INTRODUCTION

Opioid addiction, whether to opiates such as heroin and morphine or to non-medical use of opioids, is a major problem worldwide. Opioid abuse may be due to the illegal use of opiates or of illicit prescription drugs for the long-term treatment of chronic pain (Manchikanti et al, 2010). Chronic use of morphine can lead to brain structure and behavioral changes in individuals. Not only does long-term use of opioids have a negative effect on the individual but the parental addiction can also severely impact their offspring. Prenatal opioid exposure renders behavioral changes in the offspring, such as addiction vulnerability and depression-like behavior (Klausz et al, 2011). In addition, prenatal opiate exposure also results in brain neurochemical changes, including dendritic retraction and the downregulation of dopamine receptor gene in adult offspring (DiNieri et al, 2011; Lu et al, 2012; Vathy, 2002).

Mounting evidence has shown that transgenerational effects of opioid exposure occur even in the absence of continuous use (for a review seeVassoler et al, 2014). Adolescent maternal morphine exposure, followed by a drug-free period of several weeks, still induced anxiety-like behavior and morphine sensitization in the offspring (Byrnes, 2005). Along with behavioral changes, adolescent maternal morphine exposure also altered the response of dopamine agonists and upregulated the dopamine- and opioid-related gene in the first and second generation (Byrnes et al, 2013). However, studies of transgenerational effects of adolescent morphine exposure have primarily involved adolescent female animals (Byrnes, 2005; Byrnes et al, 2011, 2013;Mei et al, 2009). The effects of adult opioid exposure on their offspring are unknown. Noticeably, the effects of morphine are quite distinct between adults and adolescents (Koek et al, 2012; Schramm-Sapyta et al, 2009; White et al, 2008). For example, adolescents are more sensitive to the rewarding effects of drugs and less sensitive to withdrawal effects (Schramm-Sapyta et al, 2009). Given that chronic use of opiates in the adult population has increased greatly in recent years, it is important to understand the effects of adult morphine exposure on their offspring (Compton and Volkow, 2006; Manchikanti et al, 2010)

It is well known that neurogenesis and dendritic plasticity are closely associated with the pathogenesis of anxiety and depression (Eisch and Petrik, 2012; Kheirbek and Hen, 2011; Mateus-Pinheiro et al, 2013). A few molecular signals linking dendritic plasticity and psychiatric disorders have been identified (Lanoue et al, 2013). Insulin-like growth factor (IGF-2) has been reported to regulate synapse formation, dendritic spine maturation, and memory consolidation (Agis-Balboa et al, 2011; Chen et al, 2011; Schmeisser et al, 2012). However, the molecular signals linking the effect of morphine exposure in parents to neuroplasticity and mood disorders in their offspring are still largely unknown.

Here, we showed that exposure to morphine in adult males and females led to two important effects in their adult male and female offspring: anxiety-like behaviors and reduced length and branch number of dendrites in the dentate gyrus (DG) of the hippocampus. In addition, IGF-2 in the hippocampus was downregulated in the offspring of parents exposed to morphine. Overexpression of IGF-2 by intra-DG injection of lentivirus encoding the human IGF-2 gene (LV-IGF2-EGFP) reversed the anxiety-like behavior. The anxiety-like behavior and the shortened dendrites in the hippocampus of adult offspring could be reversed by enriched environment (EE) exposure in the adolescent offspring. Finally, adolescent EE experience also prevented the downregulation of IGF-2 in the hippocampus. Thus, hippocampal IGF-2 has a critical role to link the neuropsychiatric disorders and retracted dendrites in the offspring of parents’ morphine exposure, which can be modified by EE.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects

Sprague–Dawley rats of each sex (8 weeks of age) were purchased from Central South University Animal Services (Changsha, China). All animals in the study were group housed in light-controlled (1200–1200 h) and temperature-controlled (21–24 °C) rooms and were provided with food and water ad libitum. The experimental protocol was approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of Central South University and conformed to the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

