Classic psychedelics, such as psilocybin and lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), are non-selective 5-HT2A receptor agonists with therapeutic potential for treating psychiatric disorders and mental health concerns (for a review, see1). Classic psychedelics show a fairly strong safety profile, including minimal adverse effects, toxicity, and potential for abuse2,3,4,5. A primary concern associated with classic psychedelics relates to their alteration of consciousness3, which can range from highly positive ‘peak’ experiences6,7 to psychologically challenging experiences (often referred to as “bad trips”8,9), such as grief, paranoia, and fear10.

Challenging experiences following use/administration of classic psychedelics have been reported in both controlled (e.g., clinical trials) and uncontrolled (e.g., ritual or recreational use) studies (e.g.,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19). For instance, in a clinical trial in which individuals with major depressive disorder received two doses of psilocybin alongside psychotherapy, 65% of individuals described one of their psilocybin experiences as one of the five most psychologically challenging experiences of their life and 25% of individuals described it as the single most psychologically challenging experience of their life13. Furthermore, across their two psilocybin experiences in this clinical trial13, 92% of individuals reported that they felt like crying (although note that catharsis-related responses such as crying are more likely to be regarded as therapeutically useful, e.g., see20), 79% of individuals reported experiencing sadness, 56% reported experiencing anxiousness, and 77% reported experiencing emotional or physical suffering. Among healthy individuals that were administered psilocybin, 31% of individuals reported experiencing strong or extreme fear and 22% reported that a significant portion or their entire psilocybin experience was characterized by anxiety or unpleasant psychological struggle15. Within a nationally representative sample, 40.9% of individuals with lifetime psychedelic use reported having a challenging psychedelic experience19. Cross-sectional surveys of psychedelic-induced challenging experiences12 and so-called “God encounter experiences”21 have also found that for some individuals (11% and 15%, respectively) these were the single most psychologically challenging experience of their life.

Although challenging experiences are sometimes described as ultimately beneficial or therapeutic9,10,12,17,20,22, these experiences can sometimes contribute to post-acute distress, functional impairment, and medical attention seeking (e.g.,11,18,23,24,25,26,27). For instance, among individuals with lifetime use of a classic psychedelic, 8.9% of individuals reported experiencing functional impairment for longer than one day, and 2.6% of individuals reported seeking medical or psychological assistance, following a challenging psychedelic experience19. There are also reports of the emergence of psychiatric diagnoses, suicidality, and harm to self and others during and after challenging psychedelic experiences4,11,12. Importantly, concerns about challenging experiences and their effects are commonly noted as a reason that health care providers28,29,30 and users31,32,33 are reluctant to suggest or receive treatment with classic psychedelics.

Several factors likely contribute to the intensity of challenging psychedelic experiences, including trait level neuroticism, preparedness for the psychedelic experience, and the setting in which the psychedelic is used34,35,36,37. Co-use of other pharmacological agents may also intensify or buffer against challenging experiences. For instance, relative to use of classic psychedelics alone, co-use of classic psychedelics with lithium and other mood stabilizers was associated with greater intensity of challenging experiences19 (in addition to medical complications, such as seizures38,39). Another study found a quadratic relationship between co-use of cannabis and classic psychedelics, such that (relative to use of a classic psychedelic alone) co-use of low dose cannabis was associated with less intense challenging experiences and co-use of large dose cannabis was associated with more intense challenging experiences40.

Co-use of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) with psilocybin (referred to as “hippy flipping”) and LSD (referred to as “candy flipping”) is one method that is reportedly used to reduce challenging experiences and enhance positive experiences41,42. MDMA, a potent serotonergic entactogen/empathogen, induces the release of serotonin, norepinephrine, dopamine, vasopressin, and oxytocin; dampens amygdala blood flow43; decreases feelings of fear and sadness44,45; and may increase positive feelings43, including love46,47, compassion48,49, and self-compassion50,51.

