Main

Mucosal surfaces are the main ports of entry for most environmental antigens. The primary humoral mediators of mucosal immunity are secretory IgA (SIgA) and secretory IgM (SIgM), preventing adherence and penetration of antigens through the mucosal epithelium (1). As their polymeric form with an attached secretory component (SC) makes them resistant to cleavage by proteolytic enzymes, they are well suited for surface protection (1). Mucosal SIgA antibody responses are noninflammatory and are induced by immunoregulatory and IgE-inhibitory cytokines, such as TGF-β and IL-10 (1). An efficient mucosal immune response is a prerequisite for health, because the various mucosal sites are favored as portals of entry by potentially pathogenic antigens and allergens. At birth, only traces of SIgA are found in human exocrine glands, and adult levels are not reached until several years later (2).

Atopic diseases are characterized by high and prolonged circulating IgE antibody responses to allergens (3). A still unsolved conundrum, however, is why some sensitized children do not develop clinical allergy symptoms. We have previously reported that skin prick test (SPT)-positive infants without any clinical symptoms of allergy have higher levels of salivary SIgA than those who manifest symptoms (4). Although there is no clear association between mucosal IgA production and development of allergic disease, it is conceivable that high levels of SIgA in the mucosa may prevent allergen absorption and also possibly interfere with the interaction between allergen and IgE antibodies in sensitized individuals, thereby preventing the development of allergic symptoms. High salivary SIgA levels may also reflect a more mature mucosal immune system (2). Recently, high fecal IgA levels at 6 mo were reported to be associated with a reduced risk to develop IgE-associated disease (5).

The prevalence of allergic diseases has increased considerably in industrialized countries during the last decades (6). Results derived from studies comparing the incidence of atopic diseases in countries of Western and Eastern Europe (7,8) indicate that the increase is due to environmental factors associated with an affluent lifestyle. Decreased and/or altered microbial exposure because of e.g. improved hygiene, less infections, antibiotic treatment, and altered gut microbiota has been suggested to account for the increase of atopic diseases (9). The latter suggestion is supported by studies showing differences in the composition and diversity of the gut microbiota between allergic and nonallergic infants (1013) and between infants living in countries with a low (Estonia) and high (Sweden) allergy prevalence (14). Moreover, growing up in an environment with heavy continuous external microbial exposure, e.g. farms, reduces the risk to develop allergic diseases (15). Higher levels of lipopolysaccharide (LPS) from the cell wall of Gram-negative bacteria were found in house dust collected from farming than from nonfarming households (16). Furthermore, we have previously shown higher LPS levels in house dust from Estonian than from Swedish homes and that high LPS levels were associated with less atopy in Swedish infants (17). It is not known whether early microbial exposure affects the maturation of mucosal IgA responses, however, or if the postnatal maturation process differs among infants living in different environments.

The aim of this study was to investigate the development of mucosal immunity in Estonian and Swedish children followed up from birth to 5 y of age, relating the IgA levels in saliva to development of sensitization, allergic disease, infections, and environmental endotoxin exposure.

METHODS

Subjects.

This study comprised 110 Estonian and 91 Swedish children participating in a prospective study regarding development of allergic disease in relation to environmental factors. The study was approved by the Ethics Review Committee on Human Research of the University of Tartu and the Human Research Ethics Committee at the Faculty of Health Sciences, Linköping University. The parents of all children gave their written informed consent. The Estonian children were born during the period February 1997 to June 1998 and the Swedish children were born between March 1996 and March 2000. The children were followed up from birth to 5 y of age as described in detail elsewhere (18). Briefly, at 3, 6, and 12 mo and at 2 and 5 years of age, the parents answered questionnaires regarding e.g. allergic symptoms, diarrhea, upper and lower respiratory infections, antibiotic treatment, and breastfeeding (Table 1). Furthermore, the children were clinically examined either by a pediatrician or an experienced allergy research nurse; SPTs were performed and saliva and blood samples were obtained. Atopic dermatitis was defined as pruritic, chronic, or chronically relapsing noninfectious dermatitis with typical features and distribution. Asthma was defined as three or more episodes of bronchial obstruction during the last 12-month period, at least once verified by a physician. Allergic rhinitis/conjunctivitis was diagnosed after appearing at least twice within 1 h after exposure to a particular allergen and not related to infections. Urticaria was defined as allergic if it appeared at least twice within 1 h after exposure to a particular allergen. The incidence of allergic symptoms is presented in Table 2.