Adult Morphine Exposure

Beginning at 8 weeks of age, female (defined as maternal), male (defined as paternal), or both (defined as biparental) were treated with morphine (morphine sulfate; Shenyang First Pharmaceutical, Shenyang, China) for a total of 10 days, using an increasing dose regimen, as described previously (Byrnes et al, 2011). On day 1 of exposure, animals received 2.5 mg/kg of morphine sulfate (subcutaneously) two times daily (0800 hours and 1600 hours). On each of the following 9 days, the dose of morphine was increased by 2.5 mg/kg each day, such that by the final day of treatment, rats received two times daily 25 mg/kg injections. Age-matched control animals received the saline vehicle (0.9% NaCl, subcutaneously) two times daily with volumes adjusted to match those of drug-treated animals. Following this regimen, animals remained undisturbed in the colony until mating.

Mating and Postpartum Care

Four groups were arranged for mating and further study: male morphine exposure, female morphine exposure, both morphine exposure, and control (no morphine exposure). Twenty-one days after the final drug or vehicle administration (87 days of age), the morphine-exposed females or males were mated with the colony normal males or females, respectively, or were mated with each other. The saline-injected females and males were mated and served as controls. After mating, the females were monitored for pregnancy. The birth of each litter was designated postnatal day 0 (PND 0). On PND 1, pups were weighed and genders were determined. All litters were culled to four males and four females. Rat littermates of each sex were housed together throughout the experiment. Litters were weighed on PND 21 and PND 60. On PND 60, mortality rates in the control and morphine-exposed groups were determined from the number of offspring remaining divided by the total number of offspring on PND 0.

Maternal Behavior Test

Maternal behavior tests were performed in the postpartum female rats in the control and morphine-exposed groups as described previously (Zhao and Li, 2012). Detailed procedure has been described in the Supplementary Materials and Methods.

IGF-2 Lentivirus Production

High-titer lentivirus vectors encoding IGF-2 gene (LV-IGF2-EGFP) (pGV287-IGF2-EGFP, 2.2 × 109 U/ml) or GFP only (lentivirus encoding enhanced green fluorescent protein gene (LV-EGFP) (pGV287EGFP, 1.8 × 109 U/ml) were obtained from Genenchem (Shanghai, China). The IGF-2 gene was ligased into pGV-287 plasmid (Genenchem, Shanghai, China) to generate pGV287-IGF-2. Production of LV-IGF2-EGFP or LV-EGFP is described in the Supplementary Materials and Methods.

Bilateral Intra-DG Injection

The method of intrainjection of lentivirus into the DG region of the bilateral hippocampus was described in our previous study with a slight modification (Dai et al, 2011). In brief, rats were anesthetized with an intraperitoneal injection of chloral hydrate (40 mg/kg), and securely placed into a stereotaxic device with bregma and lambda at a horizontal level. Two 30-G stainless-steel cannulae with 33-G stainless-steel stylet plugs were bilaterally implanted 0.5 mm above the DG regions of the hippocampus: anteroposterior −3.50 mm, relative to bregma; lateral ±1.50 mm; and dorsoventral 3.5 mm from skull, according to the atlas of Paxinos and Watson (1998).

Behavioral Tests

Elevated plus-maze test

Anxiety-like behavior was tested using the elevated plus-maze (EPM) test, as described previously (Weaver et al, 2006). Briefly, rats were placed in the middle of EPM apparatus, and their behavior was recorded by video camera for 5 min. As an index of the anxiety-like behavior, data were expressed as the percentage of time spent in the open arms and the percentage of open-arm entries, and as an index of overall locomotor activity, a total number of open- and closed-arm entries. The maze was cleaned after each trial.

