Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Letter
  • Published:

Odour-imagery ability is linked to food craving, intake, and adiposity change in humans

Abstract

It is well-known that food-cue reactivity (FCR) is positively associated with body mass index (BMI)1 and weight change2, but the mechanisms underlying these relationships are incompletely understood. One prominent theory of craving posits that the elaboration of a desired substance through sensory imagery intensifies cravings, thereby promoting consumption3. Olfaction is integral to food perception, yet the ability to imagine odours varies widely4. Here we test in a basic observational study whether this large variation in olfactory imagery drives FCR strength to promote adiposity in 45 adults (23 male). We define odour-imagery ability as the extent to which imagining an odour interferes with the detection of a weak incongruent odour (the ‘interference effect’5). As predicted in our preregistration, the interference effect correlates with the neural decoding of imagined, but not real, odours. These perceptual and neural measures of odour imagery are in turn associated with FCR, defined by the rated craving intensity of liked foods and cue-potentiated intake. Finally, odour imagery exerts positive indirect effects on changes in BMI and body-fat percentage over one year via its influences on FCR. These findings establish odour imagery as a driver of FCR that in turn confers risk for weight gain.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Study overview and the perceptual measure of odour-imagery ability.
Fig. 2: Decoding of imagined, but not actual, odours in the right piriform cortex provides a neural measure of odour-imagery ability.
Fig. 3: Better odour-imagery ability is associated with stronger cravings for liked foods and greater intake.
Fig. 4: Odour-imagery ability indirectly predicts changes in BMI and body-fat percentage via FCR.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The raw MRI data and sniff airflow traces can be downloaded from the OpenNEURO repository under accession no. ds004327 at: https://doi.org/10.18112/openneuro.ds004327.v1.0.1. Statistical maps of the human brain data are available on the NeuroVault repository at: https://neurovault.org/collections/14751/. Source data are provided with this paper.

Code availability

Custom code used in data collection and analysis is available at: https://github.com/eeperszyk/odor-imagery.

References

  1. Hendrikse, J. J. et al. Attentional biases for food cues in overweight and individuals with obesity: a systematic review of the literature. Obes. Rev. 16, 424–432 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Boswell, R. G. & Kober, H. Food cue reactivity and craving predict eating and weight gain: a meta-analytic review. Obes. Rev. 17, 159–177 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Kavanagh, D. J., Andrade, J. & May, J. Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: the elaborated intrusion theory of desire. Psychol. Rev. 112, 446–467 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Schifferstein, H. N. J. Comparing mental imagery across the sensory modalities. Imagin. Cogn. Pers. 28, 371–388 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Djordjevic, J., Zatorre, R. J., Petrides, M. & Jones-Gotman, M. The mind’s nose: effects of odor and visual imagery on odor detection. Psychol. Sci. 15, 143–148 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Bensafi, M. & Rouby, C. Individual differences in odor imaging ability reflect differences in olfactory and emotional perception. Chem. Senses 32, 237–244 (2007).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Marks, D. F. Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. Br. J. Psychol. 64, 17–24 (1973).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Patel, B. P., Aschenbrenner, K., Shamah, D. & Small, D. M. Greater perceived ability to form vivid mental images in individuals with high compared to low BMI. Appetite 91, 185–189 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Djordjevic, J., Zatorre, R. J., Petrides, M., Boyle, J. A. & Jones-Gotman, M. Functional neuroimaging of odor imagery. NeuroImage 24, 791–801 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Howard, J. D., Plailly, J., Grueschow, M., Haynes, J.-D. & Gottfried, J. A. Odor quality coding and categorization in human posterior piriform cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 932–938 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Zatorre, R. J., Jones-Gotman, M., Evans, A. C. & Meyer, E. Functional localization and lateralization of human olfactory cortex. Nature 360, 339–340 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Zatorre, R. J. & Jones-Gotman, M. Human olfactory discrimination after unilateral frontal or temporal lobectomy. Brain 114A, 71–84 (1991).

