Introduction

Impairments in social functioning are a hallmark feature of a wide range of mental health disorders, such as generalized anxiety, major depressive disorder, and schizophrenia1,2,3,4,5. Indeed, social impairment represents a large share of the cost to society of these disorders, as measured by the burden accrued by patients, caretakers, and treatment institutions6,7. However, options for treatment of impairments to social functioning have critical limitations to their efficacy8,9,10,11. Increasingly, the need to explore alternative approaches to supporting social functioning has been recognized, with promising results in the realm of novel pharmacotherapies12,13,14,15. In particular, MDMA (ā€œecstasyā€) and classic psychedelics represent two potential avenues for further exploration of potential treatments for impairments in social functioning, as preliminary evidence has demonstrated that these compounds might help to alleviate social difficulties. To this end, the present study aims to explore possible protective associations between MDMA/ecstasy and classic psychedelic use and impairments in social functioning in a naturalistic context.

First synthesized by Merck in 1912, MDMA/ecstasy is a synthetic amphetamine compound which has been linked to feelings of euphoria and connection, disinhibition, and fear extinction16,17. At the neurotransmitter level, MDMA/ecstasy acts by binding to serotonin transporters, and to a lesser degree dopamine and norepinephrine transporters, promoting the release of these neurotransmitters and blocking their removal18,19. Most prominently, in the last few years, an impressive body of clinical work has demonstrated the efficacy of MDMA-assisted psychotherapy in treating symptoms of PTSD20,21,22,23,24.

Concurrently, a broader literature has explored the relationship between MDMA/ecstasy and prosociality across a range of domains and methods. A meta-analysis conducted by Regan et al., aggregating data across 27 experimental studies and 592 participants, found a moderate-to-large effect (dā€‰=ā€‰0.86) of MDMA/ecstasy on self-reported sociability-related measures, such as feeling loving, talkative, and friendly25. Meanwhile, Britt and Bedi summarize a wide-ranging set of findings demonstrating MDMA/ecstasyā€™s effects on social behavior in humans and laboratory animals, including increased cognitive empathy (the ability to understand the feelings of another and take anotherā€™s perspective), reduced aggression, and blunted emotional responses to social rejection26. In addition, MDMA/ecstasy administration has also been linked to short-term improvements in emotional empathy (the ability to feel what another person feels) and increased social approach behavior (i.e., willingness to facilitate social interaction)27,28.

Drawing upon this evidence, researchers have begun to explore the clinical application of MDMA/ecstasy to the treatment of impairments in social functioning. In light of the link between MDMA/ecstasy and increased prosocial behavior, a prospective review paper by Heifets and Malenka (2016) proposes that MDMA/ecstasy may alleviate impairments in social functioning in the context of a wide range of mental health disorders (e.g., autism, schizophrenia, social anxiety disorder, and major depressive disorder)29. Additionally, a groundbreaking pilot study demonstrated a large (dā€‰=ā€‰1.4) effect of two MDMA-assisted psychotherapy sessions on social anxiety symptoms in adults with autism30, laying the groundwork for larger-scale studies and applications to social difficulties beyond the context of this disorder.

Classic psychedelics (Greek for ā€œmind-manifestingā€), meanwhile, are a set of serotonergic compounds either found in nature or synthesized from natural compounds, which lead to profound alterations in perception and may induce mystical-type experiences of lasting personal and spiritual significance31,32. Alongside their exploration in the treatment of addiction33,34, depression35,36, and end-of-life anxiety37, classic psychedelics have also been probed for their applications in the realm of social functioning. Researchers have linked the mystical experiences occasioned by classic psychedelics to increases in measures of prosocial attitudes and behaviors, while others have linked classic psychedelics to enhanced emotional empathy and sociality38,39. In addition, a pharmacological fMRI study by Preller et al. found that classic psychedelics modulate multiple aspects of social cognition via serotonin 2A receptor agonist activity, and these researchers thus suggest that these substances may treat social impairments related to mental health disorders40. Further basic and clinical evidence supports the possibility that classic psychedelics might have a role to play in the treatment of social impairments related to mental illness as well41,42.