Several studies have reported on co-use of LSD/psilocybin and MDMA, with prevalence rates ranging from 8 to 52% among recreational drug users with lifetime LSD/psilocybin use41,42,52,53,54. Among polydrug users in the United Kingdom, participants reported co-using LSD and MDMA to improve the effect of LSD and to ease its aftereffects41. As one anecdotal report noted, “…when taken in conjunction…[MDMA] acts as a safety buffer and allows you to go a lot further than you normally would”55. Importantly, to date, only a single study has examined the effects of co-using MDMA alongside LSD in humans (with no studies on co-use alongside psilocybin). This was a recent double-blind placebo-controlled study56 that did not observe significant differences in acute experiences between LSD (100 µg) plus placebo relative to LSD (100 µg) plus MDMA (100 mg). Importantly, this study was conducted in a controlled setting and excluded individuals with a personal or family history of psychiatric disorders, which reduce the potential for LSD-related challenging experiences10,12,22 and thereby may have resulted in floor effects. Furthermore, the study examined only a single (medium–high) dose of MDMA and did not measure certain positive experiences (e.g., self-compassion, compassion, love, and gratitude) that may be impacted by MDMA. Finally, the study sample was small (N = 24), which increases the likelihood of Type II errors. Therefore, in an observational study, we further examined whether (relative psilocybin/LSD use alone) co-use of psilocybin/LSD and MDMA was associated with lower acute challenging experiences and increased acute positive experiences.

Results

Demographics and identification of covariates

The final sample included 698 individuals. For participant demographics, see Table 1. 342 individuals reported using LSD and 356 individuals reported using psilocybin during their experience. 27 individuals co-used psilocybin/LSD and MDMA (psilocybin + MDMA = 14; LSD + MDMA = 13). For further details regarding LSD/psilocybin dosage, see Fig. 2. For means and standard deviations for dependent variable and Kruskal Wallis tests (and post hoc Dunn’s tests), see Table 2.

Table 1 Demographics.
Table 2 Descriptive data and comparison of dependent variables by MDMA use.

MDMA use (none, low dose, and medium–high dose) was significantly associated with: (a) conscientiousness (F = 3.20, p = 0.041; individuals who co-used low dose MDMA were significantly lower than those who did not co-use MDMA); (b) openness (F = 3.23, p = 0.040; individuals who co-used medium–high dose MDMA were significantly lower than those who did not co-use MDMA); and psilocybin/LSD use in the following contexts (c) recreational/social (χ2 = 18.80, p < 0.001; more common among co-users of low and medium–high dose MDMA); (d) live singing (χ2 = 9.81, p = 0.007), (e) emotional support (χ2 = 9.38, p = 0.009), and (f) strangers (χ2 = 15.88, p < 0.001; higher among co-users of low dose MDMA relative to those who did not co-use MDMA). Correlation coefficients and VIFs were all below cutoffs (i.e., all r < 0.4 and all VIF < 5), indicating that multicollinearity was not of significant concern. These variables were therefore included in the primary analyses (see below) examining the relationship between co-MDMA use with psilocybin/LSD and acute challenging and positive experiences. See Supplementary Material (Supplementary Table 1) for a full list of analyses examining potential confounds.

Primary analyses

Challenging experiences

Co-use of MDMA with psilocybin/LSD was associated with significant differences in total challenging experience (F(2,672) = 3.62, p = 0.031). Relative to psilocybin/LSD use alone, psilocybin/LSD + low dose MDMA was associated with significantly lower levels of total challenging experience, t(672) = 2.54, p = 0.011. There was no significant difference between psilocybin/LSD use alone and psilocybin/LSD + medium–high dose MDMA use, t(672) =  − 0.68, p = 0.498.

Examining group differences on CEQ subscales, co-use of MDMA with psilocybin/LSD was associated with significant differences in experiences of grief (F[2,672] = 4.64, p = 0.012) and fear (F[2,672] = 33.80, p = 0.023), but not physical distress (F[2,672] = 0.43, p = 0.654), insanity (F[2,672] = 1.30, p = 0.273), isolation (F[2,672] = 1.25, p = 0.287), death (F[2,672] = 2.42, p = 0.090), or paranoia (F[2,672] = 1.64, p = 0.196). Relative to psilocybin/LSD use alone, psilocybin/LSD + low dose MDMA was associated with significantly lower levels of grief and fear (t[672] = 2.83, p = 0.005; t[672] = 2.21, p = 0.027, respectively). There was no significant difference between psilocybin/LSD use alone and psilocybin/LSD + medium–high dose MDMA use for grief and fear (t[672] = 1.02, p = 0.310; t[672] =  − 1.61, p = 0.108, respectively).