    Google Scholar 

  13. Wei, C.-S. et al. Editorial: inter- and intra-subject variability in brain imaging and decoding. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 15, 791129 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Stettler, D. D. & Axel, R. Representations of odor in the piriform cortex. Neuron 63, 854–864 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Boswell, R. G., Sun, W., Suzuki, S. & Kober, H. Training in cognitive strategies reduces eating and improves food choice. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115, E11238–E11247 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Robinson, E. et al. The bogus taste test: validity as a measure of laboratory food intake. Appetite 116, 223–231 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. O’Rourke, H. P. & MacKinnon, D. P. Reasons for testing mediation in the absence of an intervention effect: a research imperative in prevention and intervention research. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 79, 171–181 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Preacher, K. J. & Hayes, A. F. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 36, 717–731 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Moulton, S. T. & Kosslyn, S. M. Imagining predictions: mental imagery as mental emulation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 364, 1273–1280 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. de Araujo, I. E., Schatzker, M. & Small, D. M. Rethinking food reward. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 71, 139–164 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Tsuneki, H. et al. Food odor perception promotes systemic lipid utilization. Nat. Metab. 4, 1514–1531 (2022).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Gottfried, J. A., Winston, J. S. & Dolan, R. J. Dissociable codes of odor quality and odorant structure in human piriform cortex. Neuron 49, 467–479 (2006).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Ganis, G., Thompson, W. L. & Kosslyn, S. M. Brain areas underlying visual mental imagery and visual perception: an fMRI study. Cogn. Brain Res. 20, 226–241 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Steel, A., Billings, M. M., Silson, E. H. & Robertson, C. E. A network linking scene perception and spatial memory systems in posterior cerebral cortex. Nat. Commun. 12, 2632 (2021).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Perszyk, E. E., Davis, X. S. & Small, D. M. Olfactory decoding is positively associated with ad libitum food intake in sated humans. Appetite 180, 106351 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Stafford, L. D. & Whittle, A. Obese individuals have higher preference and sensitivity to odor of chocolate. Chem. Senses 40, 279–284 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Han, P., Chen, H. & Hummel, T. Brain responses to food odors associated with BMI change at 2-year follow-up. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14, 402 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Patel, Z. M., DelGaudio, J. M. & Wise, S. K. Higher body mass index is associated with subjective olfactory dysfunction. Behav. Neurol. 2015, e675635 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Sun, X. et al. Basolateral amygdala response to food cues in the absence of hunger is associated with weight gain susceptibility. J. Neurosci. 35, 7964–7976 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Poessel, M. et al. Brain response to food odors is not associated with body mass index and obesity-related metabolic health measures. Appetite 168, 105774 (2021).

  31. Han, P., Roitzsch, C., Horstmann, A., Pössel, M. & Hummel, T. Increased brain reward responsivity to food-related odors in obesity. Obesity 29, 1138–1145 (2021).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Li, G. et al. Brain functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging of obesity and weight loss interventions. Mol. Psychiatry 28, 1466–1479 (2023).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Dalton, P. Odor perception and beliefs about risk. Chem. Senses 21, 447–458 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G. & Buchner, A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A. & Lang, A.-G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Peirce, J. W. PsychoPy—psychophysics software in Python. J. Neurosci. Methods 162, 8–13 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Bartoshuk, L. M. et al. Valid across-group comparisons with labeled scales: the gLMS versus magnitude matching. Physiol. Behav. 82, 109–114 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Green, B. G., Shaffer, G. S. & Gilmore, M. M. Derivation and evaluation of a semantic scale of oral sensation magnitude with apparent ratio properties. Chem. Senses 18, 683–702 (1993).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Green, B. G. et al. Evaluating the ‘labeled magnitude scale’ for measuring sensations of taste and smell. Chem. Senses 21, 323–334 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Lim, J., Wood, A. & Green, B. G. Derivation and evaluation of a labeled hedonic scale. Chem. Senses 34, 739–751 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Gilbert, A., Voss, M. & Kroll, J. Vividness of olfactory mental imagery: correlations with sensory response and consumer behavior. Chem. Senses 22, 686 (1997).

