INTRODUCTION

Social anxiety disorder (SAD), often referred to as social phobia, is characterized by persistent fear and avoidance of social situations. Epidemiologically, SAD is the third most common psychiatric disorder, with a 12-month and lifetime prevalence of 6.8% and 12.1%, respectively (Kessler et al, 2005a, 2005b). For diagnostic purposes, SAD has been divided in two subtypes: specific and generalized SAD. The specific form refers to the fear and avoidance of a particular social situation and includes performance anxiety (eg, fear of giving a public speech), interaction anxiety (fear of social interaction and observation situations), and fear of showing anxiety symptoms (Bögels et al, 2010). Patients with generalized SAD are more impaired as they fear and avoid a wide range of social situations (den Boer, 1997; Kessler et al, 1998; Ruipérez et al, 2002). This avoidant behavior has an important role in the maintenance of SAD and prevents the reversal of fear in social situations (American Psychiatric Association, 1994; Stangier et al, 2006).

At present, SAD treatment consists of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) (Gould et al, 1997; Fedoroff and Taylor, 2001), which leads to gradual fear extinction, ie, a decline in the fear response as a result of repeated exposure to the feared situation, and is often combined with medication originally designed for depression or generalized anxiety, such as antidepressants, β-blockers, and benzodiazepines. However, a high percentage of SAD patients fail to respond to the available treatment options, or achieve only partial remission of symptoms, with antidepressants providing the best response rates (Liebowitz et al, 1992; Baldwin et al, 1999; Van Ameringen et al, 2001). Given the high prevalence and unsatisfactory treatment options for SAD, a better understanding of the etiology and underlying neurobiological mechanisms of social fear, particularly extinction of social fear, is urgently needed. This, in turn, might provide important information for the development of more specific medication and an improved treatment outcome for SAD patients.

However, there are currently no appropriate animal models available to study the disorder. Social anxiety/avoidance is presently induced using a number of paradigms, including the social defeat paradigm- used both acutely and chronically (for reviews see Huhman, 2006; Yan et al, 2010), and foot-shock exposure (Haller and Bakos, 2002; Louvart et al, 2005; Mikics et al, 2008a). However, these paradigms are rather unspecific with respect to the behavioral alterations they induce, as increased general anxiety, depression, and impaired locomotion were found to accompany the social avoidance (Denmark et al, 2010; Hollis et al, 2010).

Therefore, we aimed to establish a novel and specific animal model of SAD using the social fear conditioning (SFC) paradigm, and use this model to assess the therapeutic efficacy of diazepam and paroxetine, currently used for SAD patients. The SAD-like phenotype was induced in naïve mice by punishing them when investigating an unfamiliar con-specific. Mice were conditioned to associate a shock-induced pain with the investigation of a social stimulus and, therefore, avoid social stimuli. The conditioned social fear is specific to several social stimuli, long-lasting and not accompanied by changes in general anxiety, depressive-related behavior, and locomotion. The social fearful phenotype was dose-dependently reversed by acute diazepam, at a dose that was not anxiolytic in a non-social context, ie, the elevated plus maze (EPM). Furthermore, chronic antidepressant treatment also reversed social fear, validating the SFC model.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

Male CD1 mice (Charles River, Sulzfeld, Germany) weighing 30–35 g were individually housed in polycarbonate cages (16 × 22 × 14 cm) for 1 week before experiments started, and remained isolated throughout. Isolation was shown to increase social motivation (Niesink and Van Ree, 1982) and prevent the attenuation of behavioral effects of stressors observed in group-housed mice (Ruis et al, 1999; Cherng et al, 2010). Mice were transferred to observation cages (30 × 23 × 36 cm) 3 days before experiments started. Age- and weight-matched male CD1 mice were used as social stimuli.

Mice were maintained under standard laboratory conditions (12:12 light/dark cycle, lights on at 0600 hours, 22°C, 60% humidity, food and water ad libitum). Experiments were performed during the light phase, between 0800 and 1200 hours, in accordance with the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the Government of Oberpfalz and the guidelines of the NIH.

SFC Paradigm

On day 1, mice were conditioned for social fear, whereas on days 2 and 3 or 15 and 16, social investigation was assessed as readout of short- and long-term social fear and fear extinction.