Open-field test

The open-field test (OFT) was undertaken as described previously (Weaver et al, 2006). Briefly, animals were placed directly in one corner of the open field (120 cm × 120 cm), which was divided into an 8 × 8 grid of squares. Movement of each animal in the area was video-recorded during the 5-min testing session. An observer blind to the experimental conditions analyzed the movement from the video recordings. Exploration was defined as the time spent in the inner 6 × 6 squares, and overall activity was defined as the number of squares crossed during the testing session.

Morris water-maze test

Morris water-maze test was undertaken as described previously (Li et al, 2008). Detailed information has been depicted in the Supplementary Materials and Methods.

Adolescent EE Experience

After weaning, the offspring from the groups that experienced EE were placed in a large vitrine (60 cm × 60 cm × 40 cm), as a cohort of 12, for 4 h every day. Inside the vitrine, there was approximately 2 cm of wood shavings covering the base, and toys (eg, wheels, ladders, plastic tunnels, wooden shelters, mazes made of colored building blocks, swings, wheel-runners) that were rotated every 2 days. Offspring from the control group (no EE experience) stayed in their regular home cages during this time. During non-experimental hours, littermates from the EE condition groups were returned to their regular home cage and housed together. Littermates without EE condition were kept in home cage and housed together throughout the experiments.

Golgi Staining and Quantitative Analysis of Dendrites and Spine

Brains tissues from five rats from each group were processed for Golgi staining as described previously by us and others (Li et al, 2008; Wallace et al, 2006). Detailed experimental procedure has been described in the Supplementary Methods. Four to six granule cells within the DG per animal were imaged and traced for the analysis of dendritic length and branching. We selected for tracing only well-impregnated cells with soma located in the middle plane of the section, without broken arbors and no overlap with neighboring cells. Neurodraw and Image Prosoftware (MicroBrightField, Williston, VT, USA) were used for all analyses. Experimenters who carried out the analyses were blind to group identities. To determine dendritic spine density, spines from each of the neurons selected were counted on two 20 μm unbranched dendrite segments 120 μm away from the cell body at high power ( × 1000 magnification) and data were expressed as the number of spines per μm of dendrite length. For each animal, cell values were averaged to derive a single value per variable (total dendritic length, number of branching points, and spine density).

Immunohistochemistry

IGF-2 immunohistochemistry was performed as described in our previous study (Dai et al, 2011). Briefly, transverse sections of the brain (30 μm) were cut with a cryostat and mounted on 3-aminopropyl triethoxy-silane-coated slides, and rabbit anti-IGF2 antibodies (dilution 1:200; Abcam, Eugene, OR). The number of IGF-2-positive neurons was measured by an author blind with respect to the treatments.

Western Blot

Western blot analysis was performed as described by our previous study with a slight modification (Dai et al, 2011). Briefly, frozen hippocampus tissues were homogenized in a lysis buffer containing protease inhibitors cocktail (Roche, Mannheim, Germany) and PMSF (Sigma, St Louis, MO, USA). Samples were then centrifuged at 10 000 r.p.m. for 15 min at 4 °C. The supernatants were used for western blotting for IGF-2 with rabbit anti-IGF-2 (Abcam).

RNA Extraction, Reverse Transcription and Real-Time PCR

Total RNA was isolated from the hippocampus using TRIzol reagent based on the company protocol (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA, USA). Real-time PCR assay protocol was performed using SYBR Green Real-Time PCR Master Mixes (Roche) as described by our previous studies (Ruan et al, 2012). PCR primers were 5′-GCCGACAGGATGCAGAAGGAGATCA-3′ and 5′-AAGCATTTGCGGTGGACGATGGA-3′ (β-actin); 5′-AGGGGAGCTTGTTGACACG-3′ and 5′-GGGTATCTGGGGAAGTCGTC-3′ (IGF-2); 5′-ACCAGAGACCCTTTGCGGGGCT-3′; and 5′-AAGTGTACTTCCTTCTGAGTCT-3′ (IGF-1). The specific genes were normalized to the level of β-actin in each individual sample.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using software SPSS 13.0 (SPSS, Chicago, IL) or GraphPad Prism 5 (GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA, USA). Data are presented as means±SEM. Differences between groups were compared using repeated-measures one-way ANOVA, followed by post hoc Bonferroni multiple comparison tests, where appropriate. Unpaired two-tailed Student’s -test was used if only two groups were applied. Unless otherwise specified, those were the statistical analyses used in all the sections reported below. We considered differences to be significant if probability values were ⩽0.05.

RESULTS

There were no significant differences apparent in weight and ratio of gender among all offspring of the control and morphine-exposed groups in the indicated times measured. No significant differences in mortality rate on PND 60 were observed among the offspring of the control and morphine-exposed groups (Supplementary Table S1).

Development of Anxiety-Like Behaviors in the Adult Offspring of Morphine Exposure

As Figure 1a shows, parental morphine exposure reduced the time spent in the open arm of the EPM in the male offspring, F (3, 88)=12.51, p<0.0001, and female offspring, F (3, 84)=12.56, p<0.0001. Post hoc tests showed that time spent in the open arms in the male or female offspring of morphine-exposed parents was significantly lower than for the corresponding control groups (male offspring, p=0.009, 0.001, and 0.004, respectively, for the paternal, maternal, or biparental morphine-exposed groups vs the control groups; in female offspring, p=0.004, 0.006, and 0.004, respectively, for paternal, maternal, or biparental groups vs the corresponding control groups). Similarly, a significant difference in the number of open-arm entries was also observed in the male, F (3, 58)=5.741, p<0.01, and female offspring, F (3, 58)=3.662, p<0.01, of morphine-exposed groups. Post hoc tests shows the significantly less number of open-arm entries in the offspring of morphine-exposed groups than that of control group (male offspring, p=0.026, p=0.029 and p=0.034, respectively, for the paternal, maternal or bi-parental morphine-exposed groups vs the control groups; in female offspring, p=0.034, 0.026, and 0.036, respectively, for paternal, maternal, or biparental groups vs the corresponding control groups)

Figure 1
figure 1

Anxiety-like behaviors in the elevated plus-maze (EPM) and open-field test (OFT) in the offspring of morphine-exposed groups. (a and b) Quantitative analysis of the time spent in the open arm (a) and the percentage of open arm entries (b) in EPM in both male and female offspring of the morphine-exposed groups. (c) Reduced time spent in the inner area in the offspring of morphine-exposed groups in OFT. (d) Quantitative analysis of total travel distance among offspring of the control and morphine-exposed groups. Values are expressed as a mean±SEM. *P<0.05 and **p<0.001, respectively, vs offspring of the control group.

PowerPoint slide

OFT showed that the time spent in the inner area was markedly reduced in the adult offspring of the parents exposed to morphine, as compared with the offspring of the control parent groups, F (3, 88)=29.371, p<0.001 for male offspring, and F (3, 83)=26.92, p<0.001 for female offspring. There were no significant differences in the total travel distances between the offspring of morphine-exposed groups and their corresponding control offspring, F<1.00 for both male and female groups.

There was no significant difference in the latency to find the platform in the adult offspring of morphine-exposed groups as compared with those of the corresponding control groups in the Morris water-maze test, F<1.00 for both male and female groups (Supplementary Figure S1). Thus, parental morphine exposure mainly resulted in the anxious phenotype, but did not impair the learning and memory in their adult offspring.

Effects of Morphine Exposure on Dendritic Morphogenesis in the DG of the Hippocampus in their Adult Offspring

Given that synaptic plasticity is implicated in the pathogenesis of anxiety and depression (Bessa et al, 2009; Kheirbek and Hen, 2011), we performed Golgi staining to examine dendritic morphology in the DG (Figure 2). Both in the male and female offspring of the morphine-exposed groups, the dendritic length (Figure 2e) were greatly reduced as compared with the corresponding offspring of the control groups (F (3, 16)=24.08, p<0.001, F (3, 16)=9.009, p<0.001, respectively, for male and female offspring). Dendritic branching number (Figure 2f) was also reduced (F (2, 16)=3.906, p=0.029, F (3, 16)=5.714, p=0.007, respectively, for male and female offspring). No significant differences were observed in the spine density among the offspring of all groups (Figure 2g). Post hoc analyses showed that, within the offspring of morphine-exposed fathers, mothers, or both parents, there were no significant differences in dendritic length, branch points, or spine density when compared with each other. These findings indicate that parents’ exposure to morphine—whether maternal, paternal, or biparental—led to the dendritic retraction in both their male and female offspring.

Figure 2
figure 2

Effect of adult morphine exposure on dendritic morphogenesis in the dentate gyrus (DG) of the hippocampus in their offspring. (a and b) Representative photomicrographs (a) and its reconstruction (b) of Golgi staining in the subgranular zone of the DG. Arrow indicates the cell body of neuron. (c–e) Quantitative analysis of dendritic length (c), number of branching points (f) and spine density, as indicated by the number of spine per μm (g). Values are expressed as a mean±SEM. *P<0.05 and **p<0.001, respectively, vs offspring of the control group.

PowerPoint slide

As the behavioral and hippocampal morphologic changes in the offspring of parental morphine exposure may be due to the maternal care alterations (Kikusui and Mori, 2009), we evaluated maternal behaviors on postpartum days 6–8, as described previously (Zhao and Li, 2012). As shown in Supplementary Figure S2, among the different groups with or without parental morphine exposure, there were no significant differences in the duration of crouching ((F (3,15)=0.078, p>0.05), duration of pup retrieval ((F (3,15)=0.177, p>0.05), duration of pup licking (F (3, 15)=0.508, p>0.05), and duration of nest building (F (3,15)=0.038, p>0.05). Thus, the anxious phenotype of the offspring of morphine-exposed parents is unlikely because of the inadequate maternal care.

Effect of the Morphine Exposure on the Hippocampal IGF-2 Expression in their Adult Offspring

IGF-2 has recently been reported to be involved in dendritic plasticity and memory consolidation and enhancement (Agis-Balboa et al, 2011; Chen et al, 2011; Schmeisser et al, 2012). Thus, we hypothesized that the dendritic retraction and anxiety-like behavior may be closely associated with IGF-2 signaling. As shown in Figure 3a, IGF-2 mRNA markedly decreased in the offspring of paternal, maternal, and parental morphine-exposed groups, as compared with those of the control groups. However, the IGF-1 gene was not altered in the offspring of the parents with morphine exposure (Figure 3b). Immunohistochemistry and western blot also showed that IGF-2 expressed in the neurons was significantly decreased in the offspring of parents with morphine exposure compared with controls (Figures 3c and d). These findings suggest the downregulation of IGF-2 in the DG region in the offspring of morphine-exposed parents.

Figure 3
figure 3

Downregulation of hippocampal insulin growth factor-2 (IGF-2) in the offspring of adult morphine-exposed groups. (a and b) Quantitative analysis of IGF-2 (a) and IGF-1 (b) mRNA levels in the hippocampus in the offspring of the control or morphine-exposed groups. (c) IGF-2 immunohistochemistry in the hippocampus. (A) representative photomicrographs of IGF-2 immunohistochemical staining in the offspring of the control group; (B) higher magnification of inset in (A); (C) higher magnification of inset in (B); (D–F) representative photomicrograph of IGF-2 expression in the offspring of adult male (D), female (E), and both (F) morphine exposure. Bar, 100 μm. (d) Representative western blot image of IGF-2 in the hippocampus. (e) Semiquantitative analysis of IGF-2 protein expression in the offspring of morphine-exposed or the control groups. Values are expressed as a mean±SEM. *P<0.05 and **p<0.001, respectively, vs offspring of the control group.

