Introduction

Distinguishing bipolar disorder (BD) from major depressive disorder (MDD) remains challenging in clinical practice because diagnostic criteria for depressive episodes in the context of BD and MDD are identical [1]. Neuroimaging studies directly comparing depressed individuals with BD and MDD may help to identify objective neurobiological markers for these disorders, thus improving symptomatic and functional outcomes for affected individuals [2].

Working memory is a limited-capacity system used to maintain, manipulate, and update information online [3]. The difficulty of a working memory task is determined by the amount of information that an individual needs to process at one time. A more difficult vs. easy task requires processing of more information, recruits additional neurocognitive resources, and results in behavioral performance worsening and greater activation in a so-called working memory network consisting of prefrontal cortical regions (PFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), posterior parietal cortex, and some medial temporal and striatal regions [3,4,5,6,7,8]. Damage to the neural substrates of the working memory network leads to impairment in learning, reasoning, and decision-making [4, 9].

Task performance is preceded by task anticipation. During task anticipation, individuals prepare for a task by recollecting task-related rules and forming expectations related to the task’s difficulty and emotional valence. Anticipation-related brain activation may interact with performance-related brain activation to ensure optimal brain dynamics and resource usage. For example, PFC and ACC are involved during anticipation of emotional stimuli [10]. Anticipatory activation in these regions predicted reaction time (RT) during problem-solving tasks [11]. Activation in the left ventrolateral PFC (VLPFC) and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) increased when a more difficult vs. easy task was performed, but decreased when a more difficult task was anticipated [7]. This activation pattern might be related to “clearing of” online storage of information to free neural and cognitive resources prior to performing a difficult working memory task [7].

Individuals with mood disorders often suffer from cognitive dysfunction, especially during depressive episode [12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19] that maybe related to aberrant functioning of the regions described above [19,20,21,22,23,24]. Depressed individuals also show strong negative bias when anticipating future events and experiences [25, 26]. Given that previous studies have demonstrated that individuals with BD, MDD, and HC have different activation patterns in ACC and PFC during working memory task performance [24, 27] and also during anticipation of monetary reward and loss [28, 29], it is possible that the three groups may also differ during anticipation of easy vs. difficult working memory tasks. Even though in real life people are constantly involved in evaluating the difficulty of upcoming tasks, neural correlates associated with this process are largely underexplored. Meanwhile, task difficulty anticipation may play a critical role in cognitive and psychosocial functioning because misappraisal of task difficulty could result in poorly informed behaviors that might, in turn, impair functioning.

In this study, we aimed to examine the differences in anticipatory processing characterizing BD, MDD, and HC, and to shed light on whether cognitive dysfunction in depression is related to the decreased ability to perform the task, decreased ability to prepare for the task, or both. Given that processing of emotion-independent stimuli may be differentially biased and altered by emotion-dependent cognition in depressed individuals [30], we used affective stimuli such as happy, fearful, and neutral faces as stimuli in a Cued Emotional N-back Task that examined anticipation of and performance on an easy (1-back) vs. difficult (2-back) working memory tasks in depressed individuals with BD, those with MDD and HC.

The differences (or deltas ([]) between easy vs. difficult tasks in RT (∆RT), accuracy (∆accuracy), brain activation during task anticipation (∆cue), and task performance (∆task)) were calculated as they may be indicative of the amount of the additional neurocognitive resources required to perform a difficult vs. easy task. The interaction between ∆cue and ∆task, and ∆RT and ∆accuracy, may represent a trade-off between resource usage, freeing, and allocation prior to and during the task. These values together with behavioral and clinical variables were used in a multinomial elastic-net-regularized regression to identify variables contributing to BD vs. MDD vs. HC classification [31]. The predictive ability of selected variables was then validated using the random forest classifier [32].

Methods

Participants

The study was approved by the University of Pittsburgh and Carnegie Mellon University (CMU) Institutional Review Boards. Participants were recruited from community, universities, counselling, and medical centers. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. Participants were right-handed, fluent in English, and matched on age, gender, and IQ. HC had no personal or family history of psychiatric disorders. Symptomatic individuals met DSM-5 criteria either for major depressive (MDD) or bipolar (BD) disorders, and were depressed at scan (Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD-25) [33] score >14). Of 21 individuals with BD, one did not finish the scan, one had low accuracy, and one had excessive motion (>4 mm). Of 25 HC, one had excessive motion, and one did not follow task instructions. Of 24 individuals with MDD, one participant had low accuracy. These six individuals were removed from the analyses. The final data set consisted of 64 participants: 18 with BD (BD Type I = 3, Type II = 15), 23 with MDD, and 23 HC.

Clinical assessment

All diagnoses were made by a trained clinician and confirmed by a psychiatrist according to DSM-5 criteria using MINI7.0 [34]. Exclusion criteria included a history of head injury, metal in the body, pregnancy, claustrophobia, neurodevelopmental disorders, systemic medical illness, premorbid IQ < 85 as per the National Adult Reading Test (NART) [35], current alcohol/drug abuse, the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) [36] score >10 at scan, and meeting criteria for any psychotic-spectrum disorder. We also collected the following measures: current depression symptoms using the HDRS-25 [33], current mania symptoms using the YMRS [36], lifetime depression, hypo/mania spectrum symptomatology, mixed irritability and mixed impulsivity using the mood spectrum self-report questionnaire (MOODS-SR) [37], state/trait anxiety using the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAIY1/STAIY2) [38], and anhedonia using the Snaith–Hamilton Pleasure Scale (SHAPS) [39]. A total psychotropic medication load was calculated for each participant, with greater numbers and doses of medications corresponding to a greater medication load [29, 40]. Table 1 reports means, standard errors, and group statistics for participants’ demographic and clinical characteristics.

Table 1 Demographic and clinical characteristics.

Behavioral paradigm

The Cued Emotional N-back Task modeled after Manelis and Reder [7, 41] included a total of 48 blocks of 15 trials, each with 8 blocks per task condition: 1- and 2-back with fearful, neutral, and happy faces. Face stimuli were taken from the NimStim [42] and the Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces [43] databases, and were presented on the white background in the oval frame limiting the background (Fig. 1). The trials in a block were separated with 500-, 750-, or 1000-ms intertrial intervals randomly inserted between the trials. The blocks were separated with 6–7.5 s of rest. The n-back task required participants to determine whether the face currently presented on the screen matched the face presented n faces earlier in the sequence. Matching faces were called targets, and all other faces—nontargets. Participants responded by pressing a button with the index finger on one hand for targets and on the other hand for nontargets. A hand assignment was counterbalanced across participants.

Fig. 1: N-back task.
figure 1

An example of a 2-back block with neutral emotional faces.

Each block of trials was preceded with the 6-s cue screen containing information about the upcoming n-back condition (1-back/2-back) and a symbol indicating the emotional valence of upcoming stimuli [44] whose emotions were never intermixed within a block (Fig. 1). A straight line predicted a neutral emotional expression, a concave curve resembling a smile predicted a happy emotional expression, and a convex curve predicted fearful faces. During cue presentation, participants were instructed to anticipate the n-back condition and face emotion specified by the cue that always matched the task difficulty and emotional valence of the block of trials. Subjects underwent extensive practice outside the scanner to ensure high accuracy on the task inside the scanner.

Neuroimaging data acquisition

All neuroimaging data were collected at the Scientific Imaging and Brain Research Center at CMU using a Siemens Verio 3 T scanner with a 32-channel coil. The fMRI data were collected using a multiband sequence (factor = 4, TR = 1500 ms, resolution = 2 × 2 × 2 mm, 68 slices, FOV = 220, TE = 30 ms, and flip angle = 72°). High-resolution T1w images were collected using the MPRAGE sequence (TR = 2300 ms, resolution = 1 × 1 × 1 mm, 176 slices, FOV = 256, TE = 1.97 ms, and flip angle = 9°). Two spin-echo images were acquired with opposite phases (i.e., AP and PA) encoding directions (TR = 2240 ms, resolution = 2 × 2 × 2 mm, 68 slices, FOV = 220, TE = 76.4 ms, and flip angle = 90°) for distortion correction.

Data analyses

Clinical data analysis

The demographic and clinical characteristics were compared among groups using a one-way ANOVA or chi-square test, as appropriate. BD and MDD were compared using a t test. One missing trait anxiety score was imputed using the R package “mice” (https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/mice/mice.pdf).

Behavioral data analysis

A three-way ANOVA, with group (BD/MDD/HC) as a between-subject factor, and n-back (1-/2-back) and emotional conditions (happy/fearful/neutral faces) as within-subject factors, was conducted using the “sjstats” package in R (https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/sjstats/index.html).

Neuroimaging data analysis

Preprocessing

The quality of the neuroimaging data was examined using the MRIQC [45]. Data were preprocessed using fMRIPprep [46] (see Supplemental Materials). In short, the anatomical images (T1w) were corrected for intensity nonuniformity. Brain surfaces were reconstructed using recon-all (FreeSurfer6.0.1) [47]. Brain tissue segmentation of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), white matter (WM), and gray matter was performed on the brain-extracted T1w using FAST [48]. Susceptibility distortions of BOLD images were estimated based on two spin-echo images with opposing phase-encoding directions using 3dQwarp of AFNI [49]. The BOLD images were co-registered to the T1w images using bbregister [50] and motion corrected using MCFLIRT [51]. Automatic removal of motion artifacts was achieved using independent component analysis (ICA-AROMA) [52]. Further preprocessing included removing the effects of CSF, WM, and non-steady-state volumes and high-pass temporal filtering (90 s).

Subject-level analysis

The preprocessed and registered-to-MNI-space BOLD images served as an input to a subject-level analysis. A hemodynamic response was modeled using a gamma function. A subject-level general linear model included 13 explanatory variables: 2 levels of task difficulty (1-back/2-back), 3 emotional conditions (happy/neutral/fearful), 2 task conditions (task anticipation/task performance), and motor responses. The former 12 variables were modeled as blocks. The motor response was modeled as the last 200 ms of each valid trial. The 1-minus-2-back contrasts were computed for task anticipation and performance across all emotions.

Group-level analysis

Two group-level analyses (1-minus-2-back during task anticipation, and 1-minus-2-back during task performance) were conducted across all subjects and all emotional conditions in the whole brain using the randomise tool [53] for nonparametric permutation inference with 5000 permutations, Threshold-Free Cluster Enhancement correction and FWE-corrected p-values were <0.05. Subjects’ age, gender, and IQ were used in the models as covariates. A conjunction analysis was used to map the brain regions sensitive to anticipated task difficulty (1-back>2-back or 2-back>1-back during anticipation) on the working memory network (2-back>1-back during task performance). The percent signal changes were then extracted from the regions identified in the conjunction analysis for each subject for each n-back, emotion, and anticipation/task condition, and then ∆cue and ∆task (1-minus-2-back differences) were calculated for all these conditions. Functional localization was determined using the Harvard–Oxford cortical and subcortical structural atlases.

Multinomial regression with elastic-net regularization

The measures of ∆cue, ∆task, ∆cue*∆task interaction, ∆RT, ∆accuracy, ∆RT*∆accuracy interaction for the same emotional condition, and current depression and mania severity, lifetime depression, mania, instability and irritability, state and trait anxiety, anhedonia, and a total medication load were used as inputs to the multinomial (BD vs. MDD vs. HC) and binomial (BD vs. MDD) elastic-net-regularized regression models (the elastic-net penalty α = 0.5) implemented using the R package “glmnet” [31]. Elastic net is a regularization and variable selection method that combines the L1 norm (for model sparsity) and L2 norm (for model robustness) into a penalized model [31] used when the model has too many predictors relative to the number of observations. A lambda.1se computed across 5000 repetitions of the elastic-net algorithm with tenfold cross-validation was used to predict subjects’ diagnostic groups. The binomial elastic net to discriminate BD and MDD diagnoses was run on clinical and neuroimaging, clinical-only, and neuroimaging-only variables. Table 2 reports the model’s coefficients. Despite a progress in developing test statistics for other types of elastic-net regression [54, 55], procedures for calculating p-values for multinomial/binomial elastic nets are not yet available. In order to validate the elastic-net results and estimate a classification accuracy, we used the variables selected by elastic net in a random forest classification analysis using the R package “randomForest” [32] (https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/randomForest/randomForest.pdf) with ntrees = 10000. The significance was determined using the R package ‘rfUtilities’ (https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/rfUtilities/index.html) with ntrees = 10000 and 5000 permutations (see Supplemental Methods).

Table 2 Elastic-net regression coefficients for multinomial and binomial elastic regressions.