In summary, an emerging body of evidence suggests that MDMA/ecstasy, and perhaps also classic psychedelics, have a role to play in treating impairments in social functioning. However, the nature of this evidence remains preliminary, especially in light of the limited number of clinical investigations in this research area as well as the small sample sizes of prior work.

Therefore, the current study aims to investigate the associations between use of MDMA/ecstasy and classic psychedelics and four different facets of social impairment, using a large-scale nationally representative sample. While this method cannot be used to infer direct causal associations between use of MDMA/ecstasy and classic psychedelics and quality of social functioning, analyses based on a large (nā€‰>ā€‰200,000) and representative population sample can complement basic science and small-sample clinical findings within this research area and facilitate future experimental studies within this research domain.

Methods

Data for this project are from the National Survey on Drug Use and Health (2015ā€“2019), an annual survey that assesses substance use and mental health in a nationally representative sample of the United States population aged 12 and older43,44,45,46,47. The NSDUH survey is administered by interviewers in participantsā€™ homes using a computer-assisted interviewing paradigm. Currently incarcerated individuals, active duty military members, and individuals experiencing homelessness that are not residing in a shelter are not surveyed by the NSDUH. We included all adults 18Ā years and older in our analyses (unweighted Nā€‰=ā€‰214,505). This study was exempt from IRB review as all NSDUH data are publicly available at the following web address: https://www.datafiles.samhsa.gov. Furthermore, this study was conducted in accordance with all of the relevant guidelines and procedures.

Dependent variables

We included all four dependent variables from the NSDUH that assessed impairments in social functioning caused by mental health problems or emotional difficulties. All participants were asked to think of the one month in the past 12 when they were ā€œthe most depressed, anxious, or emotionally stressedā€ when answering questions related to their social functioning. The four dependent variables are as follows:

  1. 1.

    The degree of difficulty interacting with strangers (ā€œhow much difficulty did you have dealing with people you did not know well?ā€) (ordinal: 1ā€“4)

  2. 2.

    Being prevented from interacting with strangers due to mental health issues (ā€œDid problems with your emotions, nerves, or mental health keep you from dealing with people you did not know well?ā€) (binaryā€”yes/no)

  3. 3.

    The degree of difficulty participating in social activities (ā€œhow much difficulty did you have participating in social activities, like visiting friends or going to parties?ā€) (ordinal: 1ā€“4)

  4. 4.

    Being prevented from engaging in social activities due to mental health issues (ā€œDid problems with your emotions, nerves, or mental health keep you from participating in social activities?ā€) (binaryā€”yes/no)

The variables assessing difficulty were assessed on a scale of ā€œ1ā€ (no difficulty) to ā€œ4ā€ (severe difficulty); although participants could also respond with a score of ā€œ5ā€ (did not engage with strangers/participate in social activities), we re-coded these responsesĀ as N/As, as these responses do not capture whether the lack of engagement or participation was caused by emotional difficulties or mental health problems.

Independent variables/covariates

The primary independent variable for our analyses was lifetime use of MDMA/ecstasy (yes/no). Additionally, lifetime use of four commonly used classic psychedelics (psilocybin, LSD, peyote, mescaline) served as exploratory independent variables in our analyses. We included the following demographic factors and substance use variables as covariates: marital status, educational attainment, sex, age, income level, race/ethnicity, self-reported engagement in risky behavior, and lifetime use of various legal and illegal substances (PCP [phencyclidine], cocaine, inhalants, tranquilizers, heroin, pain relievers, stimulants, sedatives, and marijuana).

These covariates have been used in various population-based studies on psychedelics48,49,50,51,52,53, allowing for comparability between the results of this study and prior research within this domain. Additionally, the demographic covariates control for many common confounds (e.g., race, socioeconomic status), while the lifetime use variables control for the impact that co-occurring substance use may have on our results. Overall, these covariates reduce the likelihood that any associations between our independent and dependent variables are spurious.

Analyses

For our project, we used the Survey package in R version 4.1.254, which allowed us to incorporate the survey weighting and complex design of the NSDUH into our analyses. We also used the gtsummary package in R to generate all descriptive tables and statistics55.

To report demographic information on our sample of interest, we calculated the weighted percentage of each category of our main demographic factors, stratified by those who have versus have not used MDMA/ecstasy. Furthermore, we also used chi-squared analyses to compare whether there were significant demographic differences between individuals who have versus have not used MDMA/ecstasy.

We used multivariable ordinal and logistic regression to assess the relationships between MDMA/ecstasy use, classic psychedelic use, and our dependent variables assessing impairments in social functioning. We used ordinal regression for our two models that include the ordinal dependent variables assessing the difficulty of engaging with strangers/participating in social activities. Our large sample size provided adequate power for the models included within our study, allowing us to satisfy this fundamental criterion for conducting regression models.ā€‰~ā€‰0.5% of total responses were omitted from the models due to missing data. Additionally, we used multivariable logistic regression for our two models that include the binary dependent variables assessing whether one was prevented from interacting with strangers/engaging in social activities due to mental health issues. All independent variables and covariates were entered simultaneously into our models.

Results

The demographics of our sample, stratified by those who have versus have not used MDMA/ecstasy are presented in Table 1. Individuals who have used MDMA/ecstasy are more likely to fall into the following demographic categories: never married, more formally educated, younger, male, Non-Hispanic White, and more likely to engage in risky behavior.

Table 1 Demographics of those who have versus have not used MDMA/ecstasy.

The results of our four models assessing the relationships between MDMA/ecstasy and classic psychedelic use and impairments in social functioning are presented in Table 2. Overall, MDMA/ecstasy conferred lowered odds of three of our four outcomes: difficulty dealing with strangers (aOR: 0.92; 95% CI [0.87, 0.97]), difficulty participating in social activities (aOR: 0.90 [0.85, 0.95]), and being prevented from engaging in social activities due to mental health issues (aOR: 0.84 [0.71, 0.99]). MDMA/ecstasy was not associated with being prevented from interacting with strangers. Mescaline use was also associated with lowered odds of difficulty dealing with strangers (aOR: 0.85 [0.76, 0.95]). All other substances, including classic psychedelics, either did not share a relationship to social impairment or conferred increased odds of social impairment. Lifetime LSD use was associated with increased odds of two social impairment outcomes: difficulty dealing with strangers (aOR: 1.13 [1.06, 1.20]) and difficulty participating in social activities (aOR: 1.11 [1.05, 1.17]).

Table 2 Results from multivariable ordinal and logistic regression models testing the associations between MDMA/ecstasy, classic psychedelics (psilocybin, LSD, peyote, mescaline), and impairments in social functioning.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to assess the relationship between use of MDMA/ecstasy and classic psychedelics, on one hand, and impairments in social functioning, on the other. Overall, lifetime use of MDMA/ecstasy conferred lowered odds of three of four outcomes related to impairments in social functioning. Mescaline was also associated with lowered odds of one outcome. All other substances either did not demonstrate an association with impairments in social functioning or conferred increased odds of social impairment. Furthermore, this study represents one of many that demonstrates lifetime psychedelic use to be associated with lowered odds of deleterious outcomes in a population-based survey sample48,50,51,52,53,56,57.

Potential explanations: MDMA/ecstasy and lowered odds of social impairment

There are a few possible explanations for our results linking MDMA/ecstasy use to lowered odds of social impairment. We first summarize third variable factors which might explain this pattern of association; next, we follow this summary with a discussion of potential neurotransmitter-receptor-level mechanisms, and conclude by reviewing potentially relevant neural and behavioral factors downstream of them.

Third variable factors

Third variable factors, such as personality traits, political affiliations, andĀ spirituality may drive the observed associations between MDMA/ecstasy and lowered odds of social impairment. ter Bogt et al. (2006) demonstrated that there were personality differences between MDMA/ecstasy users and non-users in a house party setting. Namely, the study revealed higher rates of extraversion associated with MDMA/ecstasy users58, a third-variable trait that may be linked to lowered odds of social impairment. Another study by Nour et al. (2017) discovered an association between psychedelic use and liberal political views59, providing another example of possible pre-drug differences that may exist in our sample. Altogether, existing studies suggest that third-variable, pre-drug traits may possibly account for some share of the observed correlation between MDMA/ecstasy use and reduced odds of social impairment in this study.

Neurotransmitter-receptor-level effects

The association between use of MDMA/ecstasy and reduced odds of social impairment is possibly linked to the drugā€™s effects on several critical neurotransmitters in the brain, namely, dopamine and serotoninā€”which lie upstream of other potential mechanisms at the neural and behavioral levels, mentioned later. Some evidence exists to suggest that MDMA-induced changes to these neurotransmitter-receptor systems in the brain are indeed long-lasting60, offering a plausible explanation for how limited intake of MDMA could be linked to persistent changes in social behavior.

Given that MDMA mainly impacts serotonin levels, it is worth considering that the association between lifetime use of MDMA and lowered odds of social impairment can be ultimately linked to changes in serotonergic neurotransmission. Lower levels of circulating serotonin in the prefrontal cortex are associated with more aggressive behavior in humans, and serotonergic supplementation is associated with greater agreeableness, cooperation, and affiliative behaviors in human and animal models61,62. In addition, MDMAā€™s dopaminergic activity might be implicated in the association between MDMA and lowered odds of social impairment, as evidence suggests that dopaminergic signaling promotes social approach behavior in the context of positive cues and plays a role in the sensitivity of rewards62.

Oxytocin release

Another potential mechanism by which MDMA/ecstasy is believed to increase sociability is through elevated oxytocin release, which may be mediated by the aforementioned changes in serotonergic neurotransmission63. Oxytocin is known to play an important role in facilitating social learning and social connection in mammals, including humans64,65,66. In a randomized controlled trial by Dumont et. al. aimed at assessing the relationship between MDMA/ecstasy administration and oxytocin release, MDMA/ecstasy was observed to increase both subjective prosocial feelings and oxytocin concentrations relative to placebo66. Moreover, increased prosocial feelings were more strongly correlated with elevated blood oxytocin levels than with blood MDMA/ecstasy levels, suggesting that the relationship between MDMA/ecstasy use and prosocial feelings might be distinctly mediated by oxytocin release. Given that oxytocin promotes trust and emotional connection, MDMA/ecstasyā€™s facilitation of its release might explain why MDMA/ecstasy users often feel more connected and prosocial67,68. Thus, increased social bonding via oxytocin might in part explain the association between MDMA/ecstasy and lowered oddsĀ of social impairments.

Decreased amygdala reactivity

MDMA/ecstasy may also contribute to increased sociability by reducing amygdala reactivity to social situations, an effect downstream of changes in serotonergic neurotransmission and oxytocin release69,70. The amygdala has been shown to be important in socioemotional processing, particularly for threat-related information71. In a 2009 fMRI study, Bedi et al., found that MDMA/ecstasy had the effect of diminishing amygdala reactivity toward angry facial expressions72. Suppression of the social threat fear response produced by the amygdala may be one explanation for the prosocial effects of MDMA/ecstasy. Furthermore, in the aforementioned study by Dumont et al. that found MDMA to simultaneously increase prosocial feelings and oxytocin levels, the authors also propose that decreased amygdala activity may mediate the association between oxytocin and prosocial feelings66. Specifically, these authors draw on prior evidence that indicates oxytocin may diminish the amygdala response to novel social encounters, ultimately increasing prosocial feelings and potentially decreasing social impairment66,73. Thus, decreased amygdala reactivity promoted by MDMA/ecstasy may also contribute to decreased fear of social interaction, ultimately lowering the odds of social impairment.

Increased social motivation

MDMAā€™s serotonergic and dopaminergic properties might also be related to experimentally observed increases in social motivation after ingestion of MDMA. Both self-report and behavioral measures suggest that MDMA promotes a desire to be with other individuals, and enhances intrinsic motivation to engage in social approach behavior74. Meanwhile, MDMA might also blunt the effects of perceived social rejection, an experience which often reduces motivation to engage socially75. Taken together, these factors might have the effect of promoting more effective socialization and greater social connection, thereby reducing the likelihood of social impairment in the long run.

Enhanced attention and reward response to positive social stimuli

Downstream of its promotion of dopaminergic neurotransmission and oxytocin release, MDMA has demonstrated the capacity to enhance attention and reward response to positive social stimuli, perhaps accounting for some of its observed prosocial effects. An increasingly wide body of research suggests that MDMA increases attention to positive facial expressions, heightens sensitivity to social reward cues as measured by ventral striatum activation, and reopens the window for social reward learning by means of neuroplastic changes in the nucleus accumbens76,77,78. It is possible that enhanced sensitivity to reward in the context of social interaction helps to promote both higher levels of social engagement, and the development of more effective, reward-conditioned social attunement.

Increased empathy

The empathy-promoting effects of MDMA/ecstasy, widely attested to in the research literature and perhaps downstream of changes in serotonergic neurotransmission, might also underlie our core finding that lifetime MDMA/ecstasy use was associated with lowered odds of impairments in social functioning79. In a 2017 pooled analysis, participants from 6 controlled studies were found to have enhanced emotional empathy as a result of MDMA/ecstasy use80. Moreover, in a 2014 trial that compared performance between participants given MDMA/ecstasy and participants given methylphenidate (Ritalin) on a variety of empathy-related tasks, only participants who received MDMA/ecstasy demonstrated emotional empathy for positive stimuli, as well as increased subjective empathogenic feelings81. Furthermore, in another placebo-controlled trial performed by Hysek et al., a wide battery of measures captured increases in both emotional empathy and prosocial orientation due to MDMA/ecstasy administration27. In particular, Hysek et al. found decreased competitive behavior and increased empathetic concern in male subjects27. Overall, this experimental evidence suggests that an increase in empathy induced by MDMA/ecstasy might promote prosociality.

Potential explanations: classic psychedelics and lowered odds of social impairment

Despite the present studyā€™s primary focus on protective associations between lifetime use of MDMA/ecstasy and impairments in social functioning, we also took an interest in whether lifetime use of classic psychedelics, including tryptamines such as psilocybin, phenethylamines like mescaline, and lysergamides like LSD, shared protective associations with social impairment as well. Notably, the one significant association between lifetime use of a classic psychedelic and lowered odds of social impairment involved mescaline, which, like MDMA/ecstasy, belongs to the class of phenethylamine compounds.

Two possible explanations can be offered to explain this distinctive pattern, in which mescaline, but no other classic psychedelics, confers lowered odds of social impairment. First, it is possible that because MDMA and mescaline belong to the same class of compounds (phenethylamines), the associations that mescaline shares with lowered odds of social impairment are downstream of the common pharmacological mechanisms of this class of substances. Accordingly, classic psychedelics in other compound classes such as psilocybin and LSD would therefore lack these associations with social impairment. Second, it is possible that the common, specific association of MDMA and mescaline with reduced odds of social impairment can be attributed to common pre-drug factors that unite MDMA and mescaline users, or at minimum, set mescaline users apart from users of other classic psychedelics. For example, similar to the above-mentioned link between higher extroversion and individuals who use MDMA at the pre-drug level58, it is possible that a similar pattern applies to its structural cousin, mescaline. Additionally, while mescaline was once a popular drug of choice among individuals who use classic psychedelics, use of mescaline is now relatively less common82ā€”suggesting that mescaline users might be a distinct, self-selected group who might differ in pre-drug traits from those who use more common classic psychedelics like psilocybin and LSD. However, it is important to note that these two explanations are speculative and additional research is needed to better understand this pattern of results.