PowerPoint slide

Overexpression of IGF-2 in the Adolescent Period Prevented Anxiety in the Adult Offspring

To further investigate whether downregulation of IGF-2 in the hippocampus contributed to anxiety-like behavior and dendritic shortening, we constructed the LV-EGFP (control) or LV-IGF2-EGFP for IGF-2 overexpression. LV-IGF2-EGFP or LV-EGFP was bilaterally injected into the DG region of the hippocampus in the male and female offspring of morphine-exposed parents on PND 28, a critical period of adolescence. On PND 60, the injected rats undertook the behavior tests, followed by biochemical examination. As shown in Figure 4b, EGFP was highly expressed in the injection zone at 32 days after injection, indicating successful overexpression of the target gene. Consistently, IGF-2 mRNA and protein were also highly expressed in the hippocampus after bilateral injection of LV-IGF2-EGFP (Figures 4c–g).

Figure 4
figure 4

Effects of overexpression of insulin growth factor-2 (IGF-2) in the adolescent offspring of morphine-exposed groups on anxiety-like behavior in their adulthood (a and b) Representative photomicrographs of Hoechst (blue, staining nuclei) and green fluorescent protein (GFP) expression (green) in the dentate gyrus (DG) after bilateral injection of LV-IGF2-EGFP or LV-EGFP. (c and d) Quantitative analysis of IGF-2 gene expression in adult male (c) and female (d) offspring receiving LV-IGF2-EGFP or LV-EGFP injection in adolescence. (e–g) Representative western blot image (e) and its semiquantitative analysis of IGF-2 expression in male (f) and female (g) offspring after LV-IGF2-EGFP or LV-EGFP injection. (h and i) Quantitative analysis of the time spent in the open arm in male (h) and female (i) offspring of morphine-exposed groups after LV-EGF2-EGFP or LV-EGFP injection. (j and k) Quantitative analysis of time spent inner area in the male (j) or female (k) offspring received LV-IGF2-EGFP or LV-EGFP bilateral DG injection. Values are expressed as a mean±SEM. *P<0.05 and **p<0.001, respectively, rats received LV-IGF-2 injection vs the responding littermate received LV-EGFP injection; ##p<0.01 and ###p<0.001, offspring of the morphine-exposed group vs offspring of the control group. LV-IGF2-EGFP: lentivirus encoding IGF-2 gene; LV-EGFP: lentivirus delivered EGFP gene, the reporter gene.

PowerPoint slide

Rats overexpressing IGF-2 in the hippocampus exhibited more time spent in the open-arm (Figures 4h and i) (F (3,54)=32.73, p<0.01 in male groups, F (3,56)=35.58, p<0.01 in female groups) and more time spent in the inner area (Figures 4j and k) (F (3,54)=27.86, p<0.01 in male groups, F (3,56)=31.12, p<0.01 in female groups) as compared with the littermates injected by LV-EGFP. This indicates that IGF-2 overexpression could prevent the development of anxiety-like behaviors in the offspring of rats with morphine exposure.

Effect of Adolescent EE on Anxiety and Hippocampal IGF-2 Expression in the Offspring

Early life environmental enrichment (EE) experience reduces the anxious phenotype in the rats exposed to stressors (Baldini et al, 2013; Francis et al, 2002). Therefore, we attempted to examine whether adolescent EE exposure could reverse the adult anxiety-like behavior in the offspring of parent animals exposed to morphine.

As shown in Figures 5a and b, in male and female offspring of different morphine-exposed groups, rats with adolescent EE experience exhibited more time spent in the EPM open arms than littermates without EE experience (F (3,102)=49.07, p<0.01 in male groups, F (3,103)=31.08, p<0.01 in female groups). In the offspring of the control group, rats with adolescent EE experience did not increase the time spent in EPM open arms (p>0.05 for both male and female offspring). Similarly, rats that received adolescent EE experience spent more time in the inner area of the open field than littermates without EE (Figures 5c and d). Thus, adolescent EE experience appeared to prevent the occurrence of anxiety-like behaviors in the offspring of rats with morphine exposure.