SFC was performed with a computerized fear conditioning system (TSE System GmbH, Bad Homburg, Germany). The conditioning chamber consisted of a transparent Perspex box (45 × 22 × 40 cm) enclosed in a wooden chamber to reduce external noise and visual stimulation. The floor consisted of a removable stainless steel grid connected to a shock delivery unit used for manual application of foot shocks. A video camera at the top of the chamber enabled video recording.

SFC (day 1)

Mice were placed in the conditioning chamber and, after a 30-s adaptation period, an empty wire mesh cage (7 × 7 × 6 cm) was placed as a non-social stimulus near one of the short walls. Mice were allowed to investigate the non-social stimulus for 3 min, before it was replaced by an identical cage containing an unfamiliar male mouse. Unconditioned mice were allowed to investigate the social stimulus for 3 min. Conditioned mice were given a 1-s electric foot shock (0.7 mA, pulsed current) each time they investigated the social stimulus, defined by direct contact with the mouse. Mice received between 2 and 5 foot shocks, with a variable inter-shock interval, depending on when direct social contact was made. The first social contact and foot shock occurred within 15–30 s. Mice were returned to their home cage when no further social contact was made for 2 min, meaning that conditioned mice spent between 3 and 6 min in the conditioning chamber while the social stimulus was present. The time mice spent investigating the non-social stimulus, as a pre-conditioning measure of non-social anxiety, was analyzed using the JWatcher program (V 1.0, Macquarie University and UCLA).

Extinction (day 2 or 15)

To investigate whether conditioned mice displayed social fear and whether this fear could be extinguished, social investigation was assessed in the home cage 1 or 15 days after SFC for short-term and long-term social fear, respectively. In detail, extinction consisted of exposing the mice to three empty cages identical to the cage used during day 1 (non-social stimuli) to assess non-social investigation. Mice were then exposed to six unfamiliar male mice enclosed in wire mesh cages (social stimuli) to assess social investigation. Each stimulus was placed near a short wall of the home cage and presented for 3 min, with a 3-min inter-exposure interval. Reduced social investigation and aversive responses toward the social stimuli, such as freezing, stretched approaches, and defensive burying indicated social fear and successful conditioning of social fear. As the empty cage elicited a fear response in conditioned mice, an empty cage was placed over night in the home cage to extinguish the fear of the cage.

Extinction recall (day 3 or 16)

To investigate whether repeated exposure to social stimuli during extinction leads to a complete reversal of social fear, social investigation was assessed in the home cage 1 day after extinction. Extinction recall consisted of exposing the mice to six unfamiliar social stimuli for 3 min, with a 3-min inter-exposure interval.

Specificity of the Induced Social Fear

One day after SFC, we performed the EPM and the forced swim test (FST) to assess general anxiety-related and depressive-like behavior, respectively (Lister, 1987; Pellow et al, 1985; Slattery et al, 2005). Further, we assessed home cage locomotor activity as described previously (Reber et al, 2007; Slattery et al, 2011; for experimental details see Supplementary Methods). Moreover, we assessed the effects of SFC on novel object investigation, and the effects of foot shocks on social fear.

Novel object investigation

Novel object and social investigation were assessed in the home cage 1 day after SFC to differentiate between fear of novelty and social fear. Mice were exposed to three non-social stimuli, three cages containing a white ball (novel object stimuli, size and color matched to the social stimuli), and three unfamiliar social stimuli. Each stimulus was presented for 3 min, with a 3-min inter-exposure interval.

Electric foot-shock exposure

To assess the effects of foot-shock exposure in the absence of a social stimulus on social fear, mice were placed in the empty conditioning chamber and, after a 30-s adaptation period, received five electric foot shocks (1 s, 0.7 mA, pulsed current, ie, the maximum number received during SFC), with a 2-min inter-shock interval. Mice were returned to their home cage 2 min after the last foot shock. An empty cage was placed in their home cage over night to allow for comparable behavioral effects with SFC. One day later, social investigation was assessed during extinction.

Drugs

Diazepam (Ratiopharm GmbH, Germany) was freshly dissolved in saline and administered intraperitoneally (i.p.) at a volume of 5 ml/kg and doses between 0.5 and 1.25 mg/kg. The highest doses were chosen based on previous studies (Corbett et al, 1993; Dalvi and Rodgers, 1996; Stachowicz et al. 2008).

