INTRODUCTION

Stress is believed to contribute to the variability of the aging process and to the development of age-related neuro- and psychopathologies (Heim and Nemeroff 1999; McEwen, 2002; Miller and O'Callaghan, 2005). Clinical data suggest that a stressful lifestyle can be a risk factor for Alzheimer's disease (AD) (Wilson et al, 2005) and stress-related psychiatric disorders (ie, major depression) have been identified as a risk for developing AD (Ownby et al, 2006). There is much interest, therefore, in understanding the mechanisms responsible for interactions among stress, aging and memory.

The effects of stress are thought to be mediated by the activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, culminating in increased levels of glucocorticoids (Berton and Nestler, 2006). Aged rats and elderly humans show progressive loss of control of the HPA axis, resulting in hypersecretion of glucocorticoids (Sapolsky et al, 1983; Born et al, 1995). Abnormal glucocorticoid secretion is thought to be involved in many aging-related diseases, including depression (Kendler et al, 1999), memory impairments (Sapolsky et al, 1986; Lupien et al, 1998; Aisa et al, 2007) and AD (Hartmann et al, 1997; Elgh et al, 2006). In experimental models of AD, stress or glucocorticoids exacerbated AD-like neuropathology (Green et al, 2006; Jeong et al, 2006). Glucocorticoids not only have been suggested to contribute to age-related loss of neurons in the hippocampus of rats (Sapolsky, 1985), but also have been shown to potentiate hippocampal damage induced by various noxious insults (Sapolsky and Pulsinelli, 1985) including the amyloid β (Aβ) peptide, the main component of senile plaques in AD (Catania et al, 2009). Thus, a recently developed view suggests that hypercortisolemia, leading to hippocampal atrophy and further HPA axis disinhibition (ie, ‘the glucocorticoid cascade hypothesis’), would initiate a chain of events, ultimately culminating in the development of lesions typical of AD (Dhikav and Anand, 2007). The mechanisms underlying the effects of altered glucocorticoid levels remain largely unknown. However, among them, it is worth mentioning the inter-relation between glucocorticoids and insulin. Glucocorticoid excess results in insulin resistance by blunting insulin's action to suppress hepatic glucose production and stimulate peripheral glucose utilization (Amatruda et al, 1985). In addition to their effects on insulin sensitivity, glucocorticoids inhibit insulin secretion from pancreatic β-cells (Delaunay et al, 1997; Lambillotte et al, 1997). Glucocorticoids are elevated in rodents with experimental diabetes (Magariños and McEwen, 2000) and it has been suggested that cognitive impairment in diabetes may result from glucocorticoid-mediated deficits in neurogenesis and synaptic plasticity (Stranahan et al, 2008).

Maternal separation (MS) is an animal paradigm designed to mimic repeated exposure to stress during early life, resulting in animals with behavioral and neuroendocrine signs of elevated stress reactivity as adults (Lehmann and Feldon, 2000; Heim and Nemeroff, 2001; Aisa et al, 2007). The peak period of neurogenesis overlaps the stress hyporesponsive period (postnatal days, PND 4–14) in neonatal rats (Sapolsky and Meaney, 1986). Therefore, early stress, such as MS, could be interfering with the normal maturation of the hippocampal structure, which might ultimately lead to an increased vulnerability for psychiatric diseases. After the early developmental period, it is at present unknown how stress will have an effect on the aging process because of a lack of studies.

In this work we have studied the effects of an early life stress in aged rats. The main results found are that the interaction between stress and aging induced: (1) altered HPA axis reactivity, depressive-like behavior, cognitive impairments, intracellular insulin pathways alterations and plastic changes; and (2) promoted amyloid precursor protein (APP) processing towards the amyloidogenic pathway. These results provide a tentative mechanism through which aging interplay with stress to influence cognition, with a bias towards consideration of insulin-mediated mechanisms and neuroplastic events in the hippocampus, as the basis of AD.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

All the experiments were carried out in strict compliance with the recommendations of the EU (DOCE L 358/1 18/2/1986) for the care and use of laboratory animals. Timed-pregnant Wistar rats were provided on gestation day 16 from Charles River Laboratories (Portage, MI, USA), individually housed in a temperature (21±1°C) and humidity (55±5%) controlled room on a 12-h light–dark cycle (lights on at 0800 h) with food and water freely available. Every effort was made to minimize the number of animals used and their suffering.