    Google Scholar 

  42. Hagströmer, M., Oja, P. & Sjöström, M. The International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ): a study of concurrent and construct validity. Public Health Nutr. 9, 755–762 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Francis, H. & Stevenson, R. Validity and test–retest reliability of a short dietary questionnaire to assess intake of saturated fat and free sugars: a preliminary study. J. Hum. Nutr. Dietetics 26, 234–242 (2013).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Doty, R. L. Office procedures for quantitative assessment of olfactory function. Am. J. Rhinol. 21, 460–473 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Small, D. M., Veldhuizen, M. G., Felsted, J., Mak, Y. E. & McGlone, F. Separable substrates for anticipatory and consummatory food chemosensation. Neuron 57, 786–797 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Hayes, A. F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach 2nd edn (Guilford Publications, 2017).

  47. Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W. & Smith, S. M. FSL. NeuroImage 62, 782–790 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M. & Smith, S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage 17, 825–841 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Friston, K. J., Williams, S., Howard, R., Frackowiak, R. S. J. & Turner, R. Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magn. Reson. Med. 35, 346–355 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Power, J. D., Barnes, K. A., Snyder, A. Z., Schlaggar, B. L. & Petersen, S. E. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. NeuroImage 59, 2142–2154 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Bartra, O., McGuire, J. T. & Kable, J. W. The valuation system: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. NeuroImage 76, 412–427 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Mai, J. K., Majtanik, M. & Paxinos, G. Atlas of the Human Brain 4th edn (Academic Press, 2015).

  53. Hammers, A. et al. Three-dimensional maximum probability atlas of the human brain, with particular reference to the temporal lobe. Hum. Brain Mapp. 19, 224–247 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Rolls, E. T., Huang, C.-C., Lin, C.-P., Feng, J. & Joliot, M. Automated anatomical labelling atlas 3. NeuroImage 206, 116189 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Yarkoni, T., Poldrack, R. A., Nichols, T. E., Van Essen, D. C. & Wager, T. D. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat. Methods 8, 665–670 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Hebart, M. N., Görgen, K. & Haynes, J.-D. The Decoding Toolbox (TDT): a versatile software package for multivariate analyses of functional imaging data. Front. Neuroinform. 8, 88 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Chang, C.-C. & Lin, C.-J. LIBSVM: a library for support vector machines. ACM Trans. Intell. Syst. Technol. 2, 27:1–27:27 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship under Grant No. 2139841 (E.E.P.), the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases of the National Institutes of Health under Award No. F31DK130556 (E.E.P.), and the Modern Diet and Physiology Research Center (D.M.S.). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Science Foundation or the National Institutes of Health. We would like to thank J. Avery for advice on the fMRI decoding methods; B. Kuzmanovic for guidance on the fMRI preprocessing pipeline; J. Howard for example code to perform the sniffing analyses; T. Hummel, J. Lundström, J. Mainland, and P. Wise for their thoughts on troubleshooting the odour-detection threshold testing; A. Dagher, R. DiLeone, and B. Green for their helpful suggestions on project design and analyses; and K. Martin for MR technical assistance.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization, E.E.P. and D.M.S.; Methodology, E.E.P., X.S.D., J.D., M.J.-G., J.T., Z.H., M.G.V., L.K., T.D.W., H.K., and D.M.S.; Formal Analysis, E.E.P., L.K., and X.S.D.; Investigation, E.E.P. and J.T.; Resources, X.S.D., J.D., M.J.-G., Z.H., M.G.V., L.K., T.D.W., H.K., and D.M.S.; Data Curation, E.E.P.; Writing – Original Draft, E.E.P. and D.M.S.; Writing – Review & Editing, X.S.D., J.D., M.J.-G., J.T., Z.H., M.G.V., L.K. T.D.W., and H.K.; Visualization, E.E.P.; Supervision, X.S.D., H.K., and D.M.S.; Funding Acquisition, E.E.P. and D.M.S.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Emily E. Perszyk or Dana M. Small.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Metabolism thanks Annette Horstmann, Gene-Jack Wang and Nils Kohn for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling editor: Ashley Castellanos-Jankiewicz, in collaboration with the Nature Metabolism team.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data