Paroxetine (Bayer Schering, Germany) was administered over 14 days via the drinking water at a dose of 10 mg/kg/day. The paroxetine dose was chosen based on previous studies (Da-Rocha et al, 1997; Hascoët et al, 2000a, 2000b; Massé et al, 2005; Elizalde et al, 2008; Thoeringer et al, 2010).

Experimental design

Effects of SFC on Short- and Long-Term Social Fear

Initial experiments were designed to characterize the effects of SFC on short- and long-term social fear. Therefore, separate groups of mice were subjected to SFC and social investigation was assessed 1 or 15 days later during extinction (n=13 per group for short- and n=9 per group for long-term effects). Extinction recall was measured 1 day later.

Specificity of the Induced Social Fear

To verify the specificity of the induced social fear, separate groups of mice were subjected to general anxiety (EPM; n=8 per group), depressive-like behavior (FST; n=8 per group), home cage locomotion (n=7 per group), or novel object investigation (n=8 per group) testing 1 day after SFC. Another group of mice was exposed to electric foot shocks in the absence of the social stimulus, and social investigation was assessed 1 day later during extinction (n=8 per group).

Reversal of Short- and Long-Term Social Fear by Acute Diazepam and Chronic Paroxetine Treatment, Respectively

To determine whether the effects of SFC on short- and long-term social fear could be reversed by medication used for SAD patients, we assessed the effects of diazepam and paroxetine, respectively. Mice (n=10 per group) were subjected to SFC. The following day, 30 min before extinction, mice were injected i.p. either with vehicle (5 ml/kg saline) or with diazepam (0.5, 0.75, 1.0, and 1.25 mg/kg). Extinction recall was measured 1 day later. To determine whether diazepam has anxiolytic effects in a non-social context at doses used to reverse social fear, naïve mice were injected i.p. either with vehicle (5 ml/kg saline) or with diazepam (0.5, 0.75, 1.0, and 1.25 mg/kg) 30 min before EPM testing. In a separate group of mice (n=8 per group), social investigation was assessed 15 days after SFC. Paroxetine (10 mg/kg/day) was administered chronically via the drinking water over 14 days, starting 1 day after SFC to prevent possible confounding effects on fear memory consolidation. Extinction recall was measured 1 day later.

Statistical Analysis

For statistical analysis PASW/SPSS (Version 17) was used. Data were analyzed by Student's t-tests, one-way or two-way ANOVA for repeated measures, followed by a Bonferroni's post-hoc analysis whenever appropriate. Statistical significance was set at p<0.05. Overall statistics are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Overall Effects for the SFC Data

RESULTS

Effects of SFC on Short- and Long-Term Social Fear

Short-term social fear

Mice showed similar pre-conditioning non-social anxiety (Figure 1a, Table 1). One day after SFC, conditioned mice showed similar non-social investigation, but reduced social investigation (p<0.05, Figure 1b) compared with unconditioned mice. No difference between the mice was found during extinction recall (Figure 1c).

Figure 1
figure 1

Social fear conditioning (SFC) induces short-term social fear in mice. (a) Pre-conditioning investigation of the non-social stimulus (empty cage) by unconditioned (uc) and conditioned (c) mice during SFC (day 1; n=13 per group). (b) Investigation of non-social (ns1–ns3) and social (cages with mice; s1–s6) stimuli during extinction on day 2 (3-min exposure to stimulus, 3-min inter-exposure interval). (c) Investigation of social stimuli (s1–s6) during extinction recall on day 3. Data represent mean percentage of investigation time±SEM. *p<0.05 vs uc mice.

PowerPoint slide

Long-term social fear

Mice showed similar pre-conditioning non-social anxiety (Figure 2a, Table 1). Fifteen days after SFC, conditioned mice showed similar non-social investigation, but reduced social investigation (p<0.05;. Figure 2b) compared with unconditioned mice. In contrast to short-term social fear, conditioned mice still showed reduced social investigation during extinction recall (p<0.05, Figure 2c).

Figure 2
figure 2

Social fear conditioning (SFC) induces long-term social fear in mice. (a) Pre-conditioning investigation of the non-social stimulus (empty cage) by unconditioned (uc) and conditioned (c) mice during SFC (day 1; n=9 per group). (b) Investigation of non-social (ns1–ns3) and social (cages with mice; s1–s6) stimuli during extinction on day 15 (3-min exposure to stimulus, 3-min inter-exposure interval). (c) Investigation of social stimuli (s1–s6) during extinction recall on day 16. Data represent mean percentage of investigation time±SEM. *p<0.05 vs uc mice.