Positive experiences

Co-use of MDMA with psilocybin/LSD was associated with higher levels of self-compassion, feelings of love, and experiences of gratitude (F(2,256) = 3.62, p = 0.028; F(2,256) = 3.97, p = 0.020; F(2,256) = 3.92, p = 0.021, respectively). Relative to psilocybin/LSD use alone, psilocybin/LSD + low dose MDMA was associated with greater feelings of self-compassion, love, and gratitude (t(256) =  − 2.61, p = 0.010; t(256) =  − 2.69, p = 0.008; t(256) =  − 2.12, p = 0.035, respectively), while psilocybin/LSD + medium–high dose MDMA was not (t(256) = 0.59, p = 0.557; t(256) = 0.79, p = 0.431; t(256) = 1.78, p = 0.076, respectively). Co-use of MDMA with psilocybin/LSD was not associated with significant differences in compassion (F(2,256) = 2.67, p = 0.071) or mystical-type experience (total score F[2,577] = 0.65, p = 0.524; mystical F[2,556] = 0.49, p = 0.613; positive mood F[2,577] = 2.13, p = 0.120; transcendence F[2,577] = 0.35, p = 0.703; and ineffability F[2,577] = 0.87, p = 0.421).

Discussion

Psilocybin/LSD experiences can range from being profoundly positive to overwhelmingly challenging. Anecdotal reports indicate that individuals sometimes co-use MDMA to buffer against challenging experiences and enhance positive experiences associated with psilocybin/LSD55. To date, only a single study had examined the association between co-use of MDMA and psilocybin/LSD and acute subjective drug effects. Therefore, in a convenience sample, this study examined whether co-use of MDMA with psilocybin/LSD is associated with lower challenging experiences and higher positive experiences.

Controlling for potential confounds, co-use of MDMA (specifically low dose) with psilocybin/LSD was associated with lower levels of total challenging experiences, as well as grief and fear (measured by CEQ Total and CEQ subscales, respectively). These reductions in total challenging experience, fear, and grief are in line with research indicating that MDMA reduces experiences of sadness and fear44,45 and anecdotal reports regarding the effects of “hippy flipping” and “candy flipping”41,55. Although death-related challenging experiences were also significantly lower among individuals that co-used low dose MDMA and psilocybin/LSD, when controlling for potential confounds, co-use of MDMA was not associated with significant differences in death-related or other aspects of challenging experiences (i.e., physical distress, insanity, isolation, and paranoia). These non-significant results may be explained by: (1) co-MDMA use targeting affective/emotional systems over cognitive systems, explaining why emotions like fear and grief were altered, while having limited influence on more cognitively-dependent states like death and paranoia; (2) floor effects and high variability (i.e., ‘fear of death’ was low across groups, and the mean score for the low dose MDMA group was 0; ‘isolation’ and ‘insanity’ have large standard deviations); and/or (3) underpowered sample size for small-to-moderate effects in non-parametric analyses.

Regarding positive experiences, co-use of low dose MDMA (but not medium–high dose MDMA) with psilocybin/LSD was associated with enhanced feelings of self-compassion, love, and gratitude relative to psilocybin/LSD alone. These findings are in line with previously reported motivations for co-using MDMA with psilocybin/LSD41, as well as research indicating that MDMA (alone) may increase acute positive experiences (e.g.,46,47). We did not find significant differences between groups for mystical-type experiences (MEQ-30 total score or subscale scores) and compassion (single-item measure) suggesting that these experiences may be unaffected. However, it is noteworthy that (compared with LSD/psilocybin alone) co-use of low dose MDMA was associated with relatively higher mean scores for compassion and relatively lower mean scores for total mystical-type experience. Interestingly, while several MEQ-30 subscales (i.e., positive mood and ineffability) were descriptively higher in the group that co-used low dose MDMA, other subscales (i.e., mystical and transcendence) were descriptively lower in this group, suggesting a potentially complex relationship between co-use of MDMA and mystical-type experiences. Further research in larger samples is needed to causally elucidate these relationships.

