Introduction

Developmental dyslexia (DD) is a specific learning disorder causing persistent failure to acquire efficient reading. It is the most prevalent neurodevelopmental disorder, affecting about 9% of children1, and it has profound lifelong consequences on academic and professional success2. Understanding the pathophysiological mechanisms of DD is paramount to advance this society's grand challenge.

Thousands of studies have investigated the pathophysiology of DD, thereby documenting various impairments and developing highly debated theories3,4. Although DD has long been considered a purely cognitive disorder, mounting evidence revealed that DD is associated with sensory impairments5,6, for a review see7. Yet, an outstanding question remains. Do sensory impairments cause DD? On one side, sensory theories argue that sensory deficiencies cause reading disabilities4,7. On the other side, cognitive theories argue that this is the lack of reading practice that causes sensory deficits8,9. Because DD is mostly associated with visual and hearing impairments—two senses heavily involved in reading—this long-standing controversy is still prominent.

According to sensory theories, troubles are supposed to encompass the whole sensory system, not only vision and hearing4,7. Cognitive theories, instead, would hypothesize that sensory troubles are restrained to reading-related senses. Results that support the generalization of sensory impairments in DD are scarce10. Testing a sensory function that is independent of reading practice would thus significantly advance the pathophysiology of DD. A positive result would undeniably support sensory theories and vice-versa.

Proprioception is an essential component of sensorimotor control11,12, and several studies have reported sensorimotor impairments in DD13,14. Here, we directly test proprioceptive acuity in dyslexic children and age-matched controls.

Results

To compare basic reaction time levels between groups, we measured simple reaction times to salient visual and auditory stimulus (See Methods and supplementary Table 1 for values). Figure 1 reveals similar reaction times in dyslexics and controls for both sensory modalities (visual: t(32) = 0.26, p = 0.79, d = 0.09 ; auditory: t(32) = 1.22, p = 0.23, d = 0.42). Since developmental dyslexia is associated with noisier behaviors13,15, we also compared variable reaction times—i.e., intra-participants variability—between Controls and Dyslexics. Variable visual and auditory reaction times were similar between groups (visual: t(32) = 0.19, p = 0.85, d = 0.19; auditory: t(32) = − 0.43, p = 0.67, d = 0.15). It is worth reminding that vision and hearing impairments have already been demonstrated in DD for more difficult sensory tasks5,6,15. Here, simple reaction time to salient stimulus cannot capture these fine impairments and only provide baseline results to help interpreting finer measurements in the proprioceptive task.

Figure 1
figure 1

Mean Visual and Auditory Reaction Times. The box lines indicate the 25th, 50th, and 75th percentiles, and the whiskers extend to the 5th and 95th percentiles. Dots display individual values (n = 17 in each group), and the cross indicates the average.

We also used a simple reaction time task to compare proprioceptive acuity between dyslexics and controls. A robotic manipulandum passively rotated their elbow joint, and children pressed a trigger as soon as they felt the motion. The robot speed changed from trial to trial, thereby modulating the proprioceptive stimulation intensity; i.e., the signal to noise ratio. Proprioceptive impairments are known to cause longer and more variable reaction times for the weakest stimulations16. Figure 2A–B displays mean and variable proprioceptive detection times of the two groups for the six passive movement speeds (see supplementary Table 2 for values). Both Dyslexics and controls showed the well-known effect of stimulus intensity; i.e., mean and variable proprioceptive reaction times decreased when passive movement speeds increased16,17. It is striking, however, that the slope of these relationships differed between groups. Although dyslexics and controls exhibited similar values at higher speeds, dyslexics were slower and more variable than controls at slower speeds. At the slowest speed (0.25° s−1), dyslexics were even twice as long and twice as variable as controls. Out of seventeen dyslexics, twelve (71%) fell outside normality (95% confidence interval) according to their mean reaction time and ten (59%) according to their variable reaction time. ANOVA yielded strong group x speed interaction effects on mean [F(5,160) 9.60; p < 1e−6; η2 = 0.23] and on variable proprioceptive reaction times [F(5,160) 7.22; p = 4e−6; η2 = 0.18]. Post-hoc comparisons confirmed that dyslexics were longer (p = 2.7e−5, d = 1.14) and more variable (p = 1.6e−4, d = 0.92) than controls at the slowest speed (0.25° s−1). This echoes previous results showing noise-related sensorimotor impairments in DD; for a review see4,15.