Figure 5
figure 5

Anxiety-like responses in rats receiving adolescent environmental enrichment (EE), in relation to hippocampal insulin growth factor-2 (IGF-2) signaling. (a–d) Quantitative analysis of the time spent in open arm (a and b) in elevated plus-maze experiment, and the time spent in the open arm (c and d) in open-field test in the adolescent offspring receiving EE or the responding littermate without EE experience. (e and f) Real-time PCR showing IGF-2 gene expression in the male (e) and female (f) offspring with or without EE experience. (g–i) Western blot of IGF-2 (g) and its semiquantitative analysis in IGF-2 protein expression in the male (h) and female (i) offspring with or without EE experience. Values are expressed as a mean±SEM. *P<0.05 and **p<0.001, respectively, rats received EE experience vs the responding littermate without EE experience; ##p<0.01 and ###p<0.001, offspring of the morphine-exposed group vs offspring of the control group.

PowerPoint slide

In the offspring of rats with morphine exposure, the length and branching points in dendrites of neurons in the DG region of the hippocampus were less than the offspring of the control group (Figures 2b and c). However, among the adult offspring with adolescent EE experience, there was no significant difference in dendritic parameters between the morphine-exposed groups and the control group (Supplementary Figure S3). These findings suggest that adolescent EE experience could normalize the dendritic retraction in the offspring of parental morphine exposure.

As the downregulation of hippocampal IGF-2 contributed to anxious phenotype in the offspring of morphine-exposed groups, we speculated that IGF-2 signaling might mediate the preventive effect of adolescent EE on anxiety. Indeed, adolescent EE could reverse the downregulation of IGF-2 in the hippocampus in the male (Figures 5g and h) and female offspring (Figures 5g and i). These findings support the hypothesis that adolescent EE reduces the anxious phenotype through hippocampal IGF-2 signaling.

DISCUSSION

Extensive studies have shown that adolescent maternal morphine exposure can lead to anxiety-like behavior in their female offspring (for a review see Vassoler et al, 2014). In male rats, adolescent morphine exposure delayed sexual maturation, resulted in smaller litters, and induced significant endocrine modification in their offspring as adults (Cicero et al, 1991). In the present study, adult morphine exposure did not affect general parameters perhaps because the animals had reached full sexual maturity when the drug was administered. Previous studies showed sex-differential behavior in the offspring of adolescent morphine-exposed rats (Byrnes, 2005; Johnson et al, 2011). Female offspring developed anxiety-like behavior, whereas male offspring demonstrated significantly enhanced morphine sensitization (Byrnes, 2005; Johnson et al, 2011). By contrast, in our study, both male and female offspring demonstrated anxiety-like behavior in the EPM and OFT procedures. The discrepant observations of anxiety-like behavior in the present study compared with previous studies are likely due to age-related differences in the effect of morphine exposure. Indeed, adolescence represents a period of significant brain maturation consisting of behavioral characteristics, cortex myelin accretion, and drug vulnerability (Chen et al, 2009; Mengler et al, 2014; Spear, 2000). In contrast, in the adult with the endocrinal and brain maturity, the sex-differential response of offspring may not be obvious. Supporting this hypothesis, a previous electrophysiologic study showed that long-term potentiation in the hippocampus was impaired in both male and female offspring of morphine-exposed mothers, fathers, and both parents (Sarkaki et al, 2008). Further study may be warranted to investigate the age-dependent effect of morphine-exposed animals on their offspring.

