INTRODUCTION

Schizophrenia is a common psychiatric disorder that affects approximately 1% of the population worldwide (Mueser and McGurk, 2004; Sawa and Snyder, 2002) and is characterized by positive (hallucinations, delusions, disorganized speech, and disorganized behavior) and negative (affective flattening, alogia, and avolition) symptoms (Andreasen and Flaum, 1991; van Os and Kapur, 2009). In addition to these symptoms, cognitive deficits are becoming increasingly recognized as an important feature of schizophrenia (Breier, 2005; Keefe and Fenton, 2007; Ranganath et al, 2008), however, no therapies are currently available that improve the cognitive performance of these patients.

Although the etiology of schizophrenia is poorly understood, converging evidence from a number of fields supports the view that deficits in neuronal connectivity linked to synaptic alterations is a key contributing factor to this devastating disorder (Harrison, 1999; Mirnics et al, 2000; Robertson et al, 2006). For instance, the protein encoded by a gene (disrupted in schizophrenia 1, DISC1) that is mutated in a familial form of schizophrenia is involved in various interactions between microtubule-related organelles or proteins suggesting that connectivity deficits in schizophrenia may result from dysfunction of the cytoskeleton (Ayhan et al; Callicott et al, 2005). In keeping with this proposal, Shimizu and coworkers have reported an association between schizophrenia and polymorphisms in the gene-encoding microtubule-associated protein 6 (MAP6) (Shimizu et al, 2006). The Map6 gene product known as STOP (stable tubule only peptide) is a Ca2+-calmodulin regulated microtubule-associated protein present in neurons, oligodendrocytes, and astrocytes (Bosc et al, 2003; Denarier et al, 1998; Galiano et al, 2004). Mice lacking a functional copy of STOP (STOP null) display several synaptic abnormalities that are accompanied by an array of cognitive deficits (Begou et al, 2008; Brun et al, 2005; Eastwood et al, 2007; Powell et al, 2007). STOP null mice display a two-fold reduction in pre-synaptic glutamatergic vesicle density in the CA1 region of the hippocampus, a brain structure implicated in learning and memory (Andrieux et al, 2002). This pre-synaptic vesicular depletion is accompanied by deficits in electrophysiological measures of synaptic plasticity such as LTP, long-term depression and post-tetanic potentiation that may account for performance deficits in behavioral tests of learning and memory (Andrieux et al, 2002). STOP null mice also show neurochemical, pharmacological and behavioral profiles indicative of hyperactivity of the mesolimbic dopamine system (Bouvrais-Veret et al, 2008; Brun et al, 2005; Fradley et al, 2005). In STOP null mice, mesolimbic dopaminergic hyperactivity is accompanied by reduced glutamatergic transmission (Brenner et al, 2007), an interrelationship now thought to be a neurochemical hallmark of schizophrenia (Carlsson et al, 2000; Jablensky, 2004). Delotterie et al, (2010) reported that long-term treatment with risperidone, a representative atypical antipsychotic drug, did not improve working memory of STOP null mice assessed with the Y maze. This finding is consistent with the lack or weak effects of antipsychotic drugs on cognitive deficits associated with schizophrenia (Young et al, 2009) suggesting that the STOP null mouse may be a useful model for the identification of therapeutics capable of improving the cognition of schizophrenics. Taken together, these findings indicate that the STOP null mice model several important features of schizophrenia.