Figure 2
figure 2

Proprioceptive detection times of passive motion at all tested speeds. (A) Mean detection time (± SE) for both groups. (B) Mean intra-participant variability of detection time (± SE) for both groups. (C) Box plots of mean and variable detection times at the slowest speed. The box lines indicate the 25th, 50th, and 75th percentiles, and the whiskers extend to the 5th and 95th percentiles. Dots display individual values (n = 17 in each group), and the cross indicates the average. (D) Box plots of mean and variable detection times at the highest speed. Same organization as panel (C).

Box-plots in Fig. 2C–D detail mean and variable reaction times for the slowest and the highest speed. Although results obtained at the slowest speed show clear group effects (Fig. 2C), results obtained at the highest speed demonstrate that dyslexics and controls responded equally quickly to proprioceptive stimulations (Fig. 2D). Also, comparing the first three to the last three trials did not reveal any effect in dyslexics nor in controls (t(16) = 9e−3; p = 0.99, d = 0.04) nor in controls (t(16) = 1.16; p = 0.26, d = 0.01). Thus, in both groups, the baseline response speed to proprioceptive stimuli was stable throughout the experiment. Furthermore, EMG activations of three major muscles around the elbow joint ensured that dyslexics and controls equally-well respected the instruction to keep their muscles relaxed during robotic manipulations (see supplementary Table 3).

Next, one wonders how much proprioceptive impairment can explain reading impairment. We computed Pearson's correlation coefficients between a proprioceptive acuity index and a reading ability index (see methods). Within these indexes, higher values mean better function. Figure 3 reveals a strong positive correlation between proprioceptive acuity and reading ability (Pearson R = 0.61, p = 1.3e−4). Importantly, this is not the result of a bimodal distribution of dyslexics' and controls' data. When pooled across groups, both indexes showed normal distribution (p > 0.2). When separating groups, a significant correlation existed in the dyslexic group (Pearson R = 0.52, p = 0.03) but not in the control group (Pearson R = 0.15, p = 0.57). No such correlation existed between the reading ability index and visual or auditory reaction times (see supplementary Table 4). Thus, although not all dyslexics show a strong proprioceptive impairment, the continuous distributions of proprioceptive and reading levels are tightly linked. More, proprioceptive indexes of the elbow and hip jointstested in a subset of 12 participants (6 dyslexics and 6 controls) – were significantly correlated in dyslexics (Spearman R = 0.83, p = 0.04) but not in controls (Spearman R = 0.03, p = 0.96). Proprioceptive impairment in dyslexia likely generalizes to other joints.

Figure 3
figure 3

Correlation between proprioceptive acuity and reading abilities (n = 17 in each group). An index representing proprioceptive acuity (vertical axis) was computed as the normalized average of Mean and variable detection time at the slowest speed. An index representing reading abilities (horizontal axis) was computed as the normalized average of four clinical scores that are routinely used to diagnose developmental dyslexia (see Methods). Dashed lines depict linear regressions for each group.

Discussion

The first level of interpretation of the present results concerns the understanding of the pathophysiological mechanisms of DD. Until now, sensory impairments in DD were mostly documented for vision and hearing5,6,10, for a review see7, making them prone to the criticism that they could result from a lack of reading practice8,9. Because reading does not solicit arm and hip proprioception, the lack of reading practice cannot explain the sizeable proprioceptive impairment revealed in this study. Instead, our results undeniably support the hypothesis that damaged neural mechanisms cause widespread sensory troubles in DD. The present results complement recent longitudinal and training evidence supporting sensory theories of developmental dyslexia18,19,20,21,22,23.

Several frameworks propose candidate neural structures and mechanisms whose impairment could explain sensory and sensorimotor troubles in DD. For example, the Delayed neural commitment framework proposes that abnormal temporalities in the development of distributed neural networks lead to reading and other learning disabilities4. More specific, the Magnocellular theory proposes that this is the impaired development of the Magnocellular system that causes poor temporal sensory processing, hence impeding efficient reading7. Impaired attentional processes have also been proposed to cause DD24,25. Particularly, feature-selective attention is known to influence noise-correlation in sensory cortices and may explain noise-exclusion deficit in DD15,26,27.

Research on other cognitive pathologies, such as autism and Alzheimer's dementia, suggests that testing sensorimotor functions allows more precocious diagnoses than testing cognitive functions28,29. Developmental studies of sensory and sensorimotor functions in DD have the potential to further our understanding of its etiology and to develop innovative remediations4,8. In addition to vision and hearing, future work shall also consider proprioception.

The second level of interpretation of the present results concerns the role played by proprioceptive impairments in DD. We found a strong correlation between a reading ability index and a proprioceptive acuity index. Proprioceptive acuity also significantly correlated with phonological processing alone (Pearson R = 0.45, p = 0.008). One may wonder whether proprioception could cause reading impairments itself. The motor theory of speech perception30 postulated that gesture recognition participates in the perception of phonological units. Several experiments then demonstrated that mirror neurons allow such gesture recognition31,32 and that gesture recognition—involving proprioceptive and tactile information—indeed participates in phonological perception33,34. Thus, proprioceptive impairment, along with vision and hearing impairments, may well participate in causing Developmental Dyslexia.

Materials and methods

Participants

Eighteen children with developmental dyslexia (7 girls; mean age: 10.92 ± 0.78 years) and seventeen age-matched typical readers (9 girls; mean age: 11.11 ± 0.85 years) participated in the study. We restricted the age for inclusion to 10–12 years because previous work has shown that proprioceptive function was roughly stable between these boundaries for a review see35. All were native French speakers, right-handed (Edinburgh Handedness Inventory; Oldfield, 1971), and normally integrated the school grade corresponding to their age (neither skipped nor doubled) without need for specific assistance. It is important to stress here that, in French schools, medical doctors screen children for cognitive disabilities two times before that age. Thus, although not ideal, this indicates that all children had a normal cognitive level. They also had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, normal hearing, and no history of neurological, proprioceptive, nor psychiatric disorders. Children and parents gave their free and informed written consent, and an ethics committee approved all experimental procedures (Comite de Protection des Personnes Ile de France VII, France; number 2017-A01547-46). All procedures were carried out according to local requirements and international norms (Declaration of Helsinki, 1964).

We recruited dyslexics via independent speech and occupational therapists. The diagnosis of developmental dyslexia was established by specialists (neuropsychologists, neuropediatricians, or speech therapists), using both inventories and testing procedures following the DSM-IV or DSM-5 guidelines. We recruited control children in municipal associations and schools. Neither control children nor their close family members had ever seen a medical doctor or related therapist for learning disorders.

Clinical evaluations

We screened all children for attentional36 and motor abilities37. The aim was to exclude children at risk of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and/or Developmental Coordination Disorder. We excluded one dyslexic boy from the study because of his positive result on the Conners test.

A speech therapist blindly evaluated reading abilities of all children with three clinical tests: (i) the Alouette Reading Test38, which is the French standardized assessment of reading skills; (ii) the "Phonological Process" part of NEPSY 239,40, evaluating phonological segmentation skills; (iii) the Timé 341, that evaluates words identification. As expected, controls outperformed dyslexics on all measures (Table 1). We performed inclusion procedures and all above-mentioned clinical evaluations in one session, and the experimental sensory testing described here-after in a second session. For each child, no more than three months separated these sessions.

Table 1 Clinical Scores of Dyslexic and Control children at the Alouette, Nepsy2, and Timé3 tests (see Methods).

Sensory testing

First, to evaluate response speed, children performed a classical visual and auditory simple reaction time test using salient stimuli42. The order of visual and auditory tasks was pseudo-randomized. Children sat in front of a monitor with their left index finger touching the computer space key and had to press it as soon as they saw (460 * 550 pixels, high contrast) or heard (900 Hz, 50 dB) the stimulus. Ten visual and ten auditory stimuli were presented with a random—two to five seconds—inter-trial delay. Before the test, children performed three practice trials per sensory condition.