PowerPoint slide

Specificity of the Induced Social Fear

No effect of SFC on general anxiety, depressive-like behavior, and home cage locomotion

Conditioned mice showed no changes in general anxiety (percentage of time on open arms; Figure 3a, Table 1) or locomotion on the EPM (number of closed arm entries; Figure 3c), in depressive-like behavior in the FST (percentage immobility; Figure 3b), or in home cage locomotion (distance moved; Figure 3d) compared with unconditioned mice 1 day after SFC.

Figure 3
figure 3

No effect of social fear conditioning (SFC) on general anxiety on the elevated plus maze (EPM) (a), depressive-like behavior in the forced swim test (b), and locomotor activity on the EPM (c) and in the home cage (d). Data represent means±SEM for n=7 to 8 mice (separate groups). c, conditioned; uc, unconditioned.

PowerPoint slide

No effect of SFC on fear of novelty

Mice showed similar pre-conditioning non-social anxiety (Figure 4a, Table 1). One day after SFC, conditioned mice showed similar non-social and novel object investigation, but reduced social investigation (p<0.01; Figure 4b) compared with unconditioned mice.

Figure 4
figure 4

Social fear conditioning (SFC) does not induce fear of novelty. (a) Pre-conditioning investigation of the non-social stimulus (empty cage) by unconditioned (uc) and conditioned (c) mice during SFC (day 1; n=8 per group). (b) Investigation of non-social stimuli (ns1–ns3), novel object stimuli (cages with objects; no1–no3), and social stimuli (cages with mice; s1–s6) during novel object investigation on day 2 (3-min exposure to stimulus, 3-min inter-exposure interval). Data represent mean percentage of investigation time±SEM. *p<0.05 vs uc mice.

PowerPoint slide

No effect of foot-shock exposure on social fear

Exposure to foot shocks in the absence of the social stimulus did not alter social investigation 1 day later (Figure 5, Table 1).

Figure 5
figure 5

Exposure to five electric foot shocks in the absence of a social stimulus does not induce social fear. Investigation of non-social (empty cages; ns1–ns3) and social (cages with mice; s1–s6) stimuli by non-shocked and shocked mice (n=8 per group) during extinction 1 day after foot-shock exposure (3-min exposure to stimulus, 3-min inter-exposure interval). Data represent mean percentage of investigation time±SEM.

PowerPoint slide

Reversal of Short- and Long-Term Social Fear by Acute Diazepam and Chronic Paroxetine Treatment, Respectively

Dose-dependent reversal of short-term social fear by diazepam

Acute diazepam dose-dependently reversed social fear in conditioned mice (F(4, 44)=5.164, p=0.022; Table 2), with doses > 0.5 mg/kg being unsuccessful in reversing social fear because of their sedative properties (see Table 2 for details). However, these higher doses were not sedative on the EPM and caused a dose-dependent anxiolysis (see Table 2). Mice showed similar pre-conditioning non-social anxiety before treatment (Figure 6a, Table 1). Conditioned mice received a similar number of foot shocks during SFC (vehicle 2.3±0.15 vs 0.5 mg/kg diazepam 2.2±0.19; T(18)=0.42, n.s.). One day later, the four groups showed similar non-social investigation. Although in conditioned diazepam-treated mice social investigation returned to levels found in unconditioned mice, conditioned vehicle-treated mice showed reduced social investigation compared with all other groups (p<0.05, Figure 6b). No difference between the mice was found during extinction recall (Figure 6c).

Table 2 Dose-Dependent Effects of Diazepam in the Social Fear Conditioning Paradigm and the Elevated Plus Maze (EPM)
Figure 6
figure 6

Acute diazepam treatment reverses short-term social fear. (a) Pre-conditioning investigation of the non-social stimulus (empty cage) during social fear conditioning (day 1; n=10 per group). (b) Investigation of non-social (ns1–ns3) and social (cages with mice; s1–s6) stimuli during extinction on day 2 (3-min exposure to stimulus, 3-min inter-exposure interval). Unconditioned (uc) and conditioned (c) mice were injected intraperitoneally either with vehicle (Veh; 5 ml/kg saline) or with diazepam (Dia; 0.5 mg/kg) 30 min before extinction. (c) Investigation of social stimuli (s1–s6) during extinction recall on day 3. Data represent mean percentage of investigation time±SEM. *p<0.05 vs the other three groups.

PowerPoint slide

Reversal of long-term social fear by paroxetine

Mice showed similar pre-conditioning non-social anxiety before treatment (Figure 7a, Table 1). Conditioned mice received a similar number of foot shocks during SFC (vehicle 2.38±0.18 vs paroxetine 2.31±0.16; T(14)=0.26, n.s.). After 15 days, the four groups showed similar non-social investigation. Although in conditioned paroxetine-treated mice social investigation returned to levels found in unconditioned mice, conditioned vehicle-treated mice showed reduced social investigation compared with all other groups (p<0.05, Figure 7b). During extinction recall, conditioned vehicle-treated mice still showed reduced social investigation compared with all other groups (p<0.05, Figure 7c).

Figure 7
figure 7

Chronic paroxetine treatment reverses long-term social fear. (a) Pre-conditioning investigation of the non-social stimulus (empty cage) during social fear conditioning (SFC; day 1; n=8 per group). Paroxetine (Par) was administered over 14 days in the drinking water (Veh) of unconditioned (uc) and conditioned (c) mice starting 1 day after SFC. (b) Investigation of non-social (ns1–ns3) and social (cages with mice; s1–s6) stimuli during extinction on day 15 (3-min exposure to stimulus, 3-min inter-exposure interval). (c) Investigation of social stimuli (s1–s6) during extinction recall on day 16. Data represent mean percentage of investigation time±SEM. *p<0.05 vs the other three groups.

PowerPoint slide

DISCUSSION

To the best of our knowledge, this study describes the first animal model of SAD that specifically induces social anxiety without potentially confounding alterations in other behavioral measures. We show that the novel SFC model induces both short- and long-term specific fear of social stimuli, and that this fear sensitizes over time. Furthermore, SFC does not induce other behavioral changes that might account for the observed social fear, such as fear of novelty, increased general anxiety, depressive-like behavior, and impaired locomotion. We further show that social fear is dose-dependently reversed by acute diazepam; at a dose that is not anxiolytic in a non-social context. Finally, we show that chronic paroxetine treatment reverses social fear, similar to the best outcomes in SAD patients, validating the SFC model. Therefore, the SFC model represents a unique and novel model to gain a better understanding of the underlying etiology of SAD and to test compounds with novel mechanisms of action that could provide better treatment outcome for patients.

Despite its prevalence and symptom severity, the etiology of SAD remains poorly understood, due in part to a lack of appropriate animal models. Currently, lasting social anxiety/avoidance in both rats and mice is induced by two main traumatic stress procedures, namely social defeat and foot-shock exposure. Social defeat is used both acutely- one defeat by a dominant male and chronically- repeated defeat by several dominant males (for reviews see Huhman, 2006; Yan et al, 2010). Foot-shock exposure is used as exposure to a single (Short and Maier, 1993; Siegmund and Wotjak, 2007) or to repeated foot shocks (Haller and Bakos, 2002; Louvart et al, 2005; Mikics et al, 2008a). Although social defeat and foot-shock exposure decrease social investigation, they also lead to behavioral alterations including increased general anxiety, depression, and impaired locomotion that might account for the observed social deficit (Denmark et al, 2010; Hollis et al, 2010). Furthermore, in the case of acute social defeat, the induced social avoidance is generally directed toward the con-specific that performed the defeat (Lai et al, 2005; Lukas et al, 2011). Although such models are useful and have improved our understanding of SAD, there is a need for animal models that lead to specific social fear, without any confounding behavioral alterations, to further elucidate the mechanisms underlying SAD.

Our SFC model is based on operant conditioning, where animals learn to associate a voluntary behavior with its consequences. When the consequence is favorable the behavior will occur more frequently, whereas when the consequence is unfavorable the behavior will occur less frequently (Thorndike, 1933; White, 1989). SFC implies punishing naïve mice when investigating a con-specific and results in fear and avoidance of social stimuli. The social fearful phenotype is expressed in reduced investigation of social stimuli and intense aversive responses toward them, such as freezing (absence of movement except that required for respiration, Fanselow, 1980), stretched approaches, and defensive burying (Table 3). Although all these behaviors have been linked with increased anxiety and fear, the reduced social investigation was the most robust indicator of social fear in our paradigm and, therefore, the one we focused on. Furthermore, as none of the unconditioned mice showed freezing, stretched approaches, or defensive burying, by using social investigation as the main readout of social fear direct comparison with unconditioned mice is possible.