We did not observe any significant differences between co-use of medium–high dose MDMA and the psilocybin/LSD alone groups for acute challenging or positive experiences. This dose-dependent relationship is similar to that previously observed for co-use of cannabis with classic psychedelics40, which found that while co-use of low dose cannabis was associated with lower challenging experiences, co-use of high dose cannabis was associated with greater total challenging experiences, fear, and grief. These null findings are also in line with a recently conducted placebo-controlled study in which (relative to LSD [100 µg] and placebo) co-administration of LSD with a medium–high dose of MDMA (100 mg) was not associated with significant differences in challenging or positive experiences56. While neither found statistically significant effects for co-use of medium–high dose MDMA, we caution against inferring that co-use of medium–high dose MDMA does not impact the acute psilocybin/LSD experience (i.e., the analyses fail to reject the null but do not provide evidence for the null hypothesis; for discussions, see57,58,59), especially given the relatively small sample sizes. Further studies with larger samples will remain necessary. Nonetheless, these findings suggest that the relationship between co-use of MDMA and LSD/psilocybin may be dose dependent and that further research with exact doses of psilocybin/LSD and MDMA are necessary to understand the potentially complex relationship between these substances.

Findings from this study suggest that co-use of low dose MDMA with psilocybin/LSD may buffer against negative or challenging experiences and enhance certain positive experiences. These findings may inform future clinical trial designs and provide early insights into recreational co-use of MDMA with psilocybin/LSD. ​Given the nontrivial presence of challenging experiences within clinical research (e.g.,13) and non-clinical (e.g.,11,12,17,19,21) administration/use of psilocybin/LSD, these findings suggest that co-administration of MDMA may help to mitigate such experiences, as well as post-acute distress, functional impairment, and medical attention seeking that is sometimes reported following challenging psychedelic experiences11,18,23,24,25,26,27).

Provided that pharmacokinetic and larger controlled studies confirm the present preliminary findings and establish the safety and feasibility of co-administering MDMA with psilocybin/LSD, individuals with elevated anxiety about challenging experiences and clinical presentations/profiles (e.g., individuals with elevated neuroticism34, avoidant attachment style60, borderline personality disorder61,62, poor therapeutic alliance63) at a greater risk of challenging experiences may benefit from MDMA co-administration. However, further research will be necessary to examine such speculative hypotheses. MDMA-attributed increases in positive experiences may also be particularly beneficial in specific therapeutic contexts, including couples-based treatment (e.g., see64), positive psychology interventions (which are often gratitude focused; e.g., see65), and group-based treatment/sessions66,67.

Importantly, addressing concerns about challenging experiences through potential co-administration of MDMA, may help to reduce anxiety and increase openness to psychedelic-assisted psychotherapy among health care providers28,29,30 and users31,32,33. Considering the unique mechanisms of action of MDMA and psilocybin/LSD and the growing preliminary support for their efficacy for specific psychiatric diagnoses (posttraumatic stress disorder68 and depression, anxiety, and alcohol use1, respectively), it is also possible co-administration might potentiate the potential efficacy of either compound alone. Contrarily, it remains unclear if challenging experiences are integral to the therapeutic process and mental health improvements–as has been reported by some9,22, leaving open the possibility that co-administration of MDMA may interfere with the therapeutic process.

Limitations and future directions

The present study has considerable limitations including a small sample size, convenience sampling method, and uncontrolled design. The small sample size and potential floor effects may have contributed to null findings and a risk of Type 2 errors (i.e., false negatives). Additionally, given the exploratory nature of the present study and the limited power (due to the sample size and number of covariates included in the models), the present analyses were not corrected for multiple comparisons. Follow-up confirmatory studies are therefore needed to establish confidence in the replicability of the present findings. While the study did not use a controlled design (i.e., precise dosages are unknown, lack of random assignment, self-selected sample etc.), the use of a convenience sample bears some benefit to generalizability, as it is likely more reflective of “hippy-flipping” and “candy-flipping” in Western recreational users. The prospective recruitment and consistency in post-co-use data collection (day after use) are superior to other retrospective studies, which may be more confounded by time and memory-related effects. Additionally, the study examined and controlled for a wide range of potential confounds, including personality factors and the context in which LSD/psilocybin (with or without MDMA) were used. Use of psilocybin vs. LSD was also examined as a potential confound, providing preliminary support for the present effects generalizing across both psilocybin and LSD. Considering the sample largely consisted of psychedelic-experienced users of a particular demographic, further research is needed to determine whether these findings generalize to those who are psychedelic-naive and of other demographic status (e.g., minoritized individuals69,70). Additionally, the majority of the positive experiences (i.e., self-compassion, compassion, gratitude, and love) were measured using single non-validated items, limiting interpretation. Finally, information was not available regarding the exact timing of psilocybin/LSD and MDMA co-use or the MDMA dosage that was considered low, medium, or high (while some research identifies low dose MDMA as 50–75 mg71, other research identifies low dose MDMA as 30–49 mg68), which will be important for designing future controlled studies on co-administration of psilocybin/LSD and MDMA. Future studies are needed to confirm these findings utilizing larger sample sizes, healthy and clinical samples, validated psychometric instruments, and randomized controlled designs. Dose–response designs in which interactions between precise doses of MDMA and psilocybin/LSD (ranging from low to very high dosages) are administered, as well as interactions with individual traits and psychiatric diagnoses, may benefit clinical application and precision-based medicine.