Results

Clinical

BD, MDD, and HC groups did not differ from each other in age, IQ, or gender composition (Table 1). Individuals with BD and MDD were significantly more symptomatic than HC (p < 0.05). Individuals with BD had more severe symptoms of depression, hypo/mania, mood instability and irritability lifetime, had more subthreshold symptoms of mania, and were significantly more depressed at scan (p < 0.05) than those with MDD.

Behavioral

A group × n-back × emotion ANOVA revealed the main effect of task difficulty on both accuracy (F(1,365) = 201.7, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.356) and RT (F(1,365)=118.9, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.346). Participants were slower and less accurate for 2-back vs. 1-back task (Fig. 2), thus confirming that the 2-back was more difficult than the 1-back task. Neither accuracy nor RT were affected by group, emotional condition, or interactions between group, task difficulty, and emotional condition (p-value > 0.05).

Fig. 2: Behavioral results.
figure 2

Accuracy and RT for 1- and 2-back tasks with fearful, happy, and neutral faces.

Neuroimaging: 1- vs. 2-back contrast across subjects and emotional conditions

Bilateral VLPFC, DLPFC, paracingulate/dACC, insula, cerebellum, left posterior parietal cortices, right thalamus, and right caudate regions showed greater activation during performance on 2-back vs. 1-back task across all emotional conditions, while central and parietal opercular, frontal polar, and posterior cingulate cortices showed greater activation during performance on 1-back vs. 2-back tasks (Supplementary Table S1; Fig. 3a, b).

Fig. 3: Neuroimaging and elastic-net regression results.
figure 3

a Red–yellow: 2-back>1-back for task performance; blue: 1-back>2-back for task performance; green: 1-back>2-back for task anticipation. b Activation for 1- and 2-back task anticipation and task performance. c Cross-validation plots for optimal lambda. d ∆cue for the regions and conditions identified by the elastic-net regression analysis. e The interaction effect between ∆cue and ∆task in the left DLPFC for fearful faces, and in the paracingulate gyrus for neutral faces.

The left VLPFC, left DLPFC, and paracingulate/dACC brain regions showed greater activation during 1- vs. 2-back task anticipation across all emotional conditions (Supplementary Table S1; Fig. 3a, b). These regions fully overlapped with the regions showing greater activation for 2- vs. 1-back during task performance. No cortical or subcortical region showed greater activation for 2-back vs. 1-back task during anticipation. The paracingulate/dACC region will be further referred as paracingulate as per Harvard–Oxford cortical structural atlas.

Elastic-net regression

Forty-six input variables (27 neuroimaging [∆cue, ∆task, and ∆cue*∆task interaction in the left VLPFC, left DLPFC, and paracingulate gyrus/dACC for fearful, happy, and neutral faces], nine behavioral [∆RT, ∆accuracy, and ∆RT*∆accuracy interaction for fearful, happy, and neutral faces], and ten clinical [measures of current depression, mania, and anhedonia, measures of lifetime depression, mania, mood instability, and irritability, and measures of state and trait anxiety and medication load]) were predictors in the multinomial elastic net.

Multinomial model

In the multinomial model, the diagnostic group membership (BD vs. MDD vs. HC) was best explained by 15 of 46 input variables (Table 2, Fig. 3c): all ten clinical variables, ∆cue and ∆cue*∆task interaction in the left DLPFC for fearful faces, ∆cue and ∆cue*∆task interaction in the paracingulate for neutral faces, and ∆cue in the left VLPFC for neutral faces.

Binomial models

Elastic net with neuroimaging, behavioral, and clinical variables as input selected 9 of 46 variables that best explained the BD vs. MDD differences (Table 2): the same five neuroimaging variables as described above and four clinical variables (current depression severity, lifetime mania, instability, and irritability). A random forest with these nine variables as predictors classified BD vs. MDD with 85.4% accuracy (p < 0.001).

Elastic net with clinical-only variables as input selected four of ten variables: current depression severity, lifetime mania, instability, and irritability. A random forest with these 4 variables as predictors classified BD vs. MDD with 80.5% accuracy (p = 0.001).

Elastic net with neuroimaging-only variables as input selected 7 of 27 variables: ∆task in left DLPFC for fearful faces, ∆task in the left DLPFC for neutral faces, and the 5 neuroimaging variables selected by the multinomial model (Table 2). A random forest with these 4 variables as predictors classified BD vs. MDD with 70.7% accuracy (p = 0.007).

Post hoc comparisons of the neuroimaging variables selected by multinomial/binomial elastic-net models

A one-way ANOVA on the neuroimaging variables identified by the elastic-net models revealed a significant effect of group on ∆cue for fearful faces in the left DLPFC (F(2,61) = 5.7, p = 0.005, Fig. 3d). A significantly greater decrease in the left DLPFC activation during anticipation of fearful faces in 2-back vs. 1-back conditions was observed in depressed individuals with MDD vs. those with BD (difference = 0.1, p-adjusted = 0.004 in the Tukey multiple comparison test), and a marginally greater decrease in MDD vs. HC (difference = 0.06, p-adjusted = 0.09). There was a marginally significant effect of group on ∆cue for neutral faces in the left VLPFC (F(2,61) = 2.8, p = 0.07) and paracingulate gyrus (F(2,61) = 2.6, p = 0.08, Fig. 3d). Individuals with BD showed a marginally greater decrease in activation in these regions during anticipation of neutral faces in 2-back vs. 1-back tasks compared with individuals with MDD (VLPFC: difference = 0.08, p-adjusted = 0.06; paracingulate: difference = 0.077, p-adjusted = 0.076).

The group differences among the regression slopes for ∆cue*∆task interaction effect in the left DLPFC for fearful faces and paracingulate gyrus for neutral faces (Fig. 3e) were examined using the R package “lsmeans” (https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lsmeans/lsmeans.pdf). A significant interaction was observed in DLPFC (F(2,58) = 3.86, p = 0.03) and paracingulate (F(2,58) = 4.4, p = 0.01). A post hoc test with p-values adjusted using Tukey method for comparing a family of 3 estimates showed significant difference in slopes for BD vs. MDD (DLPFC: BD (B = 0.4) > MDD (B = −0.24), t.ratio = 2.6, and p-adjusted = 0.03; paracingulate: BD (B = −0.57) < MDD (B = 0.32), t.ratio = −2.97, and p-adjusted = 0.01). Specifically, greater decreases in left DLPFC activation during anticipation of fearful faces for 2-back vs. 1-back task (positive values on the x-axis of Fig. 3e) were related to greater increases in the same region during performance on 2- vs. 1-back tasks (negative values on the y axis of Fig. 3e) in individuals with MDD, but an opposite pattern characterized individuals with BD. Greater decreases in paracingulate activation during anticipation of neutral faces for 2- vs. 1-back task were related to greater increases in the same region during performance on 2-back vs. 1-back tasks in individuals with BD, but an opposite activation pattern characterized individuals with MDD.

Discussion

Individuals with mood disorders often suffer from cognitive dysfunction [12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19] and aberrant anticipatory processing [25, 26]. Here we examined for the first time how the differences in brain activation during anticipation of and performance on an easy vs. difficult emotional working memory tasks can help distinguish depressed individuals with BD from those with MDD. Given the nature of the task, we focused our analyses on the conjunction between the working memory network (i.e., the regions showing greater activation for difficult vs. easy tasks) and the network of regions involved in anticipation of easy vs. difficult tasks. The results pertaining to the regions identified in the conjunction analysis suggest that activation patterns in lateral and medial PFC during anticipation of easy vs. difficult tasks maybe an important biological marker for BD vs. MDD classification. Both multinomial (BD vs. MDD vs. HC) and binomial (BD vs. MDD) elastic-net analyses showed that the 1-minus-2-back differences in the left DLPFC for fearful faces and the left VLPFC and paracingulate/dACC for neutral faces during task anticipation, as well as the interaction between these differences for task anticipation and task performance contributed to group classification. BD vs. MDD groups were classified with 85.4% accuracy based on clinical and neuroimaging variables selected by elastic net, with 80.5% accuracy based on clinical variables, and with 70.7% accuracy solely based on neuroimaging variables: the 1-minus-2-back differences in the left DLPFC, VLPFC, and paracingulate/dACC activation during anticipation of, performance on, and the interaction between anticipation and performance on working memory tasks with fearful and neutral faces. The fact that the direct comparisons between BD vs. HC or MDD vs. HC did not reach significance, but BD was significantly different from MDD, underscores the biologic differences between BD and MDD, pointing toward the need for different and individualized treatment approaches.