Table 3 Behavioral Changes Indicating Social Fear After Social Fear Conditioning

To evaluate the effects of SFC on social fear, we used a modified version of the social approach/avoidance paradigm (Berton et al, 2006), where we first exposed mice to non-social stimuli, ie, empty cages to exclude possible confounding effects due to fear of the cage itself. As the stimulus mouse used during SFC was enclosed in a cage that was identical to the cages used as non-social stimuli during extinction, the cage additionally served as a cue and, therefore, elicited a fear response in conditioned mice (Table 4). However, this fear was extinguished by placing the empty cage in the home cage of the mice over night. As non-social investigation was not decreased in conditioned mice after extinguishing the fear of the cage, it is unlikely that fear renewal to the cage occurred and thereby decreased social investigation (Table 4). Furthermore, exposure to non-social stimuli during extinction did not affect the level of social investigation in either conditioned or unconditioned mice (Table 4). Therefore, to allow for a within-individual assessment of non-social and social fear, mice were exposed to both non-social and social stimuli throughout the experiments. For assessing social investigation, mice were repeatedly exposed to different unfamiliar con-specifics. Different individuals were used for two reasons. First, we aimed to obtain a reversal of social fear in general and not a reversal of fear toward a specific individual. Second, we aimed to maintain high interest in the social stimuli as repeated exposure to the same con-specific has been shown to decrease social interest in both mice (Ferguson et al, 2002; Choleris et al, 2009) and rats (Thor et al, 1982; Popik and van Ree, 1998). This exposure-like paradigm is similar to exposure therapy during CBT in humans, where patients are repeatedly exposed to the feared situation.

Table 4 Investigation of the Non-Social (ns1–ns3) and Social Stimuli (s1–s6) During Extinction in Relation to Over Night Exposure to the Empty Cage and Exposure to Non-Social Stimuli During Extinction

SFC induced both short- and long-term specific fear of social stimuli. Social investigation increased with each exposure to the social stimuli, indicating gradual social fear extinction, which is similar to the outcome during CBT in SAD patients (Clark et al, 2003), and extinction after cued and context fear conditioning in mice and rats (Myers and Davis, 2002). When the extinction procedure was performed 1 day after SFC, social fear was completely reversed by the next day, during extinction recall. However, when the extinction was performed 15 days after SFC, conditioned mice still showed social fear during extinction recall, indicating that the social fear sensitized over time. Previous studies also showed sensitization of fear responses over time after single or repeated foot-shock exposure (Siegmund and Wotjak, 2007; Mikics et al, 2008a), suggesting that the circuitry underlying the conditioned social fear not only remains stable, but may even strengthen over time. Moreover, the maintenance of social fear over both the short- and long-term not only offers the possibility to test medication with fast onset of action, such as benzodiazepines, but also with a delayed onset of action, such as antidepressants (Katz et al, 2006; Mitchell, 2006; Priest, 2006).

Unlike acute social defeat, SFC induced a general fear of social stimuli, which was not limited to the stimulus to which the mouse has been conditioned, but extended to unfamiliar social stimuli as well. This general social fear is similar to the general social avoidance induced by chronic social defeat (Avgustinovich et al, 2005; Berton et al, 2006), where several dominant males are consecutively used to defeat a subordinate animal (Miczek, 1979; Rodgers and Randall 1986; Kabbaj et al, 2001). However, chronic social defeat also increased general anxiety (Keeney and Hogg, 1999; Avgustinovich et al, 2005; Berton et al, 2006; Denmark et al, 2010), decreased locomotor activity (Koolhaas et al, 1997; Rygula et al, 2005), and induced a depressive-like phenotype (Avgustinovich et al, 2005; Rygula et al, 2005; Berton et al, 2006; Hollis et al, 2010). Correspondingly, these behavioral changes might account for the decreased social investigation observed. Unlike chronic social defeat, SFC induced a specific fear of social stimuli without inducing changes in general anxiety on the EPM, depressive-like behavior in the FST, or in home cage and novel environment (EPM) locomotion. We could also show that the induced social fear was specific to a social stimulus, as novel objects that were similar in size and color to the social stimuli did not induce fear responses in conditioned mice. Thus, in future studies it would be interesting to compare treatments that are effective in the SFC paradigm in such social defeat models to determine their specificity.