Methods

Design and procedure

​​The present study examined the impact of co-use of MDMA and psilocybin/LSD (relative to psilocybin/LSD alone) on acute challenging and positive experiences. Data was collected as part of two online prospective surveys of individuals with upcoming plans to use a psychedelic substance in a naturalistic setting. Data unrelated to co-use of MDMA has previously been published from Study 136,40,72 and Study 273,74,75,76. Study designs were nearly identical and therefore data were collapsed across the two studies. The studies were approved by the Imperial College London’s Research Ethics Committee and Joint Research Compliance Office and were conducted in accordance with principles of Good Clinical Practice.

Eligibility criteria for both studies were as follows: (1) 18 years or older; (2) ability to read/write English; and (3) intention to use a psychedelic substance (e.g., psilocybin/magic mushrooms/truffles, MDMA, LSD/ 1-propionyl-lysergic acid diethylamide (1P-LSD), ayahuasca, N,N-Dimethyltryptamine (DMT), 5-methoxy-N,N-dimethyltryptamine (5-MeO-DMT), mescaline, 2,5-dimethoxy-4-bromophenethylamine (2C-B), salvia divinorum, iboga/ibogaine). Individuals were included in the present manuscript if they used psilocybin or LSD alone or co-used psilocybin or LSD and MDMA during their experience. Individuals were excluded from the present analyses if they used substances other than (a) psilocybin/LSD alone or (b) psilocybin/LSD and MDMA during their experience.

Participants were recruited using online advertisements, postings on Facebook, Twitter, and email newsletters, and online forums (e.g., Reddit). Interested participants reviewed study details and provided informed consent online along with their email address. Surveys were subsequently sent via email depending upon the date the participant intended to use a psychedelic. Surveys relevant to the present manuscript were administered seven days prior to the planned psychedelic experience and 1 day after the planned psychedelic experience. The following data was collected prior to participants’ psychedelic experience: demographics (age, sex, nationality, employment, and education); personality (Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability, and Openness to Experiences; measured via the Ten Item Personality Measure [TIPI]77); self-reported psychiatric history (previous and/or current use of psychiatric medications, lifetime psychiatric disorder); and lifetime psychedelic use (frequency).

Following their psychedelic experience, participants identified the psychedelic they used and the dose they used: (1) Low dose (e.g., < 50 μg LSD); (2) Moderate dose (e.g., 51–100 μg LSD); (3) High dose (e.g., 101–200 μg LSD); (4) Very high dose (e.g., 201–300 μg LSD); and (5) Extremely high dose (> 300 μg LSD) (Fig. 1). Participants were also asked whether they co-used MDMA during their experience and the dose of MDMA they used: (1) None; (2) Low; (3) Medium; and (4) High. Participants were asked to identify (yes/no) whether they had their psychedelic experience in specific settings (retreat, reactional/social, and/or therapeutic) and whether their experience featured the following elements: music; live singing; emotional support; sense of threat; strangers, and/or disruption. Finally, relating to their psychedelic use 1 day prior, participants completed measures of acute challenging and positive experiences (see ‘Measures’ section below). All data was collected using the online ‘Psychedelic Survey’ platform (https://www.psychedelicsurvey.com).

Figure 1
figure 1

Categorization by psilocybin/LSD and co-use of MDMA by dose.

Measures

Challenging experiences

Challenging experiences were measured using the Challenging Experience Questionnaire (CEQ10), a 26-item scale developed to characterize acute adverse experiences occasioned by psychedelic substances10. Subscales of the CEQ measure grief (6 items), fear (5 items), physical distress (5 items), insanity (3 items), isolation (3 items), death (2 items), and paranoia (2 items). Reflecting on a particular psychedelic experience, items are rated on a six-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (none; not at all) to 5 (extreme [more than ever before in my life]). In line with past research20, total challenging experiences were scored by calculating the mean of all 26 items multiplied by 20 to provide a score ranging from 0 to 100. Similarly, subscales were scored by calculating the mean for each subscale multiplied by 20. The CEQ has been utilized in both non-clinical10,12,20,36 and clinical7,21 studies of classic psychedelic experiences.