MS Procedure

On PND 2 (Aisa et al, 2007; Ladd et al, 2004), all pups were sexed and randomly assigned to the control group (pups manipulated to change the bedding in their cages once weekly), or the separation group (MS, pups separated daily from their dam as a litter for 3 h from PND 2–21 inclusive). Rats were weaned on PND 23 and only males were chosen for the present work. Subsequent experiments were carried out in young (60–75 days) and aged rats (18 months). Rats were not allowed to grow older as mortality increased significantly in the aged MS group. Animals from different litters were evenly spread over the different experimental groups. Animals were monthly weighted.

Behavioral Test

Behavioral experiments were conducted between 0900 h and 1300 h. Observers were blind to the rearing condition. A different animal cohort was used for carrying out each of the different test.

Locomotor Activity

Locomotor activity was measured for 30 min in an open field (65 × 65 cm2, 45 cm height) made of black wood, using a video-tracking system (Ethovision 3.0, Noldus Information Technology B.V., The Netherlands), in a softly illuminated room. Total path length (cm) was analyzed.

Forced Swimming Test

The test was carried out as described (Porsolt et al, 1977). Briefly, two swimming sessions were conducted: an initial 15-min pre-test followed 24 h later by a 5-min test. Rats were placed individually in a vertical Plexiglas cylinder (height: 60 cm, diameter: 30 cm) filled with 45 cm of 23±1°C water. Immobility was considered as rats floating passively, making only small movements to keep its nose above the surface.

Object Recognition Test (NORT)

As previously described (Aisa et al, 2007), the open field consisted of a square open field (65 cm × 65 cm × 45 cm) made of black wood. On the previous day to the experiment, animals were familiarized with the square for 30 min. During the first trial, two identical objects were placed within the chamber, and the rat was allowed to freely explore during 5 min. Exploration was considered when the head of the rat was oriented toward the object with its nose within 2 cm of the object. An hour later a second trial took place, in which one object was replaced by another, and exploration was scored for 5 min. Results were expressed as percentage of time spent with the new object with respect to the total exploration time (discrimination index).

Morris Water Maze

This memory task was carried out as previously described (Diez-Ariza et al, 2003). The maze consisted of a black circular tank (140 cm diameter × 55 cm high) and filled with water (20–22°C). A black invisible platform, 10 cm diameter, was positioned 1 cm below the water surface. A video camera was set above the center of the pool and connected to a video traction system (EthoVision; Noldus Information Technology B.V.). The pool was surrounded by white curtains marked with black geometric paintings, so as to enable the animals to learn the platform location. On the first day, each rat becomes habituated to the training environment for 60 s. In the acquisition phase (days 2–3), rats performed 6 training trials per day (120 s each) with the escape platform in a fixed position. Time spent to reach the platform (latency), the swim path length (distance swam), and the swim speed (cm/s) were recorded. The results on time to reach the platform are not shown as, in all cases, parallel with distances swam. On day 4, (retention phase) a single transfer test was carried out, in which the platform was removed from the tank. The animal was allowed to swim for 60 s in search of the platform. The distance swam in the quadrant where the platform was previously located was measured.

Biochemical Measurements

Tissue and blood collection

Fasting rats were killed by decapitation between 0800–1000 h. Brains were removed and dissected on ice to obtain the hippocampus or frozen immediately and stored at −80 °C until sectioning. Trunk blood was collected into EDTA tubes, centrifuged at 1250 g (15 min, 4°C), and plasma was frozen.

Plasma corticosterone, insulin and IGF1 determinations

Plasma corticosterone (50 μl) was determined using a commercially available solid-phase 125I radioimmunoassay kit (Coat-a-Count, Siemens, USA). Insulin was measured in 10 μl plasma samples using the Sensitive Rat Insulin Enzyme Immunoassay Kit (EZRMI-13K, Millipore, Billerica, MA, USA). IGF1 levels were determined using a commercially available solid-phase 125I radioimmunoassay kit (DSL-2900, Diagnostic System Laboratories, TX, USA).

Western blotting

Assays were carried out as described in Table 1. Immunopositive bands were visualized using an enhanced chemiluminescense western blotting-detection reagent (ECL; Amersham, Buckinghamsire, England). The optical density (O.D.) of reactive bands visible on X-ray film was determined densitometrically. β-actin or α-tubulin was used as internal control. Results were expressed as percentage of O.D. values of control young rats.

Table 1 Conditions used in western blotting experiments

Aβ levels

Aβ1–42 levels were determined using a commercially available high-sensitive ELISA kit (Wako Pure Chemical Industries, Tokyo, Japan) following manufacturer instructions.

In situ hybridization for BDNF and activity-regulated cytoskeletal-associated protein (ARC) mRNA

Coronal brain sections containing the hippocampus (15 μm thick) were cut at the level of −3.3 mm (relative to the bregma) according to the atlas of Paxinos and Watson (1982). The oligonucleotides used were: 5′-AGTTCCAGTGCCTTTTGTCTATGCCCCTGCAGCCTTTGGTGTAAC-3′ for BDNF and 5′-CTTGGTTGCCCATCCTCACCTGGCACCCAAGACTGGTATTGCTGA-3′ for ARC, both from Sigma Genosis, UK. Probes were 3′-tail labeled with αS[35S]dATP, specific activity >1000 Ci/mmol (GE Healthcare Biosciences, Europe). Negative controls including sense oligonucleotides showed minimal background signals. Sections were exposed to Biomax MR film (Kodak) for 3 days (BDNF) or 3 weeks (ARC). The relative abundance mRNA in each region was determined by densitometric quantification of autoradiograms using an image analysis system (Scion Image, Scion Corporation, USA) correcting for non-specific signals. O.D. values were calibrated to 35S tissue equivalents using 14C microascales (Amersham, UK) and the appropriate conversion factor. Densitometric values from three sections of each animal were averaged and expressed as nCi/g tissue.

Data analysis

Data were analyzed by SPSS for Windows, release 15.0 and normality was checked by Shapiro–Wilks's test (p<0.05). In the acquisition phase of the Morris water maze, over-all treatment effects were examined by two-way repeated measures ANOVA (rearing × trial). Differences between trials within groups were analyzed using a factorial ANOVA with replicates. Data in the retention phase were analyzed with one-way ANOVA. Neurochemical data was analyzed by two-way analysis (rearing × age) of variance ANOVA. Post hoc comparisons were conducted, if appropriate, using Tukey's protected least significance test. Data are presented as mean±SEM. The level of significance for all analyses testing was set at p<0.05.

RESULTS

Overall Effects of Stress and Age on Weight Gain, Locomotor Activity, and Depression-Like Behavior

As shown in Table 2, there was a main effect of age in weight gain (two-way ANOVA, F1,60=180.673, p<0.001; n=10–22) and locomotor activity (F1,82=330.239, p<0.001; n=10–17) (Table 2).

Table 2 Effects of age and maternal separation (MS) on weight, locomotor activity, depressive-like behaviour, markers of HPA axis activity, and insulin pathways

In the forced swimming test, there was an increased immobility time, indicative of depressive-like behavior, in both young and aged MS rats (two-way ANOVA, main effect of rearing, F1,82=17.217, p<0.001; n=10–17) (Table 2).

Effects of Age and MS on Cognition

In the acquisition phase of Morris water maze, distance swam to reach the platform improved significantly over trials in all groups (factorial ANOVA with replicates) and no significant effect of rearing or age was found. However, in the retention phase, both rearing (F1,55=7.199, p<0.05; n=8–21) and age (F1,55=14.220, p<0.001; n=8–21) produced a statistically significant decrease in distance swam in the quadrant were the platform used to be located, indicative of a memory impairment (Figure 1a). Swim speed was not affected by rearing or age.

Figure 1
figure 1

Effects of age and maternal separation (MS) on cognition. (a) Retention phase of the Morris water maze, data are presented as distance swam in the quadrant where platform used to be located. There was a main effect of rearing (F1,55=7.199, p<0.05) and age (F1,55=14.220, p<0.001). (b) New object recognition test, data presented as discrimination index: time exploring the new object/total exploration time × 100. There was a main effect of rearing (F1,58=8.964, p<0.01). 3 m: 3 months, young rats; and 18 m: 18 months, aged rats *p<0.001 main effect of rearing, p<0.05 or better, main effect of age.

PowerPoint slide

A two-way ANOVA indicated a main effect of rearing in the NORT, and MS rats showed a learning impairment (F1,58=8.964, p<0.01; n=12–18) as the discrimination index was significantly lower (Figure 1b).

Age and MS Interact to Affect HPA Axis

There was a significant rearing × age interaction in plasma corticosterone levels (two-way ANOVA, F1,50=4.689, p<0.05; n=8–19). Further analysis showed a significant increase in corticosterone levels in young MS rats (Student's t-test; p<0.05) and almost significant effect in aged MS rats (Student's t-test; p=0.06) compared with young controls (Table 2).

Hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor density mediates the glucocorticoid negative feedback to modulate HPA axis activity. Statistical analysis indicates a significant interaction between rearing and age (F1,34=11.671, p<0.01; n=8–10). Post-hoc analysis showed that, compared with young controls, there was a significant decrease in glucocorticoid receptor levels in young MS, aged controls and aged MS (Student's t-test; p<0.001, in all cases). Glucocorticoid receptors were also decreased when comparing young and aged MS rats (Student's t-test; p<0.05) (Table 2).

Age and MS Interact to Affect Insulin Levels, IDE and Intracellular Insulin Pathways

There was an interaction (F1,39=3.776, p<0.05; n=10) between rearing and age in fasting plasma insulin levels. Further analysis showed that there was a significant decrease in insulin levels in MS young, aged and MS aged rats (Student's t-test; p<0.05 or p<0.01). Levels of insulin-like growth factor 1 (IGF1) were only affected by age (F1,39=22.346, p<0.001; n=10) (Table 2).

There was a significant interaction (F1,37=8.127, p<0.01; n=10–11) between rearing and age in hippocampal IDE levels. Further analysis revealed that both the MS procedure (control vs MS rats, Student's t-test; p<0.05) or age (control vs aged rats, Student's t-test; p<0.001) produced significant decreases in IDE levels (Table 2).

There was a main effect of age in levels of insulin receptors (IRs) (F1,35=23.420, p<0.001; n=8–10) and IR phosphorylation (F1,17=103.992, p<0.001; n=4–5) (Table 2). When studying the insulin pathways, it was found that there was a main effect of age in levels of phosphorylated Akt (pAkt) (Ser473) normalized to total Akt (F1,41=9.606, p<0.01; n=8–12) (Figure 2a), pGSK3β (Ser9) normalized to total GSK3β (F1,32=33.318, p<0.001; n=6–10) (Figure 2b), pTau (Ser202/Thr205) normalized to total Tau (F1,35=25.622, p<0.001; n=8–10) (Figure 2c) and pERK1 (Thr202/Tyr204) levels normalized to total ERK1 (F1,33=20.429, p<0.001; n=6–10) (Figure 2d). Consistent with a post-transcriptional regulation of these enzymes, total Akt, GSK3β, Tau, and ERK1 protein levels, normalized using actin, remained unaltered. It is interesting to note that there was an interaction (rearing × age) (F1,35=6.054, p<0.05, n=8–10) in pERK2 (Thr202/Tyr204) levels normalized to total ERK2. Further analysis showed a significant decrease in pERK2 in aged MS compared with young MS or aged rats (Student's t-test; p<0.05 in both cases) (Figure 2d). Total ERK2 levels normalized using actin did not show significant changes.

Figure 2
figure 2

Age and maternal separation (MS) interact to affect insulin levels and intracellular insulin pathways. (a) Phosphorylated Akt levels (active form of the enzyme, 60 kDa) normalized to total Akt, main effect of age (F1,41=9.606, p<0.01; n=8–12); (b) phosphorylated (inactive) GSK3β levels (46 kDa) normalized to total GSK3β, main effect of age (F1,32=33.318, p<0.001; n=6–10); (c) Phosphorylated Tau levels (40–50 kDa) normalized to total Tau, main effect of age (F1,35=25.622, p<0.001; n=8–10); (d) Phosphorylated (active) ERK1 (44 kDa) normalized to total ERK1, main effect of age (F1,33=20.429, p<0.001; n=6–10) and phosphorylated (active) ERK2 levels (42 kDa) normalized to total ERK2, interaction (rearing × age) (F1,35=6.054, p<0.05). Figure shows percentage of optical density (O.D.) values of control young rats and representative picture of the blotting. No differences were found in the non-phosphorylated (total) levels of the enzymes. 3 m: 3 months, young rats; and 18 m: 18 months, aged rats. *p<0.01 or better, main effect of age. p<0.05 or better vs control young rats, #p<0.05 vs MS young rats.

PowerPoint slide

Neonatal Stress Favors the Amyloidogenic Processing of APP in Aged Rats

As depicted in Figure 3a, APP protein levels were not altered by rearing condition or age (two-way ANOVA, F1,31=2.030, p=0.165; n=8). When analyzing the amyloidogenic route, significant increases in the C99 : C83 ratio were found associated to rearing (F1,26=4.164, p<0.05; n=7–8) or age (F1,26=5.158, p<0.05; n=7–8) (Figure 3a). Aβ levels were also significantly increased by age (F1,22=11.116, p<0.01; n=6) and rearing (F1,22=5.723, p<0.05; n=6) (Figure 3b). Regarding BACE1 levels, there was a main effect of rearing (F1,22=7.390, p<0.05; n=6) and age (F1,22=10.380, p<0.01; n=6) (Figure 3c).

Figure 3
figure 3

Neonatal stress favors the amyloidogenic processing of amyloid precursor protein (APP) in aged rats. (a) APP protein (105 kDa) levels (no effects of age or rearing) and C99 : C83 (10 kDa) ratio, main effect of rearing (F1,26=4.164, p<0.05) and age (F1,26=5.158, p<0.05). (b) Aβ levels (pmol/mg) ( × 108), main effect of age (F1,22=11.116, p<0.01; n=6), main effect of rearing (F1,22=5.723, p<0.05; n=6). (c) BACE1 (70 kDa) levels, main effect of rearing (F1,22=7.390, p<0.05) and age (F1,22=10.380, p<0.01). a and c show percentage of optical density (O.D.) values of control young rats and representative picture of the blotting. 3 m: 3 months, young rats; and 18 m: 18 months, aged rats; MS: maternal separation rats. *p<0.05 main effect of rearing, p<0.05 or better, main effect of age.

PowerPoint slide

Effect of Age and MS on Synaptic Plasticity

Significant decreases in BDNF mRNA density were found in the dentate gyrus (DG) (F1,15=14.086, p<0.01; n=4–5) and CA3 regions (F1,15=23.079, p<0.01; n=4–5) of the hippocampus of MS rats compared with control rats. In the CA1 region there was an interaction rearing × age (F1,14=7.141, p<0.05). Post-hoc analysis showed that MS produced a significant decrease of BDNF expression in young rats (Student's t-test; p<0.01; n=4) and almost significant effect in aged rats (Student's t-test; p=0.08; n=4) (Figure 4a).

Figure 4
figure 4

Effects of age and maternal separation (MS) on synaptic plasticity. Figure shows percentage of optical density (O.D.) values of control young rats and representative pictures of autoradiograms. (a) BDNF mRNA expression. (b) ARC mRNA expression. 3 m: 3 months, young rats; and 18 m: 18 months, aged rats. *p<0.01 or better, main effect of rearing, two-way ANOVA (rearing × age), p<0.01 or better, main effect of age, two-way ANOVA (rearing × age), #p<0.05 or better vs control young (3 months) rats, p<0.001 vs MS young (3 months) rats or control aged (18 months) rats.

PowerPoint slide

Regarding ARC mRNA levels, there was a main affect of age and rearing in the DG (F1,15=121.767, p<0.001; n=4–5; and F1,15=386.446, p<0.001; n=4–5, respectively) and CA3 region of the hippocampus (F1,15=89.481, p<0.001; n=4–5; and F1,15=212.861, p<0.001; n=4–5, respectively). In the CA1 region, there was an interaction (rearing × age) (F1,15=10.448, p<0.01; n=4–5). Further analysis showed significant decreases in ARC mRNA expression in MS young (Student's t-test, p<0.001; n=4), aged (Student's t-test, p<0.001; n=4–5), and MS-aged rats (Student's t-test; p<0.001; n=4–5) compared to control young rats, and further decreases in MS aged rats compared with MS young rats (Student's t-test; p<0.001; n=4).

DISCUSSION

The MS model in rat is considered nowadays as a robust model of enhanced stress responsiveness (Ladd et al, 2000; Lehmann and Feldon, 2000; Oitzl et al, 2000; Aisa et al, 2007). Even though several studies have focus in the characterization of the MS model in adult animals, little work has been done on how neonatal stress would impact the aging process. In our hands, not only the behavioral (depression-like) effects of MS persisted until senescence but also our findings suggest that neonatal stress may have accelerated cognitive decline, at least in the Morris water maze, a hippocampal-dependant task. The results found on the NORT, together with the fact that the hippocampus is only minimally involved in memory for objects (Brown and Aggleton, 2001), would support the notion that the interaction age-stress in memory deficits mainly has its anatomical substrate on the hippocampus. It is worth mentioning that even though aged rats seem to have a decreased activity in the open field test, other measurements of locomotor activity, such as swim speed in the Morris water maze or total exploration time in the first exposure to objects in the NORT, did not differ among groups.

Age and MS Interact to Affect Insulin Pathways

In accordance to our data, it has been described that alterations in behavior and cognition associated with MS stress are related to increased levels of glucocorticoids (Aisa et al, 2007; Wigger and Neumann, 1999). Aging ‘per se’ is also accompanied by a progressive increase in basal corticosterone levels in rats (Sapolsky et al, 1983; Sapolsky et al, 1986; Hauger et al, 1994). As described in the Introduction section, the effect of glucocortiocoids in vivo appears to include a decrease in circulating insulin levels. Even though the reduced peripheral levels of insulin found in this study do not necessarily imply equally reduced insulin levels in the brain (ie, the low levels of IDE in the hippocampus induced by either stress or ageing could lead to locally slower insulin degradation), the reduced IR expression and phosphorylation status suggest that effects dependent on intracellular insulin pathways activity could be inhibited in MS aged rats. In addition, it could be excluded that IGF1, known to be closely related to insulin and share similar cellular signaling events (Dupont and LeRoith, 2001) could compensate for insulin decreased levels.

The insulin signal transduction cascade of the PI3K/Akt/GSK3β pathway promotes neuronal survival by directly inactivating the proapoptotic machinery. It is interesting to note that it is this same route that is required for the induction of long-term potentiation and depression, basic processes underlying learning and memory (Van der Heide et al, 2006). In this regard, clinical and epidemiological studies have found that diabetes and hyperinsulinemia may be suspected factors to increase the risk of developing AD (Steen et al, 2005), and it seems that a dysfunction of the IR facilitates tau hyperphosphorylation (Schubert et al, 2004), which forms neurofibrillary tangles, through its regulation of GSK3β. However, even though according to the reduced input to the IR in aged rats we found a decreased phosphorylation of GSK3β, leading to increased activation of the enzyme and subsequent increased phosphorylation of tau, no additive effects of stress were seen.

Insulin binding to IR also activates another signal transduction cascade identified as the MAPK/ERK pathway. ERK1/2 has essential roles in neuronal survival (Xia et al, 1995) and synaptic plasticity related to learning and memory formation (Davis and Laroche, 2006). Furthermore, early work demonstrated an absolute requirement for ERK activity in induction of LTP (English and Sweatt, 1996) and memory consolidation (Eckel-Mahan et al, 2008). Supported by a recent study showing that the microdeletions of the ERK2 gene leads to cognitive deficits (Shaikh et al, 2007), our findings suggest that the ERK2 pathway could be involved in the cognitive deficits observed in aged MS rats, therefore implicating this insulin signaling in the stress-associated memory deficits.