Erythropoietin (Epo) is a glycoprotein derived from the kidney whose principal function is to stimulate erythropoiesis by inhibiting the programmed cell death of erythrocyte precursors (Ghezzi and Brines, 2004). Epo also has well-established neuroprotective properties in models of cerebral ischemia (Sadamoto et al, 1998; Sakanaka et al, 1998), neuroinflammation (Brines et al, 2000; Li et al, 2004), neuronal injury (Zhang et al, 2010), and neurotoxin-induced cell death (Adembri et al, 2004; Orhan et al, 2004). In addition to these neuroprotective properties, recent studies have shown that Epo enhances cognitive and neural measures of executive function using a verbal fluency task and working memory during functional magnetic resonance imaging in healthy volunteers (Miskowiak et al, 2008a) and improves cognitive function in schizophrenia patients (Ehrenreich et al, 2007). Epo is now being considered as an adjunctive therapy for cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders; however, the mechanism by which Epo improves cognition remains unclear. Based on these findings, we hypothesized that acute administration of darbepoetin alfa (D. alfa), an Epo analog, would improve performance deficits of STOP null mice in the novel object recognition task (NORT). Given that Epo has been shown to increase extracellular concentrations of nitric oxide (NO) (Yamamoto et al, 2004), a chemical messenger implicated in learning and memory (Kendrick et al, 1997; Ota et al, 2008), we also hypothesized that D.alfa would increase NO metabolites in the cortex and hippocampus and modulate nitric oxide synthase (NOS) activity within specific structures in the forebrain. Lastly, we hypothesized that administration of the NOS inhibitor L-NAME would abolish the effects of D.alfa on novel object recognition memory, indicating that the effects of D. alfa on memory performance were mediated through NO signaling.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

Adult male and female BALBc/129 SvPas heterozygous STOP mice were obtained from a colony maintained in Grenoble, France (Andrieux et al, 2002). Mice were crossed with male and female 129S1/SvImJ mice (Jackson Laboratories, Bar Harbor, ME, USA) to produce litters with increased viability. Heterozygous (BALBc/129 SvPas x 129S1/SvImJ) breeding pairs were established that generated litters at an approximate ratio of 25% wild-type (WT): 50% heterozygous (Het): 25% homozygous null for STOP (STOP null). Mice were group housed with ad libitum access to food and water, and maintained on a 12 h light–dark schedule. The Carleton Animal Care Committee at Dalhousie University approved the experimental procedures described in this article and that were also in accordance with the guidelines detailed by the Canada Council on Animal Care. Each of the four groups used to compare the effects of vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.) and darbepoetin alfa (D. alfa; 25 μg/kg, i.p.) on the NORT performance of WT and STOP null mice were composed of 12–16 animals (Figure 1). In the case of the nitrate and nitrite (NOx) tissue measurements shown in Figure 3, 8–10 animals from each of these four groups were used. For the L-NAME dose–response studies shown in Figure 4b–d, each group consisted of 5–6 mice that were also used for NOx tissue measurements (Figure 4a). In the case of the last experiment in which we examined the ability of L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) to reverse the effects of D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) on NORT performance, each group was composed of 8–9 animals (Figure 5a and b). All of these mice in the four groups were used for determination of NOx levels (Figure 5c). For the NADPH diaphorase staining shown in Figure 2, separate groups, composed of 6 mice each, were given vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.) or D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) and prepared for histological examination of NADPH diaphorase staining.

Figure 1
figure 1

Acute darbepoetin alfa (D. alfa; 25 μg/kg, i.p.) treatment reversed object recognition deficits in STOP null mice. (a) Object exploration time in the sample phase. The time spent exploring objects is shown in the bar graph as mean±SEM. Wild-type mice (WT) and STOP null mice (KO) injected with vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.; WT: N=12, KO: N=13) or D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.; WT: N=12, KO: N=16) spent equal amounts of time investigating the objects during the sample phase. (b) Discrimination ratio (d2) in the choice phase is shown in the bar graph as mean±SEM. KO mice treated with an acute injection of D. alfa (25 μg/kg i.p.) displayed a significant improvement in NORT performance compared with vehicle controls. White bars, vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.; WT: N=12, KO: N=13); black bars, D.alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.; WT: N=12, KO: N=16). Two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc t-test, *p<0.05.

PowerPoint slide

Figure 2
figure 2

Representative NADPH diaphorase staining in the forebrain of a WT mouse 3–4 h after D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) or vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.). (a and b) dorsal hippocampus; Bregma=−2.30 mm, (c and d) ventral hippocampus; Bregma=−3.16 mm, (e and f) amygdala (Am) and rhinal cortex (Rh); Bregma=−2.18 mm. Mice (WT or STOP null) treated with D. alfa (25 μg/kg i.p.) showed a marked increase in NADPH diaphorase staining in the ventral hippocampus as well as medial and cortical aspects of the amygdala relative to vehicle controls (indicated by arrowheads). Scale bars; 1 mm. (g) Quantification of signal intensity for NADPH-d staining, bars represent the means±SEM of groups composed of six mice each. Mann–Whitney U-test, *p<0.05.

PowerPoint slide

Figure 3
figure 3

Nitrate and nitrite (NOx) levels in brain tissue from wild-type (WT) and STOP null mice (KO) that received either D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) or vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.). (a) NOx levels in cortex. (b) NOx levels in hippocampus. NOx levels [μM/mg (protein)] are shown in the bar graph as mean±SEM. NOx levels in hippocampus were increased in both WT and KO mice 3–4 h after D. alfa treatment. White bars, vehicle (10 ml/kg; WT: N=8, KO: N=8); black bars, D. alfa (25 μg/kg i.p.; WT: N=10, KO: N=8). Two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc t-test, *p<0.05.

PowerPoint slide

Figure 4
figure 4

Effect of L-NAME on NOx levels and NORT performance in WT mice. L-NAME (10, 25, or 50 mg/kg; i.p.) was administered 30 min before vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.) or D. alfa (25 μg/kg i.p.). NORT was performed 3 h after D. alfa treatment, and then hippocampi were subjected to measurement of NOx levels. (a) Dose-dependent inhibition of D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.)-induced increases in nitrate and nitrite (NOx) levels in the hippocampus of wild-type mice by L-NAME. NOx levels (μM/mg (protein)) are shown in the bar graph as mean±SEM. V; vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.); Darbepoetin alfa, D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.), V/−: N=5, V/+: N=6, 10 mg/+: N=5, 25 mg/+: N=6, 50 mg/+: N=5. One-way ANOVA with Newman-Keuls test, *p<0.05. (b) Object exploration time in the sample phase. Each bar represents the mean±SEM. Animals that received vehicle or L-NAME spent equivalent amounts of time exploring objects during sample phase. (c) Discrimination ratio (d2) during the choice phase for animals injected with vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.) or L-NAME (10, 25, or 50 mg/kg; i.p.). Each bar graph represents the mean±SEM. There were no significant differences between groups that received vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.) or L-NAME (10, 25, or 50 mg/kg; i.p.), one-way ANOVA. (d) Percent time spent exploring the novel object during the choice phase. Each bar graph represents the mean±SEM. V; vehicle (10 ml/kg); Darbepoetin alfa, D. alfa (25 μg/kg), V/−: N=5, V/+: N=6, 10 mg/+: N=5, 25 mg/+: N=6, 50 mg/+: N=5. There were no statistically significant differences among the groups, one-way ANOVA.

PowerPoint slide

Figure 5
figure 5

L-NAME inhibits the improvement in NORT performance and the increase of NOx levels of STOP null mice that received D. alfa. Wild-type (WT) or STOP null mice (KO) were treated with L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) and vehicle (10 mg/kg, i.p.; WT: N=8, KO: N=8) or L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) and D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.; WT: N=9, KO: N=8). The first injection occurred 30 min before the second. NORT was performed 3 h after D. alfa treatment, and then hippocampi were subjected to measurement of NOx levels. (a) Object exploration time in sample phase for WT and KO treated with L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) and vehicle (10 mg/kg, i.p.) or L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) and D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.). Each bar represents mean±SEM. All four groups spent an equivalent amount of time investigating the objects during the sample phase. (b) Discrimination ratio (d2) in choice phase for WT or KO treated with L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) and vehicle (10 mg/kg, i.p.) or L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) and D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.). Each bar represents mean±SEM. L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) blocked the ability of D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) to improve NORT performance of KO. White bars, L-NAME (25 mg/kg) and vehicle (10 ml/kg); black bars, L-NAME (25 mg/kg) and D.alfa (25 μg/kg). Two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc t-test, *p<0.05. (c) L-NAME inhibited the increase in nitrate and nitrite (NOx) levels induced by D. alfa in the hippocampus of wild-type and STOP null mice. The hippocampi from all animals were immediately removed and NOx measurements performed. NOx levels (μM/mg (protein)) are shown in the bar graph as mean±SEM. L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) blocked the ability of D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) to increase NOx levels in both WT and KO mice. White bars, L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) and vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.); black bars, L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) and D.alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.). There were no statistically significant differences among the groups, two-way ANOVA.

PowerPoint slide

Drugs

Darbepoetin alfa (D. alfa: Aranesp, Amgen Therapeutics, Thousand Oaks, CA) was obtained as 100 μg/ml stock and diluted to 2.5 μg/ml in vehicle (0.1% bovine serum albumin in 0.1 M phosphate-buffered saline (PBS)) and administered to STOP null mice or WT littermates at a dose of 25 μg/kg (i.p.). Animals received D. alfa or vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.) 3 h before testing. N (G)-nitro-L-arginine methyl ester (L-NAME; Sigma-Aldrich, St Louis, MO, USA) was diluted in sterile water and administered to mice 30 min before the D. alfa injection at doses of 10, 25, or 50 mg/kg (i.p.).

Novel Object Recognition Task (NORT)

NORT is a test of object recognition memory that is dependent upon both the hippocampal formation and the rhinal cortex (Broadbent et al, 2004; Winters et al, 2004). The NORT was performed as described previously (Powell et al, 2007). Briefly, the NORT was conducted in an opaque plexiglass open field (35 × 45.5 × 36 cm). Animals were habituated to the open field in the absence of objects for 5 min on each of 2 successive days before the test day. On the test day, animals were habituated to the open field for 1 min and then returned to a holding cage while two identical objects were placed in the open field. During the sample phase, each mouse was placed in the center of the open field oriented towards the objects and the time spent exploring each object was recorded over a 5-min period. The mouse was then returned to the home cage for a delay of 5 min before being returned to the open field for the choice phase. In the choice phase, the mouse was placed in the center of the open field oriented toward the objects: one familiar object from the sample phase and a novel object. Time spent exploring each object over a 3-min period was recorded. Exploration was defined as directing the nose toward the object at a distance less than 1 cm, and included placing one or more paws on the object. Sitting beside or on top of the object was not considered exploration. The total time spent investigating both objects in the sample phase was recorded and compared to ensure equal motivation to explore. In the choice phase the proportion of time spent with the novel object was determined by dividing the time spent with the novel object by the total time spent exploring both objects. A discrimination ratio (d2) was calculated by taking the difference between the times spent investigating the novel and familiar objects and dividing that by the time spent investigating both objects during the choice phase. Animals that failed to reach a threshold exploration time in the sample and choice phases (10 and 5 s, respectively) were excluded from the study.

Histology

Animals were deeply anesthetized with pentobarbital (30 mg/kg i.p.) and perfused intracardially with saline followed by cold 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in 0.1 M PBS. The brains were removed, immersed for 12 h in the same 4% PFA fixative at 4°C, and cryoprotected in 30% sucrose in PBS for 48 h at 4°C. The brains were then frozen and stored at −80°C until use. Serial coronal sections (30 μm thick) were cut on a cryostat, collected as free-floating sections in PBS, and then processed immediately for NADPH diaphorase staining.

NADPH Diaphorase (NADPH-d) Staining

NADPH-d staining was performed as described previously with some modifications (Hope and Vincent, 1989; Vincent and Kimura, 1992). Sections were rinsed 2 × 10 min in 50 mM Tris-buffer saline (TBS) to equilibrate the tissue, and incubated in NADPH solution (1 mM â-NADPH (Sigma-Aldrich) and 0.1 mM Nitrotetrazolium blue (Sigma-Aldrich) in TBS with 0.1% Tween 20 (TBS-T)) for 70 min at 37°C protected from light. Sections were then rinsed 2 × 10 min in TBS-T and stored in TBS-T diluted 1 : 1 with distilled water. Slides were submerged in 100% xylene and coverslipped using Cytoseal solution (Richard Allan Scientific, Kalamazoo, MI, USA). Slides were viewed using an Axioscope (Carl Zeiss, Toronto, ON, Canada). Brain images were captured with an Axiocam HRc digital camera using imaging software (Canimpex Enterprises, Halifax, NS, Canada).

Measurement of NO Metabolite (Nitrate and Nitrite; NOx) Levels

For determination of hippocampal and cortical NOx levels, hippocampi and cortices were rapidly dissected from the brains. The dissected hippocampi and cortices were homogenized in 1 × PBS and spun at 10 000 g for 20 min at 4°C. The supernatant was placed on 30 kDa filters (Millipore, Etobicoke, ON, Canada) and spun at 14 000 g for 60 min at 4°C. The filtrate was collected to measure nitrite levels. The final products of NO in vivo are nitrite (NO2) and nitrate (NO3). The relative proportions of NO2 and NO3 are variable and cannot be predicted with certainty. Thus the best measure of total NO production is the sum of both NO2 and NO3 (NOx). This was achieved using a commercial kit from Cayman Chemical (Ann Arbor, MI, USA) that first converts all nitrate to nitrite using nitrate reductase. The second step used a Griess reaction to convert all nitrite into a deep purple azo compound that was detected using a microplate reader. Known concentrations of nitrite were run in tandem to create a standard curve for the quantification of nitrite levels in each homogenate sample derived from the hippocampus. Levels of nitrite were standardized according to protein content to yield μM nitrite/mg protein.

Densitometric Analysis of NADPH-d Staining

NADPH-d staining was quantified by densitometric analysis performed on every 12th coronal section (30 μm thick) taken from the level of the anterior caudate–putamen to the posterior aspect the hippocampus (Bregma −0.70 mm to −3.40 mm). All acquired digital images were converted to gray scale and the optical density of using NADPH-d staining measured using ImageJ software (http://rsbweb.nih.gov/ij/). The background grayscale intensity arising from white matter (corpus callosum) was subtracted from that for the regions of interest. Optical density measurements were performed on separate coronal sections for each animal. These values were averaged for each brain region (dorsal hippocampus; five sections (Bregma −1.34 to −2.46 mm), cortex; five sections (Bregma −1.34 mm to −3.40 mm), ventral hippocampus; three sections (Bregma −2.46 to −3.40 mm), amygdala; five sections (Bregma −0.70 to −2.54 mm) and rhinal cortex; five sections (Bregma −0.70 to −2.54 mm)). For each brain region that was sampled, the average optical density for the vehicle group was first calculated (0–255 gray scale). The optical density for NADPH-d staining for each animal was then converted to a NADPH-d index. This was done by dividing the optical density for each animal by the average optical density for the vehicle group. In this way, the average optical density for the vehicle group was defined as 100% or 1.0. Optical densities for each member of the D. alfa group were divided by the mean grayscale intensity for the vehicle control group and averaged to yield a mean NADPH-d index for the D. alfa group. Representative images showing the brain regions that were quantified are presented in Figure 2.

Data Analysis

NADPH-d indices for different treatments were compared using the Mann–Whitney U-test (Figure 2). NORT scores and NOx levels were analyzed by two-way analyses of variance (ANOVA) for independent groups as a 2 (genotype: Wild-type, STOP null) × 2 (drug: Vehicle, D. alfa) design followed by Bonferroni post hoc t-tests (Figures 1, 3, and 5). For experiments using only WT mice, the results were analyzed using a one-way ANOVA followed by the Newman-Keuls post test to determine differences between individual groups (Figure 4). A level of p<0.05 was considered statistically significant.

RESULTS

Acute Darbepoetin Alfa Treatment Reversed NORT Deficits in STOP Null Mice

In a first series of experiments, we compared the NORT performance of STOP null and WT mice that received either vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.) or D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.). During the sample phase when animals were exposed to the test objects for the first time, STOP null mice and WT littermates that received either vehicle or D. alfa spent the same amount of time exploring both objects (Figure 1a). In contrast, during the choice phase, STOP mice that received vehicle explored the novel object less than WT littermates, which were injected with vehicle (Figure 1b; p=0.0372). Administration of D. alfa did not alter the proportion of time that WT littermates spent investigating the novel object during the choice phase (Figure 1b). In comparison with vehicle-injected STOP null mice, D. alfa-treated mice spent significantly more time exploring the novel object during the choice phase (Figure 1b; p=0.0204).

Effects of Acute Darbepoetin Alfa NADPH Staining in the Forebrain

It has been reported that local delivery of Epo in the hippocampus increases extracellular concentrations of nitric oxide (Yamamoto et al, 2004) suggesting that Epo activates nitric oxide synthase (NOS). The NADPH-d histochemical technique is based on the presence in certain neurons of NOS that can catalyze the NADPH-dependent conversion of a soluble tetrazolium salt to an insoluble, visible formazan product (Hope et al, 1991). In a second series of studies, we compared the effects of vehicle and D. alfa on NADPH-d staining in the forebrain of WT mice. Relative to vehicle-treated animals, D. alfa produced an increase in NADPH-d staining in the ventral CA1 (Figure 2c and d; arrowheads) and amygdala (medial and cortical aspects) (Figure 2e and f; arrowheads). Image analysis indicated that the intensity of NADPH-d staining (NADPH-d index) in the ventral CA1 was elevated 3 h after D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) by 30% relative to vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.) treated controls (p=0.0091). The NADPH-d index in the medial and cortical aspects of the amygdala was elevated by about 20% in mice that received D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) compared with vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.) controls (Figure 2g; p=0.0260); however, there was not a corresponding increase in NADPH-d staining intensity in dorsal CA1, whole cortex and rhinal cortex (Figure 2g).

Effects of Acute Darbepoetin Alfa Treatment on Nitrate/Nitrite Levels in the Hippocampus and Cortex

NADPH-d staining suggested a single injection of D. alfa increased NOS activity in the ventral hippocampus but not the rhinal cortex (Figure 2g). NO, which is produced by activation of NOS, is an unstable molecule that degrades within seconds once formed, however, its end-products (nitrate and nitrite, NOx) are stable products and reflect NO concentrations in vivo (Moncada et al, 1991). In a third series of studies, we compared the effects of vehicle and D. alfa on NOx levels 3–4 h later in the hippocampus and cortex of STOP null and WT littermates. As shown in Figure 3b, basal NOx levels in hippocampus were comparable in WT and STOP null mice and elevated by more than 70% following D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) in both strains (Figure 3b; WT: p=0.0205, KO: p=0.0007). In contrast, NOx levels in whole cortex were the same in WT and STOP null mice after D. alfa treatment (Figure 3a).

L-NAME Inhibits the Increase in NOx Levels Produced by D. alfa

The results shown in Figures 2 and 3 suggest that D. alfa may increase the NOx levels by activating NOS in the mouse hippocampus. In a fourth series of experiments, we tested this hypothesis by determining whether L-NAME, a potent NOS inhibitor, prevented the increase of NOx induced by D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) in WT mice. We administered L-NAME at doses of 10, 25, or 50 mg/kg (i.p.) 30 min before D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) treatment, and then measured the concentration of NOx in the hippocampus 3–4 h later. As shown in Figure 4a, D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) increased NOx levels in the hippocampus of WT mice by about 70% relative to vehicle-injected controls (Figure 4a; p=0.0454). D. alfa-induced increases in NOx levels were reversed by 25 or 50 mg/kg of L-NAME (Figure 4a; 25 mg/kg: p=0.0116; 50 mg/kg: p=0.0151).

Effects of L-NAME on NORT Performance

In a fifth experiment, we determined the effects of L-NAME (10, 25, and 50 mg/kg, i.p.) on the NORT performance of WT mice. Compared with vehicle-injected mice, L-NAME did not reduce the amount of time that WT mice spent exploring the two test objects during the sample phase (Figure 4b). NORT performance during the choice phase was also not reduced by L-NAME (10, 25, and 50 mg/kg, i.p.) relative to vehicle-injected controls (Figure 4c). These results indicate that a dose of 25 mg/kg (i.p.) of L-NAME did not impair the basal NORT performance of WT mice, but sufficient to completely inhibit the increase in hippocampal NOx levels produced by D. alfa (25 mg/kg, i.p.).

L-NAME Inhibits the Ability of D. alfa to Improve the NORT Performance of STOP Null Mice

Since NO has a role in synaptic connectivity (Boehning and Snyder, 2003; Ota et al, 2008; Wang et al, 2005), we hypothesized that D. alfa may improve NORT performance in STOP null mice by increasing NO level in hippocampus. To test this hypothesis, we performed a sixth experiment to determine if administration of L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) blocked the ability of D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) to improve NORT performance. L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) administration did not influence the exploration times of WT and STOP mice injected with vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.) or D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) during the sample phase (Figure 5a). The ability of D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) to improve the performance of STOP null mice in the choice phase (Figure 1b) was blocked by L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) (Figure 5b; vehicle vs D. alfa in WT p=0.0366, vehicle vs D.alfa in KO p=0.0074). L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) did not alter the NORT performance of WT mice that received vehicle (10 ml/kg, i.p.) (Figure 5b).

L-NAME Inhibits the Ability of D. alfa to Elevate NOx Levels in the Hippocampus

Finally, NOx levels were measured in the hippocampus to confirm that L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) blocked the increase in NOS activity triggered by D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.). D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) produced more than a 70% increase in NOx levels in both WT and STOP null mice (Figure 3b). Injection of L-NAME (25 mg/kg, i.p.) 30 min before D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) completely blocked the ability of D. alfa to increase in NOx levels in both WT and STOP null indicating that NOS activity was blocked by systemic administration of this inhibitor (Figure 5c). Taken together, these results indicate that D. alfa improved the NORT performance of STOP null mice by increasing NO level in the hippocampus.

DISCUSSION

A major finding of this study was that acute injection of D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) reversed NORT performance deficits of STOP null mice that model several features of schizophrenia. There are several reports in the literature indicating that Epo is able to enhance cognitive function in rodents. For example, recombinant human Epo enhances the performance of healthy mice in behavioral tasks designed to assess attention and executive function (Ehrenreich et al, 2004; El-Kordi et al, 2009). Furthermore, we have previously reported that D. alfa improves the NORT performance of adult rats that have sustained lesions in the ventral hippocampus as neonates (postnatal day 7) or young adults (42 days of age) (Hori et al, 2007). This finding is consistent with a recent report that Epo enhances hippocampal LTP and learning in mice (Adamcio et al, 2008). Although D. alfa improved the NORT performance of STOP null mice, it did not enhance the performance of WT mice. This may have been due to a ceiling effect. For example, the NORT performance of WT mice may already be at an optimal level precluding any further improvements by D. alfa. However, this contrasts with a previous study showing that Epo vs placebo administration to well-functioning healthy volunteers was able to improve their neurocognitive function (Miskowiak et al, 2008a, 2008b).