Extended Data Fig. 1 The perceptual measure of odour-imagery ability positively correlates with self-reported odour and flavor, but not visual, imagery ability.

a–c, The perceptual measure of odour-imagery ability (that is, the interference effect) positively correlated with self-reported odour (a) and flavor (b), but not visual (c), imagery ability. We note that the self-report and perceptual measures of odour-imagery ability did not vary by sex, age, household income, olfactory function or perception, sniff parameters, hunger, or dietary habits (Supplementary Table 4). Scatterplots depict single participants and the 95% CI around the line of best fit. Linear relationships were tested with two-tailed Pearson’s r correlations. p.p., percentage points, referring to the difference in odour detection accuracies (percentages) during matched versus mismatched trials of the odour-imagery condition from the perceptual task (see Fig. 1e,f); VOIQ, Vividness of Olfactory Imagery Questionnaire41; VFIQ, Vividness of Food Imagery Questionnaire8; VVIQ, Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire7; *Pcorrected < 0.05 (3 tests comparing the interference effect to self-reported odour, flavor, or visual-imagery ability).

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 2 Decoding is not significant in primary visual cortex control regions.

a, Control regions for the neural decoding analyses included the left and right primary visual cortices. bc, SVM accuracies (b) and voxel correlations (c) were not significant for real, imagined, or cross-modal odours in either control region. Box-and-whisker plots represent single participants from the minimum to maximum (whiskers) around the 25th to 75th percentiles (box limits), along with the median (center line) and mean (+symbol) of the data. L, left; R, right; SVM, support vector machine; V1, primary visual cortex. See also Fig. 2 for additional details on the two decoding methods and significant effects in the piriform cortex regions of interest.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 3 Univariate fMRI activity to smelling and imagining odours.

a, BOLD responses to smelling odours (rose and cookie) > smelling clean air were significant in the bilateral insula, piriform/amygdala, orbitofrontal cortices, cerebellum, and middle frontal and cingulate gyri, among other regions (Supplementary Table 10). Whole-brain statistical map can be viewed at: https://neurovault.org/images/798927/. b, BOLD responses to imagining odours > imagining clean air (while sniffing) were significant in the bilateral insula, right putamen, and left cerebellum (Supplementary Table 11). Whole-brain statistical map can be viewed at: https://neurovault.org/images/798926/. c, BOLD responses to imagining odours > smelling clean air were significant in the bilateral insula, putamen extending into the piriform cortices, pallidum, and orbitofrontal, middle frontal, and precentral gyri, among other regions (Supplementary Table 12). Whole-brain statistical map can be viewed at: https://neurovault.org/images/798923/. d, BOLD responses in the conjunction of smelling odours > smelling clean air and imagining odours > imagining clean air were significant in the bilateral insula and putamen extending into the piriform cortices, along with the left precentral gyrus (Supplementary Table 13). Whole-brain statistical map can be viewed at: https://neurovault.org/images/798917/. e, BOLD responses to smelling odours > imagining odours were significant in the bilateral insula and amygdala along with the right uncus and orbitofrontal cortex, among other regions (Supplementary Table 13). Those to imagining odours > smelling odours were significant in the left supplementary motor area (Supplementary Table 13). Whole-brain statistical map can be viewed at: https://neurovault.org/images/798924/. Brain sections show the SPM t-map (Puncorrected < 0.005, clusters of at least 5 voxels) overlaid onto an anatomical template in MNI coordinates for illustrative purposes. In each panel, the top row depicts 3D coronal sections (18–mm thick) evenly spanning y = 56 to –88 mm (for ac) or y = 56 to –16 mm (for de), and the bottom row highlights important areas of activation with custom coordinates (see Supplementary Tables 1013). Color bars depict t values. L, left; R, right; Amyg, amygdala; Ins, insula; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; Pir, piriform cortex; Put, putamen; SMA, supplementary motor area.