Age and MS Interact to Favor an Increase in Levels of Aβ

The principal constituent of amyloid plaques observed in AD, is the amyloidβ-protein (Aβ), which is generated from APP. APP can be cleaved by β-secretase (BACE1) and α-secretase to produce C99 and C83. These C-terminal fragments can then be cleaved by γ-secretase to produce Aβ and p3, respectively. It has been shown that chronic stress or elevated glucocorticoid levels potentiate Aβ deposition and induces cognitive deficits in transgenic mouse models of AD (Green et al, 2006; Jeong et al, 2006). Glucocorticoids are also able to potentiate hippocampal damage induced by various noxious insults, including the toxic actions of Aβ (Catania et al, 2009). It has been described that treatment with soluble human Aβ not only disrupted the activation of ERK and Akt, but also that Aβ was able to bind to IR, interfering with insulin-induced autophosphorylation (Townsend et al, 2007) or inducing the loss of insulin receptor at the neuronal surface (Zhao et al, 2008; De Felice et al, 2009). C99 has intrinsic neurotoxic properties (Yankner et al, 1989), causes synaptic degeneration (Oster-Granite et al, 1996) and impairs long-term potentiation (Nalbantoglu et al, 1997) and cognition (Berger-Sweeney et al, 1999), all these effects most probably are due to an enhanced Aβ generation. In our hands, aging and stress interacts to favor the amyloidogenic route of APP processing, and increased Aβ formation, probably by upregulating BACE1 levels. A glucocorticoid response element in the promoter region of the BACE1 gene has been described (Sambamurti et al, 2004), making it likely that glucocorticoids mediates the regulatory actions of stress on BACE1 expression by directly increasing transcription of this gene.

On the other hand, the expression of IDE, a metalloprotease that not only catabolises insulin but also has a crucial part in Aβ clearance in the brain (Kurochkin and Goto, 1994; Qiu et al, 1998), which could be modulated by glucocorticoids (Kulstad et al, 2005). Therefore, in aged MS rats, increases in Aβ due to the increased amyloidogenic route and decreased metabolism by IDE, would interfere with IR function in hippocampal neurons, preventing the activation of specific kinases (ERK2) required for long term potentiation.

Synaptic Plasticity

The MS procedure, which took place during critical periods of hippocampal development, can disrupt hippocampal cytoarchitecture in a stable manner (Huot et al, 2002), which may also contribute to the learning deficits observed in adult/aged animals. It is known that different types of stress, including MS (Roceri et al, 2002; Duman and Monteggia, 2006; Warner-Schmidt and Duman, 2006), or increases in Aβ levels (Christensen et al, 2008) induce reduced BDNF expression. As BDNF signaling activates the ERK1/2 pathway, it could be suggested that the inhibition of ERK cascade in MS aged rats could be related to both a lower BDNF levels and reduced insulin pathways activation. In addition, ARC expression, which is strongly implicated in synaptic plasticity and consolidation of long-term memories (Guzowski et al, 2000), may be up-regulated by BDNF through ERK (Zheng et al, 2009).

CONCLUSION

Altogether, a tentative mechanism through which stress exert their effects on aging/AD could be suggested based on the present results (Figure 5). Our findings demonstrate that a stress history in the neonatal period exacerbates the age-related cognitive decline and therefore, the extent of neonatal exposure to glucocorticoids may determine the extent of age-related cognitive dysfunction (Montaron et al, 2006). It could also be suggested that high levels of glucocorticoids, found in AD (Hartmann et al, 1997; Csernansky et al, 2006; Elgh et al, 2006), might not be merely a consequence of the disease process, but it rather has a central role in the development and progression of the illness. Interactions between endogenous (age, genetics, C99 or Aβ production) and exogenous factors (stress) might be important determinants of the onset and progress of AD. The present findings suggest that stress management or pharmacological interventions on the glucocorticoids/insulin pathway warrant additional consideration in the regimen of AD therapies.

Figure 5
figure 5

Mechanism through which stress and/or glucocorticoids exerts their effects on aging/Alzheimer's disease (AD). Glucocorticoids by inhibiting the insulin pathway, would lead to a decrease in pERK2 levels, as well as affecting synaptic plasticity. Therefore neuronal ability would be compromised to survive insults, such as increases in glucocorticoid-induced C99 or Aβ production.

PowerPoint slide