Next, we also plan to explore the potential mechanisms by which mescaline, in particular, and classic psychedelics, in general, might also confer lowered odds of impairments in social functioning. As little research has been conducted on mescaline specifically, we will review the literature on classic psychedelics with similar subjective effects, as research on these compounds can inform us about the potential effects of mescaline on social impairment.

Neurotransmitter-receptor-level effects

Mescaline acts primarily as an agonist of 5-HT2A receptors. While this mechanism stands apart from the serotonergic pharmacology of MDMA, it may play some role in the observed relationship between lifetime use of mescaline and lowered odds of social impairment, by way of serotoninā€™s documented role in empathy modulation, recognition of facial emotions, cooperation, and aggression61,62. Meanwhile, some evidence exists for mescalineā€™s role as a weak dopamine receptor agonist, but the nature of this link is tentative and uncertain83.

Decreased amygdala reactivity

Similar to MDMA/ecstasy, mescaline may promote sociability by occasioning reductions in amygdala reactivity, as well as shifts in emotional processing related to negative facial recognition. In a randomized controlled fMRI study by Mueller et al., lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) was found to reduce reactivity of the left amygdala to fearful stimuli84. Importantly, previous studies have specifically implicated the left amygdala in the processing of negative facial expressions, relative to its counterpart to the right85,86,87,88. By reducing reactivity of the left amygdala to negative facial expressions, classic psychedelics, including mescaline, might help to reduce a felt sense of threat in the presence of others, thereby reducing impairments in social functioning.

Greater sense of interconnectedness

Additionally, mescaline, as well as other classic psychedelics, may promote prosociality and lower the odds of social impairment by promoting a felt sense of interconnectedness with other people89. In a follow-up of an open label study aimed at investigating the possible role of psilocybin-assisted therapy in promoting smoking cessation, participants also reported engaging in more prosocial behavior after treatment90. The authors theorized this effect to be mediated by self-reported increases in felt interconnectedness, which resulted in more prosocial action90. Furthermore, in an open label study that demonstrated psilocybin with psychological support to be an effective treatment for treatment-resistant depression91, participants also endorsed a greater sense of ā€œconnectednessā€ following this therapy as well92. By promoting a greater sense of interconnectedness, classic psychedelics, including mescaline, might help to ease social distress and ultimately lower the odds of impairments in social functioning.

Limitations

Our findings must be interpreted in view of several limitations which have been named in other population-based survey studies52,93,94. First, we cannot infer causal relationships from our data, which are cross-sectional in nature. Longitudinal studies and randomized controlled trials are necessary to demonstrate a causal link between MDMA/ecstasy and mescaline use with lowered odds of impairments in social functioning.

Second, due to limitations in the NSDUH dataset, we were unable to control for all potential demographic confounds that may otherwise explain the link between MDMA/ecstasy use and lowered odds of impairments in social functioning. For example, the NSDUH does not survey individuals living in treatment centers, active duty military members, and incarcerated individuals. Furthermore, as previously mentioned in our Results, there are significant differences between those who have versus have not used MDMA and such differences may underlie our results, as individuals who have used MDMA tend to be maleĀ andĀ Non-Hispanic White. Although we controlled for these demographic factors in our results, there may be other demographic factors related to individuals who use MDMA/ecstasy that we could not incorporate into our models. Overall, the absence of individuals from these unsampled populations, as well as other sources of demographic skew in the NSDUH sample, might limit the generalizability of our findings.

Third, pharmacological limitations must be taken into account, as determining the purity and authenticity of the MDMA/ecstasy ingested by NSDUH respondents is beyond the scope of our study. In a naturalistic context, doses of MDMA/ecstasy vary widely in their purity and in the amount of MDMA they contain95,96. The impure quality of naturalistic MDMA/ecstasy may weaken any pharmacological interpretations of our observed link between MDMA/ecstasy use and lowered odds of social impairments. It is notable, however, that in spite of this limitation, we still found evidence for a link between lifetime MDMA/ecstasy use and reduced impairments in social functioning, in line with the existing literature on the prosocial effects of MDMA/ecstasy. Randomized controlled trials using pure, laboratory-grade MDMA can address this limitation and help to establish a causal link between MDMA/ecstasy use and the alleviation of social impairment.