Extended Data Fig. 4 Odour-imagery ability is not associated with changes in adiposity.

ac, Neither the self-report (a), perceptual (b), nor neural (c) measure of odour-imagery ability predicted changes in BMI over one year from the baseline to follow-up sessions. df, Neither the self-report (d), perceptual (e), nor neural (f) measure of odour-imagery ability predicted changes in body-fat percentage over one year from the baseline to follow-up sessions. Scatterplots depict single participants and the 95% CI around the line of best fit. Linear relationships were tested with two-tailed Pearson’s r correlations. As no effects were significant, all P-values were left uncorrected. a.u., arbitrary units, referring to sex-adjusted changes in body-fat percentage over one year; p.p., percentage points, referring to the difference in odour detection accuracies (percentages) during matched versus mismatched trials of the odour-imagery condition from the perceptual task (see Fig. 1e,f); VOIQ, Vividness of Olfactory Imagery Questionnaire41; R, right; Pir, piriform.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 5 Participant flow diagram.

Flow diagram depicting the number of individuals at each stage of the study.

Extended Data Fig. 6 odour rating comparisons for rose versus cookie.

ad, The cookie odour was rated to be significantly more intense (a), familiar (b), and edible (c) than the rose odour in two-sided paired samples t-tests. There was no significant difference in liking (d). However, the cookie minus rose odour ratings were not correlated with any measure of odour-imagery ability (Supplementary Table 4). Truncated violin plots depict single participants (n = 44) with shading to represent the density of the points around the median line. R, rose; C, cookie; gLMS, general Labeled Magnitude Scale37,38,39; VAS, visual analogue scale; LHS, Labeled Hedonic Scale40; n.s., not significant. *Pcorrected < 0.05 (4 tests).

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 7 Sniff parameters for smelling and imagining the rose and cookie odours.

ad, Normalized sniff traces (mean ± s.e.m.) for smelling the rose (a) and cookie (b) odours and imagining the rose (c) and cookie (d) odours. e–j, Sniff amplitude (e), latency (f), volume (g), duration (h), peak airflow rate (i), and mean airflow rate (j) while smelling and imagining the rose and cookie odours. Differences in the sniff parameters for imagining the cookie minus rose odour were not correlated with any measure of odour-imagery ability (Supplementary Table 4). ANOVAs also revealed no main effects or interactions of modality (smell/imagine), odour (rose/cookie), or the perceptual measure of odour-imagery ability (the interference effect) on any sniff parameter (Supplementary Table 9). Truncated violin plots depict single participants (n = 44) with shading to represent the density of the points around the median line. S, smell; I, imagine; R, rose; C, cookie; a.u., arbitrary units.

Source data

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Methods, Supplementary Results, and Supplementary Tables 1–13

Reporting Summary

Source data

Source Data Fig. 1

Statistical Source Data

Source Data Fig. 2

Statistical Source Data

Source Data Fig. 3

Statistical Source Data

Source Data Fig. 4

Statistical Source Data

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 1

Statistical Source Data

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 2

Statistical Source Data

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 4

Statistical Source Data

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 6

Statistical Source Data

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 7

Statistical Source Data

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Perszyk, E.E., Davis, X.S., Djordjevic, J. et al. Odour-imagery ability is linked to food craving, intake, and adiposity change in humans. Nat Metab 5, 1483–1493 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s42255-023-00874-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s42255-023-00874-z

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing