INTRODUCTION

Anxiety disorders are among the most commonly diagnosed psychiatric disorders, with an estimated prevalence of 18% in the United States (Kessler et al, 2005). The most frequently prescribed pharmacologic agents for treating anxiety are benzodiazepines and selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), which are effective for 40–80% of patients depending on the disorder and the specific study (Asnis et al, 2001; Bandelow et al, 2004; Davidson et al, 1993, 2004; Hoffman and Mathew, 2008; Jorstad-Stein and Heimberg, 2009; Kasper et al, 2005; Laakmann et al, 1998; Mitte et al, 2005; Moroz and Rosenbaum, 1999; Pull and Damsa, 2008; Stein et al, 1999; Van Ameringen et al, 2009). Clinicians are often hesitant to prescribe benzodiazepines because of concerns about long-term efficacy, side effects (including sedation and cognitive impairment), and potential risks for abuse or dependence (Cloos and Ferreira, 2009; McIntosh et al, 2004). Owing to their efficacy and reduced abuse liability, SSRIs are often considered first line pharmacologic treatment for anxiety (McIntosh et al, 2004). However, SSRIs are limited because of having a relatively slow onset of effects (eg, 2–4 weeks) and side effects such as sexual dysfunction or weight gain that can be of particular concern to patients (Baldwin, 2008). Considering these issues, there remains considerable room to improve both the effectiveness and side effect profile of pharmacologic anxiety treatment.

Pregabalin (PGB), currently marketed in the United States for treatment of fibromyalgia and neuropathic pain, has also shown promise as an anxiolytic. Research indicates PGB's primary mechanism of action involves binding at the α2δ subunit of voltage-dependent calcium channels, subsequently leading to upregulation of GABA inhibitory activity and a reduction in the release of several neurotransmitters, including glutamate, noradrenaline, and substance P (Brawek et al, 2008; Dooley et al, 2007; Fink et al, 2002; Gee et al, 1996). Daily doses of 100–600 mg PGB have been shown to significantly reduce anxiety symptoms for 45–60% of generalized and social anxiety disorder patients (Feltner et al, 2003, 2008; Pande et al, 2003, 2004; Pohl et al, 2005; Rickels et al, 2005). Similar to benzodiazepines, improvement is observed within 1 week following PGB treatment initiation (Feltner et al, 2003; Pande et al, 2003, 2004; Pohl et al, 2005; Rickels et al, 2005). Additionally, PGB shows less of an effect on cognitive function and sedation than alprazolam (Hindmarch et al, 2005) and discontinuation rates because of adverse events have been lower for PGB (5–20%) than benzodiazepines (15–35%) (Feltner et al, 2003; Lydiard et al, 2010; Rickels et al, 2005).

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) can be used to delineate regional neural mechanisms of pharmacologic treatments and has the potential to serve as a biomarker for predicting anxiolytic function for current and novel agents (Paulus and Stein, 2007). fMRI studies with healthy controls report acute benzodiazepine administration to attenuate activation of insula, amygdala, and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) during paradigms involving processing of emotional faces (Paulus et al, 2005), anticipation of painful stimuli (Wise et al, 2007), and punishment during risk-based decision making (Arce et al, 2006). Sub-chronic SSRI administration (eg, 3–30 days) has been associated with decreased amygdala, insula, and/or mPFC and anterior cingulate (ACC) activation during anticipation (Simmons et al, 2009) and processing of aversive pictures (Takahashi et al, 2005) and emotional faces (Anderson et al, 2007; Arce et al, 2008; Del-Ben et al, 2005; Harmer et al, 2006; Windischberger et al, 2010). Therefore, despite unique mechanisms of action, both benzodiazepines and SSRIs may exert anxiolytic influence through a common pathway of decreasing neural responses in regions involved in emotional processing and interoceptive awareness (ie, awareness of internal bodily state).

Anticipatory anxiety is thought to be a cardinal symptom of anxiety disorders (Craske et al, 2009) and anticipation paradigms are sensitive to insula, amygdala, and PFC activation (Nitschke et al, 2006; Simmons et al, 2004) as well as to pharmacologic effects of benzodiazepines and SSRIs (Simmons et al, 2009; Wise et al, 2007). The primary aim of this study was to delineate potential neuroanatomical mechanisms of action for PGB during anticipatory processing. On the basis of the findings regarding the effects of other anxiolytic agents (eg, benzodiazepines, SSRIs), we hypothesized that PGB would be associated with attenuated activation of amygdala, insula, and mPFC during anticipation of emotional images. As PGB's proposed molecular mechanisms of action are unique compared with both benzodiazepines and SSRI's, this study also aimed to contribute knowledge concerning fMRI's potential as an anxiolytic biomarker across unique drug classes.

PATIENTS AND METHODS

Participants and Study Design

Sixteen right-handed, healthy volunteers (10 male; mean age=23.2±2.6) participated in three scanning sessions; one placebo, one low dose (50 mg) and one high dose (200 mg) of PGB, each conducted 1 to 3 weeks apart to allow for complete elimination of the drug. Sessions were pseudo-randomized and double-blind. Diagnostic interviews were conducted and exclusion criteria included satisfying DSM-IV criteria for an axis I psychiatric disorder, current substance abuse, neurological disorders, current medications, smoking, or excessive caffeine consumption (>4 cups/day). All subjects gave written, informed consent and were treated in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by the University of California, San Diego Human Research Protection Program.

During scanning, subjects performed an anticipation task that combined a continuous performance task (CPT) with interspersed presentation of affective stimuli (see Figure 1) (Simmons et al, 2008, 2009). During the CPT, subjects pressed a ‘LEFT’ or ‘RIGHT’ button corresponding to the direction of an arrow on the screen. Simultaneously, a 250 ms long 500-Hz tone was presented every 2 s. During baseline conditions, the background was gray. Subjects were instructed that when the background turned blue, accompanied by a 250-Hz tone, a positive image would appear, whereas when the background turned yellow, accompanied by a 1000-Hz tone, a negative image would appear. Trials with blue or yellow background represented anticipation periods. Picture stimuli included 17 positive (ie, pleasant social scenes) and 17 negative (ie, physical injuries, common traumatic events) images from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) (Lang et al, 2008). Positively and negatively valenced images were matched on levels of arousal (as reported in the IAPS manual) and content. No subject response was required during affective image presentation. Anticipation periods lasted 6 s, image presentation lasted 2 s, and the baseline CPT was interspersed for variable duration (mean=8 s) between task components. Total task duration was 580 s. The CPT task was included to ensure subjects attended to the task and to provide measures of response accuracy and reaction time during the various task conditions.

Figure 1
figure 1

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) anticipation task. This task combines a continuous performance task with the interspersed presentation of affective stimuli. Subjects are asked to press a left or right button based on the direction of the arrow. Subjects are instructed before the task that a blue square accompanied by a low tone indicates a positive image is going to appear. In contrast, a switch to a yellow square accompanied by a high tone signals an impending negative image. Images used in this paradigm were taken from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) (Lang et al, 2008). The color reproduction of this figure is available on the html full text version of the manuscript

PowerPoint slide

Oral doses of PGB or placebo were administered with a small amount of diet coke approximately 1 h before scanning. Plasma concentrations were acquired after 15, 30, 75, 90, and 110 min. Before dosing and after scanning, subjects completed the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) with possible scores ranging from 1 to 9 (Akerstedt and Gillberg, 1990), State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) state subscale with possible scores ranging from 20 to 80 (Spielberger, 1983), and rated their current state on a 10 cm Visual Analog Scale (VAS) for the following dimensions: anxious, embarrassed, tense, tired, dizzy, high, nervous, and trembling.

Data Acquisition

The anticipation task was conducted during one fMRI scan sensitive to blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) contrast using a Signa Excite (GE Healthcare) 3.0T scanner (T2*-weighted echoplanar (EPI) imaging, TR=2000 ms, TE=32 ms, 64 × 64 matrix, thirty 2.6 mm axial slices with a 1.4 mm gap, 290 scans). During the same session, a high-resolution T1-weighted image (spoiled gradient recalled, TR=8 ms, TE=3 ms, FOV=25 cm, approximately 1 mm3 voxels) was obtained for anatomical reference.

Whole-brain images sensitive to arterial spin labeling (ASL) were included to identify and covary for PGB's overall effects on cerebral blood flow (CBF) and acquired with a PICORE-QUIPSS II (Wong et al, 1998) sequence, dual-echo spiral acquisition (TE1=3 ms, TE2=30 ms; TR=2000 ms, flip angle=90°, 3.43 × 3.43 mm in-plane resolution, five 6 mm slices, 20 cm tag width, 1 cm gap between the tag and the proximal imaging slice, TI1=700 ms, TI2=1400 ms). ASL scans were calibrated between subjects and scanning sessions (Floyd et al, 2003). Proton density of cerebrospinal fluid, a signal index directly proportional to that of blood and relatively consistent across both time and subjects, was obtained with a single image through the ventricles at TE=3 ms.

Data Analysis

Data were preprocessed and analyzed using Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI) software (Cox, 1996) and R statistical packages (http://www.r-project.org). EPI images were aligned to high-resolution anatomical images and resampled to 4 × 4 × 4 mm voxel size (original 3.7 × 3.7 × 2.6 mm with 1.4 mm gap). Data were spatially blurred with a 4 mm FWHM spatial filter and normalized to Talairach space (via AFNI's auto Talairach program, followed by visual inspection of each image). Preprocessed individual time-series data were analyzed using a multiple regression model based on a BOLD hemodynamic response function with a 4–6 s peak. Regressors of interest included four orthogonal regressors used to quantify neural activation for each task component: (1) anticipation of positive images (APIs), (2) anticipation of negative images (ANIs), (3) presentation of positive images (PIs), and (4) presentation of negative images (NIs). Five regressors of no interest were included: three motion-related regressors (roll, pitch, yaw), data from a white-matter mask (averaged at each TR) to control for physiological noise, and a regressor for linear trends to eliminate slow signal drifts. This paper focuses on results from the anticipation phase, although results from the image phase are included in Supplementary Tables S5–S7.

Percent signal change (PSC) was calculated by dividing the regressor of interest by the control regressor. PSC for API and ANI was subjected to linear mixed effects (LMEs) analysis in R (www.cran.org). Mixed effects models offer a flexible framework to model sources of variation and correlation that arise from grouped data. They are particularly robust for analysis of unbalanced data when compared with similar analyses within the general linear model framework. Implementation of LME in R estimates parameters using the maximum likelihood estimation or restricted maximum likelihood estimation procedure. Subject was treated as random effects and dose (placebo, 50 mg, 200 mg) and task valence (API, ANI) as fixed effects. Main and interaction effects of dose and valence were examined. Bilateral amygdala, mPFC, and insula were selected as a priori regions of interest (ROIs) based on literature implicating them in emotional processing, anticipation, and anxiety disorders. These anatomical regions were defined using Talairach Atlas (see Supplementary Figure 1) (Lancaster et al, 2000). The mPFC included ventral and dorsal ACC (including BA 32, 24) and ventromedial PFC (BA 10). The mPFC was partitioned into dorsal and ventral extents at z=0, and the insula was partitioned into anterior and posterior portions using linear interpolation for location of the middle insular gyrus. Thus, there were eight a priori ROIs: bilateral amygdala (2752 μl each), dorsal (21 824 μl), and ventral (17 984 μl) mPFC, bilateral anterior (9920 μl each) and posterior (8512 μl each) insula. Whole-brain volume consisted of 2 334 464 μl. A threshold adjustment method based on Monte–Carlo simulations (conducted via AFNI's program Alpha Sim) was used to guard against identifying false-positive areas of activation (considering region voxel size, 4 mm smoothness, and 0.05 p threshold). This resulted in the following minimum cluster volumes to be considered significant: 192 μl for amygdala, 256 μl for insula regions, 320 μl for mPFC regions, and 832 μl for whole-brain analyses. Although the cluster significance of p<0.05 (two-tailed) was used for all analyses, corrected voxel-wise probabilities were: amygdala p<0.0021, anterior insular p<0.0015, posterior insula p<0.0016, mPFC regions p<0.0008, and whole-brain p<0.00001. All reported coordinates are peak-voxel Talairach coordinates (x, y, z) and labeled based on visual observation and Talairach Daemon software (Lancaster et al, 2000). For identified activation clusters, average PSC was extracted and F and p-values for the LME were calculated. To further characterize main and interaction effects, post hoc LMEs were conducted with specific contrasts (eg, PLB vs 50 mg, PLB vs 200 mg, 50 vs 200 mg), considered significant at p<0.05. Notably, although clusters exhibiting a main effect were not significant for the interaction, there were subregions of the same ROIs exhibiting both main and interaction effects.

Behavioral data (CPT reaction time and accuracy) were subjected to LME analyses with subject as random effects and PGB dose and task valence as fixed effects. Survey data (KSS, STAI, and VAS scales) were subjected to LME analyses with subject as random effects and PGB dose and time of self-report (before dosing vs post-scan) as fixed effects to examine dose by time interactions. To further characterize identified effects, post hoc LMEs were conducted with specific contrasts (eg, PLB vs 50 mg, PLB vs 200 mg, 50 vs 200 mg), considered significant at p<0.05. Voxel-based fMRI analyses were conducted with the addition of behavioral covariates (difference score between pre-dosing and post-scan) significantly influenced by PGB dose. Any changes these covariates made to fMRI ROI results are discussed.

PGB plasma concentrations were assessed at National Medical Services Lab using high-performance liquid chromatography. The detection limit of this procedure is 0.10 mcg/ml. LME analysis was conducted with subject as random effects and PGB dose and time of blood drawn as fixed effects to examine dose by time interaction effects on plasma concentration.

ASL scans were reconstructed and CBF was calculated using in-house MATLAB scripts (Rack-Gomer et al, 2009). Voxel-based LMEs were completed in R to examine potential drug effects on CBF. Whole-brain and anatomically constrained ROI analyses were conducted with these results as described for BOLD data. Voxel-based fMRI LME analysis was also conducted with CBF as a covariate and any changes this made to fMRI ROI results are discussed.

RESULTS

Behavior

Post-medication dizziness and KSS ratings were missing for one subject and there were two individual scans in which CPT behavioral data did not save correctly. All other behavioral data were available for the 16 subjects at each of the three scans. Degrees of freedom for each analysis are reflective of the number of data points available for that measure. Supplementary Table S1 displays the mean and SD for behavioral variables for each dose and time of measurement.

LME modeling revealed a significant dose by time (pre-dosing vs post-scan) effect on ratings of sleepiness (KSS; F(2, 72)=3.30, p=0.043; post hoc analyses revealed (PLB=50 mg)<200 mg) and VAS ratings of feeling dizzy (F(2, 73)=10.60, p<0.001; post hoc analyses revealed (PLB=50 mg)<200 mg), trembling (F(2, 73=3.74, p=0.028; post hoc analyses revealed 50<200 mg), high (F(2, 72=3.19, p=0.047; post hoc analyses revealed PLB<200 mg), and tired (F(2, 73=3.13, p=0.050; post hoc analyses revealed 50<200 mg). See Figure 2 for graphical presentation of these results. Dose had no significant effect on pre-dosing vs post-scan ratings of state anxiety (STAI state subscale; F(2, 73)=2.52, p=0.088, nonsignificant effect was related to STAI score increasing slightly more at the 200 mg dose sessions as compared with the 50 mg dose, but not as compared with placebo), or VAS ratings related to feeling embarrassed (F(2, 73)=1.54, p=0.220), nervous (F(2, 73)=1.08, p=0.350), anxious, or tense (Fs<1).

Figure 2
figure 2

Pregabalin (PGB) dose effect on self-report and behavioral measures. PGB exhibited dose effects on self-report measures related to (a) increased dizziness as measured by visual analogue scale (post hoc tests revealed (0=50)<200), (b) increased sleepiness as measured by the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS; Akerstedt and Gillberg, 1990; post hoc tests revealed (0=50)<200). PGB also exhibited effects on performance during the continuous performance task (CPT; averaged across conditions) conducted during the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) anticipation paradigm, including (c) increased reaction time (post hoc tests revealed (0=50)>200) and (d) decreased accuracy (post hoc tests revealed (0=50)<200).

PowerPoint slide

LMEs analyses also revealed a significant effect of dose on CPT reaction time (F(2, 114)=10.40, p<0.001; post hoc analyses revealed (PLB=50 mg)<200 mg) and accuracy (F(2, 114)=5.76, p=0.004; post hoc analyses revealed (PLB=50 mg)<200 mg). See Figure 2 for graphical representation of these effects. The main effect of valence and the dose by valence interaction effect were not significant (dose by valence effect on reaction time, F(4, 114)=1.09, p=0.363, all other Fs<1).

Although dose influenced both subjective ratings and CPT performance, the subjective measures (KSS, STAI, VAS ratings) were not significantly correlated with valence aspects of CPT performance (accuracy, reaction time).

Plasma Concentration

Plasma concentrations were elevated throughout the scanning phase for the 200 mg dose (mean concentration for 30–120 min post-administration=4.68±1.59 SD) and were near the detectable limit for the 50 mg dose (mean concentration 30–120 min post-administration=1.27±0.41). LME analyses revealed significant effects of dose (F(1, 116)=250.33, p<0.001), time (F(4, 116)=14.55, p<0.001), and dose by time interaction (F(4, 116)=11.69, p<0.001), reflecting that greater dose was associated with greater plasma concentration levels, that concentration levels increased with time, and that there was a greater increase in plasma concentration levels for the 200 mg than the 50 mg dose (see Figure 3).

Figure 3
figure 3

Pregabalin plasma concentration levels at various time points during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanning. Plasma was assessed at National Medical Services Lab using high-performance liquid chromatography. Both 50 and 200 mg was associated with detectable plasma concentration levels and greater dose was associated with significantly greater concentration levels.

PowerPoint slide

fMRI

Task-valence effects. ROI analysis revealed main effects of valence within the left (960 μl, F(1, 75)=18.60, p<0.005; −24, −4, −26) and right (192 μl, F(1, 75)=9.83, p<0.005; 38, −8, −22) amygdala (Figure 4), and left (832 μl, F(1, 75)=14.69, p<0.001; −32, −16, 18) and right (320 μl, F(1. 75)=9.43, p<0.005; 36, −20, 18) posterior insula, which were driven by greater activations for API than ANI regardless of PGB dose. One region of the right anterior/middle insula (256 μl, F(1, 75)=11.26, p<0.005; 36, 8, −2) exhibited greater activation for ANI than API (see Figure 4). No activations within the mPFC/ACC met criteria for significance. Results from whole-brain analysis of valence main effects are included in Supplementary Table S2 and include the left amygdala and posterior insula regions identified during ROI analyses as well as regions of the cortex that exhibited greater activation for either API (eg, dorsomedial PFC and superior PFC (BA 6), posterior cingulate, inferior and superior temporal) or ANI (eg, superior PFC (BA 6), dorsolateral PFC (BA 9), occipital, and superior temporal). When the LME valence analyses was constrained to placebo scans only, results remained similar for both ROI and whole-brain analyses as compared with the valence main effect across groups, although the right anterior insula ROI no longer met the cluster threshold (192 μl as compared with 256 μl; see Supplementary Table S3).

Figure 4
figure 4

Task valence effect on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) activation. Main effects for valence of anticipation, regardless of PGB dose, were observed for (a) bilateral amygdala (shown at y=−8), which exhibited less activation (% signal change) for negative anticipation (ANI) than positive anticipation (API) and (b) right anterior insula (shown at x=36), which exhibited greater activation for ANI than API. Graphs depicted below each image represent (1) average percent signal change (PSC) for each condition and (2) average PSC from the start of the anticipation conditions at each TR time point for each condition.

PowerPoint slide

Dose effects. ROI analysis revealed dose to be associated with decreases in activation (for API and ANI) within left amygdala (640 μl, F(2, 75)=11.06, p<0.001, −28, −4, −14; post hoc t-test, p<0.05, (PLB=50 mg)>200 mg) and left (384 μl, F(2, 75)=5.23, p=0.007, −32, 8, 14; post hoc t-test, p<0.05, PLB>(50 mg=200 mg)) and right (320 μl, F(2, 75)=4.86, p=0.01, 28, 20, 14; post hoc t-test, p<0.05, PLB>(50 mg=200 mg)) anterior insula (see Figure 5). There was also a dose-effect of increased activation in mid ACC (ACC, BA 32; 512 μl, F(2, 75)=6.02, p<0.005, 16, 36, −2; post hoc t-test, p<0.05, (PLB=50 mg)>200 mg; see Figure 5). Post hoc analyses of these effects suggested that insula attenuation was observed for both the 50 and 200 mg dose compared with placebo (but no differences between doses), while activation within amygdala and ACC were significantly different from placebo only at the 200 mg dose. Results from whole-brain analysis of dose main effects are included in Supplementary Table S4 and include decreased activations within regions of the lateral PFC (BA 6, 8), dorsal cingulate (BA 24), superior temporal, amygdala, thalamus, and fusiform gyrus, and enhanced activation within middle frontal (BA 3, 6), and dorsal cingulate and caudate. Of note, none of the clusters listed as showing a main effect of dose (within ROI or whole-brain analyses) exhibited a significant interaction effect (F values for the interaction effect for each region observed for the dose main effect are also displayed in Supplementary Table S4). However, there were subregions of these same ROIs that did in fact exhibit interaction effects, as discussed below.

Figure 5
figure 5

Pregabalin (PGB) dose effect on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) activation. PGB attenuated activation within in (a) left amygdala (shown at y=4; post hoc tests revealed (PLB=50 mg)>200 mg) and (b, c) bilateral anterior insula (right insula/claustraum shown at x=28 and left insula shown at x=−34; post hoc tests revealed PLB>(50 mg=200 mg)) and increased activation in (d) anterior cingulate (ACC; shown at x=14; post hoc t-tests revealed (PLB=50 mg)>200 mg), for anticipation of negative (ANI) and positive images (API). Graphs depicted below each image represent (1) average percent signal change (PSC) for each condition and (2) average PSC from the start of the anticipation conditions at each TR time point for each PGB dose (averaged across negative and positive conditions).

PowerPoint slide

Dose × valence effects. ROI analysis: dose by valence interaction effects were evident within subregions of the left amygdala (512 μl, F(2, 75)=9.31, p<0.005, −24, 4, −18, post hoc LME: PLB>50 mg>200 mg) and ventral ACC (vACC; 320 μl, F(2, 75)=7.93, p<0.005, −4, 24, −10, post hoc LME: (PLB=50 mg)>200 mg). Activation within the amygdala was driven by increased activations for ANI (ANI only F(2, 30)=3.23, p=0.054) and decreased activations for API (API only F(2, 30)=6.33, p=0.005) with each successive increasing dose, while activation within the vACC was characterized by increased activations for ANI (ANI only F(2, 30)=3.00, p=0.065) and decreased activations for API (API only F(2, 30)=4.56, p=0.019) at the 200 mg dose as compared with both PLB and the 50 mg dose. These activation clusters are displayed in Figure 6. Although there were also amygdala and ACC regions exhibiting dose main effects, it is important to note that these were not the same clusters as those identified for the interaction effect. The interaction effect within the amygdala was more anterior than that identified in the main effect analyses, and the interaction effect within the ACC was more ventral than that identified in the dose main effect analyses. Whole-brain analysis revealed no clusters meeting the specified criteria of significance.

Figure 6
figure 6

Pregabalin (PGB) dose × valence interaction effect on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) activation. Increasing PGB dose was associated with increased activation to anticipation of negative (ANI) images and decreased activation to anticipation of positive images (API) within the (a) left amygdala (shown at y=2; post hoc tests revealed PLB>50 mg>200 mg) and (b) ventral ACC (shown at x=3; post hoc tests revealed (PLB=50 mg)>200 mg). Graphs depicted below each image represent (1) average percent signal change (PSC) for each condition and (2) average PSC from the start of the anticipation conditions at each TR time point for ANIs and APIs for each PGB dose.

PowerPoint slide

ASL. ROI linear mixed model analysis revealed that PGB increased CBF within the left vACC (F(2, 24)=6.803, p<0.005, −12, 40, 2; post hoc test revealed PLB>50 mg). No significant effects of dose were found for other ROIs or for the whole-brain analyses. When CBF was included as a covariate in the fMRI analyses described above, results remained largely unchanged. For the dose main effect analysis, the reported right anterior insula cluster no longer met the cluster threshold. For the dose by valence interaction analyses, the reported vACC cluster was no longer significant. All other results remained unchanged.

Covariate analyses. The fMRI LME analyses described above were conducted to also include specific behavioral variables shown to be influenced by PGB dose as covariates, including sleepiness (KSS score), dizziness (as this was the VAS scale most influenced by PGB dose), and CPT reaction time (as this was the CPT measure most influenced by PGB dose). When change in KSS sleepiness score (pre-medication to post-scan) was included as a covariate, all ROI results discussed above, for both dose main effect and dose by valence interaction effects, remained unaltered. When change in dizziness ratings (baseline to post-scan) was included as a covariate, all ROI dose main effects discussed above remained unaltered. For the interaction (dose by valence) analysis, the vACC cluster no longer met significance. When CPT reaction time was included as a covariate, the right anterior insula cluster reported above regarding PGB dose main effect no longer met significance. Additionally, for the interaction (dose by valence) analysis, the vACC cluster no longer met significance. All other ROI results remained unaltered.

The fMRI LMEs analyses were also conducted with session as a covariate in order to establish whether order effects accounted for the ROI results. When session was included as a covariate in the analyses, all ROI results reported above for the valence main effect remained unaltered. For the dose main effect, the right anterior insula cluster observed was no longer significant, while all other ROI results remained unaltered. For the dose by valence interaction analyses, the observed vACC no longer exceeded the cluster threshold, while the amygdala ROI remained significant.

DISCUSSION

PGB fMRI Effects

In this study, PGB exhibited attenuation of left anterior insula and left amygdala activation during ANIs and APIs. Attenuation of insula activation during anticipation of painful or emotional stimuli has previously been reported for the benzodiazepine, midazolam (Wise et al, 2007) and the SSRI, escitalopram (Simmons et al, 2009). To our knowledge, this is the first study to report amygdala attenuation by an anxiolytic during an anticipation paradigm. However, amygdala attenuation has been reported for the benzodiazepine, lorazepam (Paulus et al, 2005) and the SSRIs, citalopram, and escitalopram (Arce et al, 2008; Windischberger et al, 2010) during processing of emotional faces. Benzodiazepines are thought to exert anxiolytic effects through modulation of GABAergic neurotransmission (Atack, 2003), SSRIs are thought to exert effects primarily through modulation of the 5HT system (Vaswani et al, 2003), and PGB is thought to influence multiple neurotransmitters through its binding to α2δ subunits of calcium channels (Fink et al, 2002; Gee et al, 1996). Despite their unique neuronal mechanisms of action, evidence collected from the current and previous studies suggests they may share a common regional brain mechanism—namely, the attenuation of insula and/or amygdala activation during anticipatory and emotional processing. This effect may therefore represent a viable biomarker for detecting anxiolytic effects of current and novel agents that is generalizable across various drug classes. Future research could further test this hypothesis by using fMRI to investigate the neural effects of novel agents that have shown initial promise as anxiolytics in preclinical trials, such as NK1 or CRF1 receptor antagonists, or agents targeting the endocannabinoid system (Christmas et al, 2008; Hill and Gorzalka, 2009).

Although the amygdala is thought to be important for processing stimuli that are emotionally salient or that have reinforcement value (Davis and Whalen, 2001; Morrison and Salzman, 2010), the insula is considered important in interoceptive, or somatosensory, processing and is thought to be involved in calculating a ‘change signal’ representing the difference between current internal state and future predicted internal state (Craig, 2009; Critchley et al, 2004). Attenuation of these regions could represent a decrease in emotional responsiveness to upcoming stimuli, or modified expectations of future internal bodily state changes (Lovero et al, 2009; Strigo et al, 2010a). If such attenuation is not unique to negatively valenced situations, it suggests that PGB could potentially dampen not only negative anticipatory emotions (eg, anxiety) but also positive (eg, excitement). However, clinical trials have not reported such affective blunting, and have in some cases reported PGB to produce feelings of euphoria (Pohl et al, 2005). Future fMRI research investigating PGB's effect on individual subjective responses to emotional situations and the relationship of such effects to amydala and insula activations could help clarify the specific meaning for PGB's attenuation of these regions across anticipation conditions.

An additional finding within the amygdala was the dose by valence interaction effect, which revealed PGB to produce enhanced activation of left amygdala during ANIs compared with relative attenuated activation during APIs. This finding is opposite of most studies that have reported valence-specific effects of anxiolytic medications (Anderson et al, 2007; Del-Ben et al, 2005; Wise et al, 2007). As discussed below, general task effects for the current paradigm revealed amygdala activations greater for anticipation of positive than negative anticipation. Regardless of the reason for this pattern of amygdala activation in response to the task (discussed further below), it seems PGB may have a greater effect on amygdala activation during events corresponding to greater amygdala activation at baseline. Notably, the amygdala region in which PGB exhibited this differential influence for anticipation of positive vs negative anticipation was more anterior than the amygdala region in which PGB exhibited a main effect across both negative and positive anticipation. Previous research has identified subregions of the amygdala that may serve different functions during emotional processing and fear learning, such as the basolateral amygdala being more involved in the evaluation of salient sensory information and the influence of other amygdala nuclei while the central amygdala may be more involved in the subsequent production of behavioral and autonomic responses to anxiety through its connections with the midbrain and brainstem (Davis and Whalen, 2001; Rosen, 2004). As these distinctions relate more to lateral vs medial or dorsal vs ventral subregions, it is difficult to interpret the meaning for different effects of PGB on posterior vs anterior amygdala, although it raises the possibility that PGB's effect on the amygdala may depend on the specific subregion.

This study also found PGB to increase ACC activation during anticipatory processing (across both negative and positive conditions). Although there was also a region of the vACC that exhibited a dose by valence interaction effect, this cluster was no longer significant when covarying for various behavioral or clinical measures (eg, continuous performance reaction time, VAS dizziness ratings) or session order. Therefore, our discussion of PGB effects on the ACC focus on the main effect within the mid ACC (BA 32). The direction of this finding was somewhat unexpected, given that previous studies with other anxiolytics have reported either no modulation or attenuated activation of ACC (Arce et al, 2008; Simmons et al, 2009; Wise et al, 2007). mPFC and ACC regions have been implicated as primary loci for automatic regulation of limbic activation and emotional responses (Phillips et al, 2003). Therefore, enhanced ACC activation could represent increased top–down regulation during anticipatory processing. Although this finding should be considered preliminary until further replication, it suggests that enhanced ACC activation may be a unique effect of PGB.

Post hoc analysis revealed that the majority of clusters identified through whole-brain analysis (as well as the dose main effects within the amygdala and ACC) were associated with significant activation differences at the 200 mg, but not the 50 mg, dose of PGB. Previous studies support the notion that 200 mg doses of PGB are effective in the treatment of anxiety (Feltner et al, 2003; Pohl et al, 2005), whereas doses of 50–150 mg have been associated with minimal clinical benefit (Feltner et al, 2003; Pande et al, 2004). The fact that the 200 mg dose seemed to be driving many of the fMRI BOLD effects observed during anticipation could therefore be considered consistent with findings from clinical research. It seems that while the 200 mg dose was associated with increased side effects as compared with both placebo and the 50 mg dose, the 50 mg dose may not influence brain activation patterns (or clinical symptoms) to the same extent as the 200 mg dose. Of note, both the 50 and 200 mg dose were associated with dose main effects in the anterior insula and interaction effects within the amygdala. This suggests that although 50 mg may be a subclinical dose and have less of a widespread effect on brain activation during anticipation, it may still exert subtle effects that can be identified using fMRI.

PGB Behavioral Effects

This study found PGB (particularly the 200 mg dose) to influence subject ratings of dizziness, sleepiness, feeling high, and trembling, as well as objective measures of reaction time and accuracy during the fMRI CPT. These findings support previous research, as studies have shown PGB to often produce side effects of dizziness and drowsiness (Bandelow et al, 2004; Pande et al, 2003, 2004; Rickels et al, 2005) and to mildly influence some measures of cognitive function (Hindmarch et al, 2005; Salinsky et al, 2010). Although the cognitive effects of PGB reportedly pale in comparison with alprazolam (Hindmarch et al, 2005), further investigation of PGB's effects on cognitive function and speed of processing are warranted. Notably, current fMRI results remained largely unaltered when including behavioral and self-report measures as covariates, suggesting that findings related to PGB effects on brain activations were above and beyond what could be accounted for by changes in alertness or cognitive functioning.