Fourth, it is possible harmful outcomes occurred due to MDMA/ecstasy or classic psychedelic use, causing or exacerbating social difficulties for some participants in this study. This idea is reinforced by our findings linking lifetime LSD use to increased odds of difficulty dealing with strangers and difficulty participating in social activities. Both MDMA/ecstasy and classic psychedelics have been associated with adverse outcomes in some instances. Although the evidence remains equivocal, MDMA/ecstasy use has been linked to neurotoxicity and lasting cognitive impairments with chronic use97,98,99. Additionally, MDMA/ecstasy can acutely cause adverse reactions such as anxiety and mood disturbance as well100,101.

Classic psychedelics have also been linked to adverse reactions upon acute administrationā€”commonly referred to as ā€œbad tripsā€ā€”in which individuals experience panic, paranoia, extreme fear and distress, and anxiety during a psychedelic experience102. Furthermore, classic psychedelics have been tentatively linked to increased risk of psychosis102,103,104, although much of the evidence that supports this association is anecdotal and historical. These potential harms might also explain the lack of associations that we found in this study for psilocybin and peyote and social impairment. Although we previously described potential drivers by which classic psychedelics may alleviate social impairment, such drivers may be counterbalanced by these potential harms at the population level. Relatedly, there may be large sub-groups in the sample who are particularly vulnerable to these harms. Analyzing these groups alongside the broader sample, which includes individuals for whom classic psychedelics have a neutral or salutary effect, may have subsequently led to our null findings for these compounds. Overall, future investigations should examine the conditions under which MDMA/ecstasy and classic psychedelics can potentially promote harmful outcomes and contribute to impairments in social functioning.

Fifth, multicollinearity is a possible limitation to our study. Many of the substance use and demographic variables in our analyses likely share robust levels of correlation, and studies utilizing a virtually identical analytical structure have identified multicollinearity within these models48. The impact of multicollinearity on our results is largely mitigated by our large sample size, however. Multicollinearity serves to inflate the standard errors of oneā€™s models and make it less likely for significance tests to reach significance, a fact that does not pose an issue for samples large enough to buffer against such inflation105. However, future studies can address this limitation by using analytical approaches that are designed to handle multicollinearity, such as ridge regression.

Sixth, this study design did not allow us to establish frequency of substance use or temporal precedence between MDMA/ecstasy and classic psychedelic use, on one hand, and decreased social difficulties related to mental health, on the other. However, psychedelics have demonstrated that they can promote salutary outcomes with few uses in therapeutic contexts20,106; thus it remains plausible that infrequent use may be associated with salutary outcomes in this study. Furthermore because MDMA/ecstasy use was assessed over a lifetime, it is likely that much of the MDMA/ecstasy use occurred prior to the onset of social difficulties as well. Future longitudinal studies and cross sectional studies featuring non-overlapping time horizons and more granular measures of substance use can more thoroughly address this limitation.

Conclusion

The aim of this investigation was to assess whether associations existed between MDMA/ecstasy and classic psychedelic use (psilocybin, LSD, peyote, mescaline) and impairments in social functioning. Overall, we found lifetime MDMA/ecstasy use to be associated with lowered odds of the majority of our social impairment outcomes and lifetime mescaline use to confer lowered odds of difficulty dealing with strangers. LSD was associated with increased odds of two social impairment outcomes as well. Although this study cannot be used to determine a causal relationship between use of these substances and changes in social difficulties, this study can pave the way for future experimental studies that assess whether these substances can support social functioning, as well as others that allow for better understanding of potential risks related to these compounds as well. Overall, this study represents incremental progress in better supporting individuals experiencing social difficulties related to mental health disorders.