The SFC model involves the use of aversive stimuli, in this case electric foot shocks. Previous studies have shown that foot-shock exposure alone decreased social investigation for both short- and long-term, however, only when assessed in a novel environment (Short and Maier, 1993; Haller and Bakos, 2002; Haller et al, 2003; Louvart et al, 2005; Leveleki et al, 2006; Siegmund and Wotjak, 2007; Mikics et al, 2008a, 2008b). When social investigation was assessed in the home cage, no differences were found, suggesting that the former result is due to novelty-induced anxiety (Mikics et al, 2008a). Shock exposure also increased general anxiety and decreased locomotion and exploratory behavior in the open field and EPM (Van Dijken et al, 1992; Bruijnzeel et al, 2001; Pijlman and van Ree, 2002; Kavushansky et al, 2009), indicating that the decreased social investigation observed after foot-shock exposure may rather be due to an increase in general anxiety. Our paradigm assessed the experimental mice in their home cage, which reduces the possibility of novelty-induced anxiety. Moreover, we did not observe changes in home cage social investigation when mice were exposed to foot shocks in the absence of the stimulus mouse, indicating that the fear induced during SFC is a result of the association between the experienced pain and investigation of a con-specific rather than a direct result of foot-shock exposure.

Having shown the specificity of the fear induced during SFC, we assessed the therapeutic efficacy of benzodiazepines (diazepam) and antidepressants (paroxetine) in reversing social fear in our model. Although β-blockers can be beneficial in humans, their efficacy is limited to performance anxiety (Faigel, 1991; Liebowitz et al, 1992), therefore we did not assess them in the SFC paradigm.

Acute diazepam treatment dose-dependently reversed short-term social fear, without further increasing social investigation in unconditioned mice. Doses >0.5 mg/kg (0.75, 1.0, and 1.25 mg/kg) had sedative effects in the home cage and even reduced investigation in unconditioned mice, counter indicating, therefore, their use in the SFC paradigm (Table 2). Diazepam reversed social fear at a dose (0.5 mg/kg) that did not alter general anxiety on the EPM (Table 2). We could only demonstrate anxiolytic effects of diazepam on the EPM at a dose of 1.25 mg/kg, confirming recent findings (Hascoët et al, 2000a). The sedative effect of doses >0.5 mg/kg diazepam might not have been observed in previous studies, as the increased arousal level in novel environments, such as the EPM, open field, and light-dark box, may have masked the sedative effect of diazepam.

Chronic paroxetine treatment, started 1 day after SFC to prevent possible confounding effects on fear memory consolidation, was also successful in reversing long-term social fear, without further increasing social investigation in unconditioned mice. However, acute SSRI treatment increased social fear in conditioned mice (data not shown). This is in line with data showing that SSRI treatment reduces cued fear after chronic treatment, but increases cued fear after acute treatment (Burghardt et al, 2004, 2007). All in all, the reversal of social fear by medication used for SAD patients provides predictive validity to the SFC model.

Finally, it is of interest to note that the use of the SFC model is not restricted to male CD1 mice, as shown in this study. Preliminary data have shown that the paradigm can also be used successfully in inbred lines such as C57/Bl6, and in a different species, namely Wistar rats (data not shown). This further demonstrates the utility of the novel paradigm for gaining a better understanding of the etiology of SAD. Moreover, SFC has not been used to induce social fear in female mice or rats yet, which, however, would be promising given the higher prevalence of SAD in women (Schneier et al, 1992; Talepasand and Nokani 2010).

In summary, we have established a novel SFC paradigm that induces specific and long-lasting fear of social stimuli in naïve mice, and shows both face and predictive validity to SAD. The induced social fear is specific to several social stimuli and not the result of fear of novelty, increased general anxiety, depressive-like behavior, or impaired locomotion. Our model might, therefore, be used to gain a better understanding of the underlying causes and mechanisms of SAD in humans and also to test compounds with novel mechanisms of action that could provide better treatment outcome for these patients.