Anticipatory Task Effects

In this study, the right anterior insula exhibited greater activation for anticipation of negative than positive affective images, regardless of PGB dose. This finding supports previous studies suggesting the anterior insula as important for negatively valenced anticipation (Nitschke et al, 2006; Simmons et al, 2004). However, current results also found the amygdala to exhibit greater activation for anticipation of positive than negative images. Previous healthy control studies involving anticipatory processing have either not observed significant differences in anticipatory valence within the amygdala (Simmons et al, 2004) or reported greater activation for negative than neutral anticipation (Nitschke et al, 2006). Although the amygdala is often considered important for negative emotional states, there is evidence that it most likely has a role in processing saliency of stimuli, rather than valence per se (Anderson et al, 2003; Garavan et al, 2001; Lewis et al, 2007; Small et al, 2003). The fact that the positively and negatively valenced IAPS images used in this study were match for level of arousal and that the positive images included people and scenery, which are potentially more salient than images of inanimate objects, as was used by Nitschke et al (while Simmons et al did not include a comparison anticipatory condition) could have contributed to this finding. However, this factor would not necessarily account for greater activation during anticipation of positively valenced stimuli. A recent study found that during anticipation of painful stimuli amygdala activation decreased when subjects completed a CPT as compared with passive viewing during anticipation periods (Strigo et al, 2010b). The CPT was included with the current anticipation paradigm to assure that subjects were attending to the task and looking at the screen, as well as to provide measures of response accuracy and reaction time during the different task conditions. However, it is possible that the inclusion of a cognitive task such as the CPT results in greater decrease in amygdala activation during negative vs positive anticipatory states, therefore contributing to the pattern of amygdala activation observed in this study. The effect of including a cognitive task such as the CPT with emotional paradigms could result in the reallocation of attentional resources or provide an implicit distraction from the emotional task. The influence of cognitive tasks on neural activation patterns during emotional (negative and positive) anticipation warrants further investigation. Additionally, studies investigating PGB effects during emotional tasks that do not also involve an attentional task would help to clarify whether the results observed in this study are specific to this CPT-anticipation task or are relevant for emotional processing in general.

As apparent from the results of whole-brain analyses, the anticipation task used in this study was related to differential activation in regions other than a priori ROIs, including regions of the prefrontal, temporal, and occipital cortices. This lack of specificity is similar to that found in previous studies using this same task (Simmons et al, 2009) or other emotional processing tasks, such as that including emotional images or faces (Britton et al, 2006; Fusar-Poli et al, 2009) and suggests that a network more extensive than limbic and mPFC are involved in processing of emotional stimuli and events. The identification of tasks that more specifically activate insula and amygdala regions may prove more useful in characterizing anxiolytic potential of pharmacologic agents. However, results from this study suggest that fMRI tasks that are currently available, even given lack of specificity, may still prove useful in this regard. It is possible that regardless of the specific patterns of activation in cortical regions, the end product of attenuated insula and amygdala activation during emotional processing may represent a biomarker of anxiolytic potential.

Limitations

This study included two doses of PGB (50 and 200 mg) that were intended to yield ‘low but discernible’ and ‘moderate but well-tolerated’ effects. Although we believe the lower dose achieved its intended effect, the higher dose may have resulted in more side-effects than would have been optimal. Although the ROI results reported in this study remained largely unchanged when sleepiness, dizziness, and reaction time were included as covariates, additional research with slightly lower doses should also be considered. Also, this study investigated neural effects of acute PGB administration. Although PGB clinical trials have reported symptom reduction within 1 week (Feltner et al, 2003; Lydiard et al, 2010) or even as early as 4 h post-administration (Feltner et al, 2003), it is possible neural mechanisms of acute PGB may differ from those of chronic PGB treatment. Future investigations regarding neural effects of sub-chronic PGB could be useful in extending the current findings. A strength of this study is that it examined the effects of different doses of PGB as compared with placebo in the same group of subjects. However, this could also be considered a limitation in that it required repeated administration of the same anticipation paradigm and therefore could have been influenced by habituation or sensitization effects. Although the ROI results reported remained largely unchanged when including session as a covariate, there remains the potential that repeated testing influenced results. Additionally, this study included healthy control subjects without diagnoses of anxiety disorders. We would expect that individual's with anxiety disorders would display greater differential activation in the ROIs during the anticipation task, as indicated in previous studies (Simmons et al, 2006, 2008). Owing to this, we would expect that the effects of PGB on activation within these regions may be even more pronounced than in this study. However, it is also possible that PGB's mechanisms differ depending on the psychological state of the individual. Future research investigating PGB effects on neural activation of anxiety disorder patients could help to determine the generalizability of current findings to clinical populations and could examine the relationship of such acute neural mechanisms to treatment outcome.

CONCLUSIONS

The current investigation provides evidence that PGB may share common neural mechanisms with SSRIs and benzodiazepines, namely the attenuation of insula and amygdala activation during affective processing. However, PGB also exhibited effects that have not been reported for other anxiolytics, including enhanced activation in the ACC. The neural effects of PGB did not seem to be due to changes in CBF as measured by ASL or to objective or subjective behavioral measures. Accumulating evidence would suggest that attenuation of insula and amygdala activation during anticipatory or emotional processing may represent an important biomarker for anxiolytic effects across various drug classes. fMRI examination of the neural effects of novel agents with anxiolytic potential could help test the viability of using such fMRI paradigms to predict anxiolytic efficacy.