Positive experiences

Self-compassion, compassion, love, and gratitude

Positive experiences of self-compassion, compassion, gratitude, and love were each measured using individual self-constructed items. Reflecting on their psychedelic experience, participants rated each item on a visual analogue scale from 0 (No/not more than usual) to 100 (Yes/very much more than usual). Items were as follows: (1) self-compassion (“I felt compassion towards myself”); (2) compassion (“I felt compassion towards others”); (3) gratitude (“I felt a great sense of gratitude”); and (4) love (“I felt a great sense of love”). These items were only collected in Study 1 (n = 282).

Mystical experience

Mystical-type experiences were measured using the revised Mystical Experience Questionnaire (MEQ-3078). The MEQ-30 is a 30-item measure of mystical effects of classic psychedelics composed of four factors: (1) mystical (i.e., unity, noetic quality, and sacredness; 15 items); (2) deeply felt positive mood (6 items); (3) transcendence of time and space (6 items); and (4) ineffability/paradoxicality (3 items). Items are rated on a six-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (none/not at all) to 5 (extreme [more than any other time in my life]). Total mystical experience was scored by calculating the mean of all 30 items multiplied by 20 to provide a score ranging from 0 to 100. Subscale scores were similarly calculated using the relevant items. The MEQ-30 has been used widely in both non-clinical (e.g.,36,79,80) and clinical samples (e.g.,81,82,83).

Statistical analyses

Only one individual reported co-using psilocybin/LSD with high dose MDMA, therefore medium and high dose were collapsed into one category. Co-use of MDMA was categorized as either none (0), low (1), or medium–high (2), as shown in Fig. 2. We examined whether co-use of MDMA (none, low dose, and medium–high dose) with psilocybin/LSD predicted the intensity of participants’ challenging (total challenging experience [CEQ Total], grief, fear, physical distress, insanity, isolation, death, and paranoia [CEQ subscales]) and positive (love, gratitude, compassion, self-compassion, and mystical-type experience [MEQ-30 total score and mystical, positive mood, transcendence, and ineffability subscales]) experiences. All dependent variables were examined via Q-Q plots, histograms, and statistical analyses (i.e., Kolmogorov–Smirnov and Shapiro Wilk tests) and were found to be non-normally distributed (e.g., all Kolmogorov–Smirnov and Shapiro Wilk tests were p < 0.001). Therefore, we conducted a series of preliminary Kruskal–Wallis tests (without covariates). When these main effects were significant we then conducted Dunn’s post-hoc tests to compare psilocybin/LSD without MDMA against: (a) psilocybin/LSD + low dose MDMA; and (b) psilocybin/LSD + medium–high dose MDMA.

Figure 2
figure 2

Distribution of psilocybin/LSD use by dose.

Based on past research36,40,84,85, the following variables were examined as potential confounding variables: age, sex, lifetime previous psychedelic use (yes/no), lifetime previous psychedelic use (frequency), lifetime psychiatric diagnosis, previous use of psychiatric medications, current use of psychiatric medications, current use of antidepressant medication, psilocybin or LSD use for experience, psilocybin/LSD dose, personality (Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability, and Openness to Experiences; measured via the TIPI77), and setting (retreat, recreational/social, or therapeutic, presence of music, live singing, emotional support, a threat, strangers, and/or disruption). A series of statistical tests (ANOVAS for continuous variables and chi-squared tests for binary variables) were performed where MDMA dose was treated as the independent variable and potential confounds were included as the dependent variable. Variables that were significantly associated with MDMA dose (p < 0.05) were identified as potential confounds and were included as covariates in the primary analyses. Multicollinearity among the selected confounders were examined by calculating Pearson correlation coefficients (cut-off: r > 0.4) and variance inflation factors (VIFs; cut-off ≥ 5).

Quade nonparametric ANCOVAs were conducted wherein MDMA dose was the independent variable, acute experience measures were the dependent variable, and potential confounds were included as covariates. Post-hoc analyses were performed for significant group differences to determine if low dose and/or medium–high dose MDMA were responsible for significant effects. All analyses were conducted in SPSS (Version 28) and the threshold for statistical significance was set at p < 0.05, two-tailed.