Impaired maternal care during the early developmental period could lead to behavioral and neuroanatomical changes in offspring at adulthood (Kikusui and Mori, 2009). In the present study, classic maternal behaviors—including duration of crouching, licking, retrieving pups, and nest building—were not significantly different among the control and morphine-exposed groups. In addition, paternal morphine exposure alone also brought out the anxiety-like behavior in the offspring. Thus, the anxious phenotype and dendritic retraction in the adult offspring of morphine-exposed mothers, fathers, and both parents were unlikely because of inadequate maternal care in the early developmental period.

Dendritic and synaptic plasticity is implicated in the pathogenesis of anxiety and depression (Eisch and Petrik, 2012; Kheirbek and Hen, 2011). Concomitant with the anxious phenotype, we found reduced lengths and numbers of dendritic branches in DG neurons in the offspring of the morphine-exposed mothers, fathers, and both parents. Direct prenatal morphine exposure is known to reduce dendrite length and branch number in the adult offspring (Lu et al, 2012; Mei et al, 2009). However, the dendritic deficits in the animals from this study did not result from direct exposure to morphine, as the morphine treatments of their parents terminated several weeks before the initiation of mating. Thus, the intergenerational effects of morphine exposure may be mediated by other mechanisms such as epigenetic mechanisms or persistent stress (Czeh et al, 2001).

IGF-2 in the hippocampus has been reported to be involved in neurogenesis and dendritic plasticity, which are greatly inhibited in anxiety and depression (Bracko et al, 2012; Mateus-Pinheiro et al, 2013; Schmeisser et al, 2012). Although elevation of hippocampal IGF-2 is closely related to the reduced anxiety scores in stressed rats (Cline et al, 2012), the role of IGF-2 in the pathogenesis of anxiety is obscure. In the present study, hippocampal IGF-2 expression was downregulated, accompanied by the dendritic retraction in the offspring of morphine-exposed groups. Overexpression of IGF-2 in the hippocampus of the adolescent offspring prevented the anxiety-like behaviors in the adult. These findings suggest that downregulation of IGF-2 contributes to the dendritic retraction and anxiety in the adult offspring of rats with morphine exposure. More interestingly, overexpression of IGF-2 in the hippocampus also reduced the anxious response in the offspring of control group. The antianxious response of IGF-2 in the normal rats may be due to the increased IGF-2 promotes dendritic morphogenesis in the hippocampus.

Early life EE could protect from, or remediate, the development of highly emotional and anxious responses in the rats exposed to stressors (Baldini et al, 2013; Francis et al, 2002). In the present study, adolescent EE evidently prevented the anxiety-like behaviors and dendritic retraction in the adult offspring of morphine-exposed rats, suggesting that early life EE can affect brain development and shape individual differences in anxiety and depression. However, the mechanisms underlying the effects of EE on anxiety and dendritic plasticity are not clear. One possibility is that EE may regulate neurotrophins which could couple the neuroplasticity and anxiety/depression. In the work reported here, IGF-2 can be upregulated by adolescent EE. These findings indicate that hippocampal IGF-2 has a critical role in linking the effects of adolescent EE with the brain plasticity and the antianxiety effect.

In conclusion, the present study has shown that adultmorphine exposure renders anxiety and hippocampal dendritic retraction in adult offspring. Hippocampal IGF-2 signaling may be a critical mediator that links neuropsychiatric disorders with the dendritic remodeling in the intergenerational effects of morphine exposure. The data indicate that adolescent EE experience prevented the behavioral and brain structural changes in the animal during adulthood, via hippocampal IGF-2 signaling. Given the high incidence of opiate exposure in adult humans, the potential for behavioral changes in their offspring should be taken as a serious public health concern. Timely intervention by EE or pharmacologic treatment targeting IGF-2 signaling may be simple and helpful methods to prevent psychological disorders in the offspring of chronic opiate users, thus potentially reducing the risk of serious social burden.

FUNDING AND DISCLOSURE

The author(s) declare that no financial support or compensation has been received from any individual or corporate entity over the past 3 years for research or professional services and there are no personal financial holdings that could be perceived as constituting a potential conflict of interest.