expressing Cre recombinase under the control of a CMV promoter.

Figure 7
figure 7

Schematic representation in scale of the targeting vector used and all the possible alleles for CCP1 gene. Orange bar: genomic DNA. Black boxes: exons with their corresponding number. Green and purple arrowheads: LoxP and Flp sequences, respectively. White bar: neo cassette, with the neomycin resistance gene (white box). Blue lines: zone of sequence homology for homologous recombination, with the corresponding size in kbp. Black arrowheads: primers used for the PCR genotyping.

Genomic DNA isolated from mouse tail snip was analyzed by PCR. Mice were genotyped by PCR according to MCI protocols using GoTag polymerase (Promega) and 33 amplification cycles. The three primer pairs listed below were used to define the genotypes.

  1. (1)

    Ef: GTACACAGACTTTTAGGCTAATCTC

    Er: ACTTCCACATCTGCACTGTGGACAG

  2. (2)

    Lf: CATAAGAACCAGGTTCATTCTGTCC

    Lr: TAGCATCATGTTAAAACTCCCTCCT

  3. (3)

    Lf: CATAAGAACCAGGTTCATTCTGTCC

Er: ACTTCCACATCTGCACTGTGGACAG

Cell culture and transfection

For in vitro analysis of the microtubules, MEFs from the CCP1 KO line were prepared as previously described66. To visualize the microtubules, MEFs were transfected using the NucleofectorTM Kits for MEFs (Amaxa Biosystems) with GFP-EB3 (to visualize microtubules (+)-ends; provided by N. Galjart, Erasmus Medical Center, Rotterdam, The Netherlands) and m-cherry α-tubulin (to visualize the entire microtubules; provided by F. Saudou, Curie Institute, Paris, France) plasmids.

Video microscopy

Video time-lapses of transfected MEFs were captured with an inverted microscope (Axiovert 200 M; Carl Zeiss, Oberkochen, Germany) and a 100× NA 1.3 Plan-Neofluar oil objective controlled by MetaMorph software (MDS Analytical Technologies, Sunnyvale, CA, USA). Images were taken every three seconds for 5 minutes. All images were captured with a charge-coupled device camera (CoolSNAP HQ; Roper Scientific, Sarasota, Florida, USA).

Microtubule dynamics and structure analysis

For the analysis of the microtubule dynamics the plusTipTracker software67 and the GFP-EB3 video time-lapse images were employed. We first adjusted the accuracy of the GFP-EB3 detection, and then analyzed the following parameters: growing and shrinking speed; growth and shrinking mean length; percentage of time in pause, growing and shrinking; distribution of microtubules based on their speed and length; catastrophe frequency and rescue frequency. These parameters were chosen based on previous studies68,69. To ensure the accuracy of the analysis, we compared the growing speed data obtained both automatically and manually.

The microtubule curvature analysis was performed using the first image of each video time-lapse with m-cherry α-tubulin. Additionally, with the GFP-EB3 labeling experiment we obtained the maximum projection of the entire video time-lapse, allowing us to reconstruct the trajectories of the microtubules, whose curvature was also analyzed. Thus, the curvature of microtubules was analyzed both in a static way and within their trajectory of movement.

Curvature (k) was determined using the Mathematica software (Wolfram Research Europe, UK). After obtaining the equation of each microtubule with a spline cubic interpolation, the general formula of curvature70 was employed to analyze each microtubule (equation 1):

$$k=\frac{x^{\prime} y^{\prime\prime} -y^{\prime} x^{\prime\prime} }{{({x^{\prime} }^{2}+{y^{\prime} }^{2})}^{3/2}}$$
(1)

where (equations 2, 3, 4 and 5)

$$x^{\prime} =\frac{dx}{dt},$$
(2)
$$x^{\prime\prime} =\frac{{d}^{2}x}{d{t}^{2}},$$
(3)
$$y^{\prime} =\frac{dy}{dt},$$
(4)
$$y^{\prime\prime} =\frac{{d}^{2}y}{d{t}^{2}},$$
(5)

i.e., the first (\(x\text{'},\,y\text{'}\)) and second (\(x\text{'}\text{'},y\text{'}\)) derivatives at each point.

Tissue extraction and preparation for in vivo studies

To analyze the effect of the pcd mutation on the PCs morphology and the general cerebellar structure in vivo, animals were anesthetized and intracardially perfused with Somogyi’s fixative without glutaraldehyde (5 ml/g body weight), as previously described 26Cerebella were sectioned sagittally at 30 µm thick using a freezing microtome (Leica Jung SM 2000, Nussloch, Germany), as previously described71,72,73. The histological analyses were focused on the vermis using three sections per animal (separated by 180 µm each), with a total of five animals per genotype and age. All values represent the mean count of the three sections analyzed.