After a 10 min break, we measured proprioceptive acuity using a well-known movement detection task16,17, which evaluates the capacity to detect the passive motion of a body–limb at various speeds. A robotic isokinetic dynamometer delivered proprioceptive stimulations (Biodex Medical Systems®, Shirley, NY, USA). Children sat on a chair and placed their right forearm on the dynamometer's resting device. We aligned the dynamometer's axis of rotation with the elbow flexion/extension axis of each child. The initial position of the arm was as follows: shoulder abducted between 70° and 85°, elbow flexed at 60° (0° was elbow extension), and forearm fully pronated16,17. Within this configuration, the dynamometer passively flexed the elbow in the horizontal plane. We instructed children to relax their muscles and to focus on detecting passive flexion. Children had to press a trigger, held in their left hand, as soon as they felt the motion. Here we call "proprioceptive reaction time", the time between the motion start and the trigger pressing. The dynamometer moved with six different angular speeds: 0.25, 0.5, 1, 5, 10 and 20°s−1. A total of seventy-two trials (12 trials per speed, in random order) were completed in six blocks separated by a two-minute rest time to avoid tiredness. Before the test, children performed two practice trials at each velocity (twelve trials). A subset of participants (six dyslexics and six controls) also performed the same proprioceptive evaluation protocol on the hip joint.

Children wore opaque goggles and noise reduction headphones (Bose, QC25) to prevent sight and hearing from influencing perceptual judgments. Because changes in muscle history can lead to errors in limb position sense, we controlled for muscle thixotropy by asking children to contract the muscles around their right elbow for one second before each passive movement11,43. Children were also required not to perform any vigorous motor or cognitive activity three hours before the tests. Children were authorized to take breaks at any time to prevent fatigue during the experiment.

We recorded surface electromyography (EMG) over the biceps brachii (BB), the brachio-radialis (BR), and the Triceps brachii long head (TBL) to control that muscles were relaxed during proprioceptive stimulations. We recorded EMG using silver-chloride surface electrodes (7 mm recording diameter, Ag–AgCl, 20 mm inter-pole distance). The skin was shaved and cleaned with alcohol. We synchronically recorded EMG signals with torque and angle using a Biopac system (Biopac MP150, Biopac System Inc, USA, gain = 1000, band-passed 1 Hz–5 kHz, sampled at 10 kHz).

Data analysis

We computed all parameters using custom MATLAB programs (MathWorks).

Reaction time. We computed visual and auditory reaction times as the time-interval between the presentation of the stimulus and the children's response on the keyboard/trigger. For each child and condition, we computed the mean and the variable reaction time (standard deviation of trials).

Electromyography. We rectified, band-passed (30–300 Hz), and integrated (300 ms skipping window) EMG signals. Then, we computed the ratio of EMG before movement start (− 300 ms) divided by maximum EMG during movement, and we used this ratio to compare muscle activation/relaxation between groups.

Statistics

All variables showed normal distribution (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test) and equivalent variance (Levene test). We performed group comparisons using independent two-tails t-tests (visual, auditory reaction times, and clinical results). We compared proprioceptive reaction times using repeated-measure ANOVA with a group (Controls vs. Dyslexics) and a speed factor (0.25°s−1, 0.5°s−1, 1°s−1, 5°s−1, 10°s−1 and 20°s−1), and post-hoc analyses (Scheffe) when appropriate. We also computed Pearson's correlation coefficients between reading abilities and elbow proprioceptive acuity. We computed a proprioceptive acuity index as the normalized average of mean and variable reaction times (at the slowest speed). We computed a reading ability index as the normalized average of four clinical scores classically used to diagnose DD in France (see Table 1): the Alouette Speed Index, the Alouette Accuracy index, the NEPSY2 result, and the Timé 3 result. We used Spearman correlation coefficients to compare arm and hip proprioceptive indexes.

Ethics approval

The ethics committee Comite de Protection des Personnes Ile de France VII, France (2017-A01547-46) approved all experimental procedures.

Consent to participate

Children and parents gave their free and informed written consent to participate.

Consent for publication

Children and parents gave their free and informed written consent for results to be published.