Table 1 The incidence of diarrhea, respiratory infections, otitis media, and antibiotic treatment during the first 2 y of life, and the length of exclusive and total breastfeeding in Estonian and Swedish children
Table 2 The incidence of allergic symptoms and SPT positivity in Estonian and Swedish children followed up from birth to 5 y of age

SPTs were done at all follow-ups in Estonia, whereas in the Swedish children they were randomly selected to be done either at 3 mo in half of the cohort or 6 mo in the remaining half of the cohort, and then at 12 and 24 mo and 5 y of age. SPTs were performed in duplicate at the volar aspects of the forearms with fresh skimmed cow's milk and thawed egg white at all follow-ups. At 12 mo, the children were also tested with cat allergen extract and in Estonia also with mite allergen extract (Dermatophagoides pteronyssinus). At 2 and 5 y, birch and timothy were added to the panel and in Estonia also cockroach (Blatella germanica) allergen. All extracts were standardized allergen extracts from ALK (Soluprick, ALK, Hørsholm, Denmark), except the cockroach extract, which was from Bayer (Spokane, WA). Histamine hydrochloride, 10 mg/mL, was used as positive control and glycerol as negative. The test was regarded as positive if the mean diameter of one of the wheals was at least 3 mm. The incidence of positive SPT is presented in Table 2.

The Swedish children were further categorized as sensitized based on the presence of a positive SPT and/or detectable circulating allergen-specific IgE antibodies. This definition is not applicable in the Estonian children, because low levels of circulating IgE antibodies are commonly detected and are poorly related to allergy and SPT positivity (18). The presence of IgE antibodies in egg white and β-lactoglobulin were determined in serum samples collected at all follow-ups up to 24 mo, and IgE antibodies to cat and birch allergens were analyzed at 12 and 24 mo by a commercial chemiluminescence method, according to the recommendations of the manufacturer (Magic Lite, ALK). Blood samples were drawn from the Swedish children either at 3 mo in half of the cohort or 6 mo in the remaining half of the cohort, and then at 12 and 24 mo and 5 y of age. Nine Swedish infants were sensitized at 3 mo, 14 at 6 mo, 21 at 12 mo, and 18 at 24 mo. The cumulative number up to that age was 30.

Saliva samples.

Nonstimulated saliva samples were collected at all follow-ups from the buccal cavity, using a hand pump connected to a thin plastic tube and immediately frozen and kept at −20°C. Before analysis, the samples were heated at 56°C for 30 min and then centrifuged at 5000 g for 15 min. Reference levels of salivary SIgA were obtained by analyzing samples from 20 healthy adults.

For analysis of total IgA by ELISA, an anti-human IgA antibody directed against the α-chain (Dakopatts AB, Täby, Sweden) was used as coating antibody, enabling detection of all IgA, including monomeric, polymeric, and SIgA. For detection of SIgA by ELISA, an anti-human SC antibody (Dakopatts AB) was used, enabling detection of only SIgA. Alkaline phosphatase-conjugated goat-anti-human-IgA antibodies (Sigma Chemical Co. Immunochemicals, Stockholm, Sweden) were used for detection. Detailed description of the methods is published elsewhere (4). Human IgA (Sigma Chemical Co. Immunochemicals) was diluted to a seven-step standard curve, ranging 40 to 2500 μg/L for total IgA and 16 to 1000 μg/L for SIgA. The limits of detection were 40 μg/L for total IgA and 32 μg/L for SIgA. The interassay coefficients of variations (CVs) were 10% for total IgA and 14% for SIgA. Total IgA and SIgA were detectable in all samples.

For detection of IgG antibodies in saliva, a human IgG ELISA quantitation kit was used (Bethyl Laboratories, Inc. Montgomery), according to the manufacturer. Human reference serum, containing 4 mg/mL of IgG antibodies, was included in the kit and used as a seven-step standard curve, ranging from 7.8 to 500 ng/mL. All saliva samples were diluted 1:10 to 1:1000.

House dust samples.

Two dust samples were collected from each household, one from the carpet and one from the child's mattress when the infants were between 6 and 12 mo of age. A dust collector device, containing a 6 μm pore size filter (ALK), was connected to a vacuum cleaner. The mattress, without sheets, and 2 m2 of the carpet were vacuum cleaned for 4 min. The filters were stored in sterile plastic bags and kept at −20°C until analysis.

The extractions and endotoxin analysis were performed as described in detail elsewhere with a chromogenic Limulus Amebocyte Lysate (LAL) assay (QCL-1000, Bio Whittaker, Walkersville, ND) (17). The lowest limit for quantitative determinations, for samples diluted 1:1000, was 50 endotoxin units (EU)/mL, corresponding to 0.50 EU/mg dust. Endotoxin analyses were performed on 98 Estonian and 68 Swedish dust samples. The median levels and the range of endotoxin in carpets were 26 (0.5–376) EU/mg in Estonia and 13 (0.5–1393) EU/mg in Sweden (p < 0.0001). The corresponding figures for mattresses were 28 (0.5–275) EU/mg in Estonia and 18 (0.5–99) EU/mg in Sweden (p = 0.004).

Statistics.

As the concentrations of antibodies were not normally distributed, comparisons between unpaired groups were performed with the Mann-Whitney U test and paired groups were analyzed using Wilcoxon-signed rank test. Correlations were calculated with Spearman correlation test. The χ2 test was used for comparisons of categorical variables. A probability level of <0.05 was considered to be statistically significant.

RESULTS

The levels of total IgA in saliva increased with age, both in Estonian and Swedish infants (Fig. 1A). The total IgA levels were higher in the Estonian infants at 3 mo, whereas at 2 y the levels were higher in saliva from Swedish children (Fig. 1A). At 5 y, the levels were similar in the two populations (Fig. 1A).

Figure 1
figure 1

Salivary IgA levels (mg/L) in children at 3, 6, and 12 mo and at 2 and 5 y of age. (A) Total IgA levels in Estonian (, n = 100, 106, 91, 78, 99) and Swedish (□, n = 39, 37, 77, 65, 60) children. (B) Levels of secretory (S) IgA in Estonian (, n = 101, 106, 91, 78, 99) and Swedish (□, n = 41, 39, 80, 67, 60) children and from 20 Swedish adults (▪). Salivary samples were collected from the Swedish children either at 3 or 6 mo, and then at 12 and 24 mo and 5 y of age. The 10th, 25th, 50th, 75th, and 90th percentiles and outliers (circles) are indicated. **p < 0.01, §p < 0.001.

The levels of SIgA also increased with age in both populations, reaching adult levels at 5 y (Fig. 1B). The levels of SIgA were much higher in saliva from Estonian than Swedish children through the first 2 y of life, however. Similar SIgA levels were observed in Estonian and Swedish children only at 5 y of age (Fig. 1B). The similar levels of total IgA and higher levels of SIgA are explained by the fact that most of the salivary IgA in the Estonian infants was SIgA, whereas in the Swedish infants the SIgA levels were markedly lower than the total IgA levels (Fig. 2). Thus, the SIgA/total IgA ratio was significantly lower in Swedish than Estonian children at 3, 6, 12, and 24 mo (Mann-Whitney U test, p < 0.0001 for all comparisons) but not at 5 y of age.

Figure 2
figure 2

The total IgA levels in Estonian (, Est) and in Swedish (□, Sw) children compared with the SIgA levels in saliva (in Estonian () and in Swedish () children) at 3 and 12 mo of age, n according to Fig. 1. The 10th, 25th, 50th, 75th, and 90th percentiles are indicated as well as outliers (circles). §p < 0.001.

Total IgG levels were analyzed in saliva collected at 12 mo and at 5 y as a marker of passive transport over the epithelium. At 12 mo, the levels of total IgG correlated with the non-SIgA levels in the Swedish infants (rho = 0.51, p < 0.0001) but not in the Estonian infants.

House dust endotoxin levels did not correlate with salivary IgA production, except for inverse correlations between the levels in dust from mattresses and carpets and non-SIgA at 6 and 12 mo of age in the Swedish infants (rho = −0.24 to 0.48, p = 0.01–0.048).

Respiratory infections, i.e. common cold, cough, and wheeze, during infancy were more common in Estonian than in Swedish children, except during the first 3 mo of life (Table 1). Also diarrhea tended to be more common in Estonia during infancy, whereas the Swedish children tended to have more otitis. The Estonian children were more often treated with antibiotics during their first 12 mo. Respiratory infections between 3 and 6 mo of age were associated with higher SIgA levels at 6 mo in both Estonian and Swedish children. Similar results were observed for respiratory infections between 6 and 12 mo and SIgA levels at 12 mo in the Estonian children (data not shown). Diarrhea, otitis, and antibiotic treatment were not associated with salivary IgA levels.

A lower proportion of Estonian than Swedish children attended day care before 2 y of age (20 versus 79%, p < 0.001, χ2 test). In Estonia, 4.5% of the children (5/110) lived at 3 mo of age in households where both parents smoked compared with 4.3% in Sweden (4/91, NS). No Estonian and two Swedish children lived in a household where the mother alone smoked (NS). The proportion of children with a smoking father and nonsmoking mother was higher in Estonia (35%, 38/110) than Sweden (4.3%, 4/91), p < 0.001, χ2 test. Day-care attendance and smoking were not related to the IgA and SIgA levels at any age neither in the Estonian nor in the Swedish children, however.

Among the Swedish sensitized infants, those with allergic symptoms during the first 2 y of life had lower levels of SIgA at 3, 6, and 12 mo than similarly sensitized infants without any clinical symptoms, with a similar trend at 24 mo of age (Fig. 3) but not at 5 y of age [median and interquartile ranges in sensitized children with and without symptoms, 141 (90–187) and 176 (134–207) mg/L, respectively]. No such association was observed for allergic symptoms at 5 y in the Swedish children, nor at any age in the Estonian children, possibly because of the low number of children with positive SPT and allergic disease among the latter (Table 2). Neither total IgA nor SIgA levels were associated with development of allergic disease or sensitization as such, however (data not shown), although non-SIgA levels, defined by subtracting the levels of SIgA from the levels of total IgA, were higher in Swedish SPT-positive than -negative children at 6 and 12 mo of age (Fig. 4). The non-SIgA levels at 5 y of age were similar in SPT-positive and -negative children [median and interquartile ranges 53 (20–100) and 53 (25–88) mg/L, respectively].

Figure 3
figure 3

Secretory (S) IgA (mg/L) in saliva collected at 3, 6, 12, and 24 mo of age from Swedish sensitized infants with (, n = 9, 5, 13, and 10) and without (□, n = 4, 8, 10, and 8) allergic symptoms during the first 2 y of life. Sensitization was defined as at least one positive SPT and/or detection of circulating allergen-specific IgE antibodies. The 10th, 25th, 50th, 75th, and 90th percentiles and outliers (circles) are indicated. *p < 0.05, and ¶p = 0.11.

Figure 4
figure 4

The levels of nonsecretory (S) IgA (the part of total IgA lacking the SC), in saliva collected at 3, 6, 12, and 24 mo of age from Swedish infants with positive (n = 11, 6, 13, 10) or negative (n = 24, 26, 54, 44) SPTs during the first 2 y of life. The 10th, 25th, 50th, 75th, and 90th percentiles and outliers (circles) are indicated. *p < 0.05.

DISCUSSION

Mucosal IgA production increased with age, reaching adult levels at 5 y. In the Estonian children, most of the salivary IgA was in the secretory form at all ages, with an SC attached to the IgA dimer. In contrast, SIgA comprised a much lower proportion of total salivary IgA in the Swedish infants during the first 2 y of life, indicating that a large proportion of their salivary IgA lacked an SC. At 5 y of age, however, the levels of SIgA and total IgA were comparable also among the Swedish children.