Immunofluorescence

Sections were incubated with the primary antisera at 4 °C during 72 h, as previously described74. The antibodies employed were mouse anti-calbindin (Cb-28k 1:1,000; Swant, Switzerland), mouse anti-parvalbumin (PV 1:1,000; Swant, Switzerland) and mouse anti-NeuN (1:8,000; Merck Millipore, Darmstadt, Germany). Appropriate secondary antibodies conjugated with Cy2 or Cy3 (1:500; Jackson Laboratories, West Grove, PA, USA) were used. Sections were counter-stained with DAPI (1:30,000; Sigma Aldrich) to identify the cell nuclei. Antibodies were selected based on their selectivity to identify different neuronal populations. Calbindin is a well-known calcium-binding protein that label the whole PCs46,75,76, allowing us to analyze the PCs morphology. In addition, parvalbumin stains both PCs and interneurons in the molecular layer46. Finally, NeuN was used to counterstain whole cerebellar nuclei during cell counting, ensuring no biases were made due to the lack of immunolabeling during degeneration.

Terminal Deoxynucleotidyl Transferase-Mediated Fluorescein dUTP Nick-End Labeling (TUNEL) detection

To study apoptosis throughout cerebellar postnatal development, the TUNEL technique was used as previously described29,74. To determine the neuronal nature of apoptotic cells, the TUNEL was combined with immunohistochemistry against NeuN or PV. Finally, both TUNEL and immunohistochemistry were revealed using Cy2-streptavidin and Cy3-secondary antibodies, respectively.

Morphological analysis of PCs

Morphological analysis of the PCs was made using sections immunostained for Cb-28k from P7 to P30. To avoid possible biases, only those Purkinje cells containing the soma and a clear dendritic arbor were analyzed. Also, all Purkinje cells fitting this criterion were analyzed in three cerebellar sections per animal (separated by 180 µm each). The final value for each animal used to compare was the mean of all Purkinje cells analyzed in these three sections. The morphological parameters studied were selected in basis to its meaning for PCs function77,78: (1) the length and width of the main dendrite; (2) the length of the dendritic arbor of the PCs (measured indirectly through the molecular layer thickness); and (3) the soma size. Morphological analyses were carried out with the Neurolucida (MBF Bioscience, Williston, Vermont, USA) and ImageJ (NIH, USA) software, as previously described77.

Behavioral analyses

We characterized the effects of progressive cerebellar defects on motor, cognitive, and social processes. Behavioral analyses were performed at P15, P17, P22 and P30, encompassing both the pre-neurodegenerative (P15–P18) and neurodegenerative (P18–P30) stages. All devices employed were cleaned with 96% (v/v) ethanol before and after each session.

The rota-rod test was used to characterize motor coordination. It was performed as previously described79, with an acceleration of 0.6 rpm/s, from 4 to 40 rpm, in 10 min.

The home-cage behavior analysis was used to characterize general behavior. Animals were placed each day in the home-cage for 10 min and allowed to freely explore. Afterwards, the animals’ behavior was observed for 10 min, during which time the following parameters were recorded manually: (1) grooming time (stereotyped behavior); (2) number of rearings (environmental exploratory behavior) and (3) time moving (general movement). Each animal was assigned to an individual home-cage to avoid the influence of odor from the other animals.

The novel object recognition test was used to analyze recognition memory. The test was performed in the same home-cage used described above to help the animals to become habituated. On the first day, at P15, animals were placed in the home-cage with two identical objects (A and A) for 10 min. Then, for each of the following sessions (at P17, P22 and P30) the animals were placed in the home-cage with: (1) the familiar object (A) and (2) a new object (B, C, and D, respectively for each of the different ages; see Fig. 5). The objects were placed in opposite corners of the home-cage, the time interacting with each object was measured and their ratio was calculated.

The social preference test was performed in a white Plexiglas® box (50 × 29 cm) divided into three chambers. The social preference test was performed as previously described80,81,82. On each test day, a mouse could explore the box for 10 min. Then, the animal was removed from the box and a different mouse, of the same age and sex, was placed in one of the lateral chambers under a drilled pencil cup. On the other side of the box, an object was placed in the other lateral chamber, also under a drilled pencil cup. The original mouse was then returned to the box and the percentage of time spent exploring each lateral chamber and each pencil cup was analyzed.

Statistical analysis

For data analysis, the Student’s t-Test was performed. For histological analysis, both genotypes were compared for each age. For behavioral analysis, the parameters analyzed and the different confrontation variables are described in each behavioral procedure. All analyses were performed with the SPSS statistical package software (IBM, NY, USA).