Brain activation during task anticipation is related to expected difficulty and the amount of effort in the task [7, 41, 56], and emotional valence of the stimuli [10, 44, 57]. The left VLPFC is involved in cognitive control, memory retrieval, resolution of proactive interference [58, 59], and updating of the task-related representations prior to task performance [60]. Paracingulate/dACC is a key region for conflict or interference monitoring [61], and may be involved with an effort calculation prior to task performance [56]. During task anticipation, these regions may work together to control neural and cognitive resource allocation to help achieve high-level performance on expected working memory. We found that individuals with BD who decreased paracingulate/dACC activation during anticipation of a difficult working memory task with neutral faces, increased it during task performance. In contrast, individuals with MDD who showed difficulty-related decreases in this region during task anticipation, failed to do so during task performance. Neutral faces are ambiguous for depressed individuals, and are often incorrectly judged as emotional [62, 63]. Anticipation of neutral faces might engage different neural mechanisms in BD and MDD. While the former might employ a “resource saving” strategy for a more difficult task with neural faces, the latter might need to maintain or even increase anticipatory activation in paracingulate/dACC to be able to activate this region during the task.

The DLPFC plays a critical role in emotion regulation, cognitive control, and planning [64,65,66]. In our study, individuals with MDD, compared with those with BD, had a significantly greater reduction in DLPFC activation for 2-back vs. 1-back anticipation of fearful faces, and had significantly more negative slope for the ∆cue*∆task interaction that did not differ from that in HC. The left DLPFC has been implicated in an early stage of threat processing [67], so failing to reduce activation in this region during anticipation of fearful faces in a difficult (2-back) working memory task might suggest aberrant anticipatory processing of negative stimuli in BD. This idea is consistent with our previous findings, suggesting that individuals with BD may “block” anticipation of negative stimuli to avoid negative emotions prior to task performance [29, 68]. Anticipating a stressful day in the morning may negatively affect cognitive functioning during the day [69], so “blocking out” potentially stressful or disturbing information before experiencing it may be a defense mechanism that depressed individuals with BD use to remain functional.

According to previous studies, the left VLPFC and dACC increase in activation for a more difficult, compared with an easy working memory task, but decrease in activation during anticipation of a more difficult, compared with an easy working memory task [7, 8]. It is noteworthy that despite using a different subject population, different type of stimuli, different fMRI acquisition sequences, and analysis pipelines, we have replicated both the brain activation patterns for easy vs. difficult working memory task anticipation and performance, and the specific brain regions showing this pattern of activation [7]. This replication is critically important because it confirms the validity, reliability, and generalizability for the role of these regions during working memory task anticipation.

One limitation of our study is a relatively small sample size that did not allow us to examine separately or compare individuals with BD-I and BD-II. It is possible that the results were driven primarily by the BD-II as they comprised 80% of our BD sample. Our supplementary results, where we directly compared BD-II and MDD, provide additional support for this hypothesis (Supplementary Table S2). Another limitation is use of medications by some individuals with BD and MDD. However, it is important to note that a medication load was not different between BD and MDD groups, and did not contribute to BD vs. MDD classification.

In summary, our findings contribute to the body of literature, highlighting the role of lateral and medial PFC regions in distinguishing BD from MDD [2, 21, 24, 27]. We showed that the 1-back vs. 2-back activation differences in these regions during anticipation of fearful and neutral faces distinguished BD from MDD when participants were depressed and thus especially difficult to diagnose. This brings anticipatory processing into focus, and suggests that anticipatory brain activation preceding performance on working memory tasks may be an important biological marker of BD and MDD. Differential aberrant anticipatory processing in individuals with BD and MDD might reflect the differences in mental strategies used to prepare to process fearful and neutral faces when neurocognitive resources are limited (as in a difficult working memory task) through changing the neural resource allocation prior to task performance. Targeting anticipatory processing may be a promising direction in developing therapies for BD and MDD. Future research should aim to understand how modulating PFC activation during mental preparation for difficult working memory tasks improves cognitive functioning in depressed individuals with BD and those with MDD.

Funding and disclosure

This work was supported by a grant from the National Institute of Health K01MH104348 to A.M. A.M. and S.I. declare no conflict of interest. H.A.S. receives royalties from Wolters Kluwer, a grant from Myriad Genetics, and an editorial stipend from APA Press. M.L.P. received one honorarium from Sunovion Pharmaceuticals in the last 3 years.