At mucosal sites, such as the salivary glands, dimeric IgA antibodies synthesized in plasma cells beneath the epithelial basement membrane bind to the polymeric immunoglobulin receptor (pIgR) on the basolateral surface of the epithelial cells (1). The complex is transported to the apical surface, where the pIgR is cleaved to leave the extracellular IgA-binding component bound to the IgA molecule as the so-called SC (1). The pIgR-mediated transocytosis of IgA across the epithelium suggests that all IgA in the lumen of mucosal organs should be in the secretory form. The synthesis of the pIgR may for some reason be delayed in Swedish infants. A reduced pIgR-mediated IgA transport may allow a larger proportion of dimeric IgA to passively diffuse over the epithelium, resulting in the presence of non-SIgA in the salivary gland. Also, a reduced epithelial barrier function may cause increased mucosal leakiness (19). A certain microbial pressure may be mandatory for preservation of an intact mucosal barrier function (20). In support of our findings of non-SIgA in saliva of Swedish infants, both monomeric and SIgA were present in saliva of Australian children at least up to 12 mo of age (21). After 12 mo, a switch to only SIgA production was proposed (21).

Total IgG levels in saliva were analyzed at 1 and 5 y of age as a marker of nonreceptor-mediated transocytosis of immunoglobulins over the epithelium. The correlation between total IgG and non-SIgA levels in the Swedish infants suggests that a large proportion of the IgA in their saliva was secreted without pIgR involvement. This was not the case in the Estonian children.

The expression of pIgR is modulated by microbes and microbial-derived products (22) and bacterial LPS and double-stranded RNA both upregulate the expression (23). The concentrations of bacterial endotoxin (i.e. LPS) in house dust were considerably higher in Estonian than Swedish homes (17). This may possibly explain the higher SIgA levels in the Estonian infants. This suggestion is supported by the inverse relation between house dust endotoxin levels and the proportion of non-SIgA in the Swedish infants. Furthermore, commensal intestinal bacteria, e.g. Bacteroides thetaiotaomicron, have been shown to upregulate the expression of the pIgR (24). Pronounced differences in the composition of the gut flora between Estonian and Swedish infants have previously been reported, with a more diverse flora in the former population (14). Moreover, an increased bifidobacterial diversity during the first 2 mo of life was associated with enhanced salivary SIgA levels in Swedish infants (25).

As one of the main functions of SIgA is to protect against mucosal infections by preventing adherence and penetration of microbes (1), low levels of SIgA could theoretically impose an increased susceptibility to infections. In this study, however, a history of infections early in life was associated with high SIgA levels, probably reflecting the stimulatory effect of the microbial pressure on mucosal immunity early in life.

High levels of salivary SIgA early in life seemed to protect sensitized children from developing clinical signs of allergy. This is in line with a previous report (4). Furthermore, we recently confirmed this observation in a cohort of 200 children followed from birth to 4 y of age (26). The presence of SC enhances both the stability and effector functions of IgA (22). High mucosal SIgA levels could possibly interfere with the interaction between allergen and IgE antibodies in sensitized individuals, thereby preventing triggering of allergic inflammation and clinical symptoms. This notion is supported by animal models showing that mucosal IgA responses inhibit airway reactivity in sensitized mice (2729). Also, induction of IgA to allergens during immunotherapy has been suggested to relate to the development of tolerance (30). High levels of non-SIgA may further reflect an immature and leaky mucosal membrane (19), allowing an uptake of allergens through the mucosa. The slow maturation of the salivary SIgA system in Swedish infants was associated with low house dust endotoxin levels and allergy development. This is in line with recent findings that high salivary IgA levels were related to early day-care attendance and less atopy in Australian children (31) and with the hypothesis that the allergy epidemic is caused by a decreased microbial exposure (9).

In conclusion, Swedish children show a markedly slower maturation of the salivary SIgA system than Estonian children, possibly as a consequence of the large differences in microbial pressure between the two countries. SIgA may limit allergy-mediated tissue damage at mucosal surfaces in sensitized individuals.