A second major finding of this study was that systemic administration of D. alfa elevated NOx levels in the brain. NO was originally identified as endothelium-derived relaxing factor (Furchgott and Vanhoutte, 1989) and since this seminal observation has been implicated a wide variety of physiological processes underlying learning and memory (Kendrick et al, 1997; Ota et al, 2008). NO can signal across the synapse in both the antero- and retrograde directions to increase levels of the intracellular second messenger cyclic guanosine-3′,5′-monophosphate (cGMP). It has been shown that cGMP activates cGMP-dependent protein kinases implicated in long-term potentiation (Feil and Kleppisch, 2008). Measurement of the nitrate and nitrite metabolites of NO (NOx) has shown that Epo can increase tissue levels of NO in both the peripheral and central nervous system. Transgenic mice that overexpress human Epo show a 3-fold increase in serum levels of NOx (Ruschitzka et al, 2000). In addition, Yamamoto et al, (2004) have reported that local delivery of Epo in the rat hippocampus induces more than a 2-fold increase in extracellular levels of NOx (Yamamoto et al, 2004). Cherian et al, (2007) have found that systemic administration of Epo (5000 U/kg, s.c.; once a day for 3 days before injury) prevents the reduction in extracellular NO concentrations in the brain that occurs after cortical impact injury. In this study, we have shown that NOx levels are elevated in the hippocampus by 1.7 to 2-fold in both WT and STOP null mice 3–4 h after injection of D. alfa (25 μg/kg=5000 U/kg, i.p.) (Figure 3).

In the central nervous system, NO has an important role as an intracellular chemical messenger (Boehning and Snyder, 2003; Dawson and Snyder, 1994; Kiss and Vizi, 2001). There are several reports that NO enhances the release of neurotransmitters implicated in cognitive performance such as acetylcholine and glutamate (Prast and Philippu, 1992, 2001). NO has also been shown to enhance the expression of synaptophysin and GluR1 necessary for synaptic connectivity (Wang et al, 2005). Moreover, it is well-established that NOS inhibitors block LTP (Arancio et al, 1996; Schuman and Madison, 1991), whereas LTP is significantly reduced in mice that lack both endothelial NOS and neuronal NOS (Son et al, 1996). Consistent with the importance of NO for learning and memory, we found that the ability of D. alfa to improve the NORT performance of STOP null mice was associated with elevated NO levels in the hippocampus. Furthermore, L-NAME, an inhibitor of both endothelial NOS and neuronal NOS, attenuated the improvement of NORT performance by D. alfa in STOP null mice, suggesting that NO may mediate the cognitive improvement induced by D. alfa (Figure 5). We did not observe impaired NORT performance in WT mice treated with L-NAME, a potent NOS inhibitor. Tanaka et al, (2009) reported that a high dose of L-NAME (100 mg/kg, i.p.) impaired Y-maze task of mice, whereas lower doses of 10, 30, and 50 mg/kg (i.p.) did not alter performance in this task. It therefore appears that the failure of L-NAME (10–50 mg/kg, i.p.) to inhibit the NORT performance of WT mice in this study was simply dose related.

It is noteworthy that serum levels of NO have been reported to be reduced in schizophrenic patients (Lee and Kim, 2008; Srivastava et al, 2001). Akbarian et al, (1993a, 1993b) have found that some schizophrenic patients show a significant decline in NADPH-d-positive neurons in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, hippocampal formation, and lateral temporal lobe of the neocortex relative to controls matched for gender, age and post-mortem delay. These findings imply that decreased NO signaling in certain parts of the brain may contribute to cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. D. alfa increased NADPH-d staining intensity in the CA1 subregion of the ventral hippocampus suggesting that it may activate regions associated with cognitive function. These results raise the possibility that Epo might compensate for attenuated NO signaling in schizophrenia by enhancing NO production in the CA1 region. Ehrenreich et al, (2007) found that chronic schizophrenic patients receiving recombinant human Epo (40 000 U per week for 12 weeks) displayed a significant improvement in cognitive deficits. Miskowiak et al, (2009) found that a single injection of erythropoietin (40 000 U) not only improved memory in depressed patients, but also enhanced mood and cognitive function in healthy volunteers (Miskowiak et al, 2008a, 2008b). The ability of Epo to improve mood and cognitive function was also associated with an enhanced functional magnetic resonance imaging response in the hippocampus and amygdala (Miskowiak et al, 2007, 2008b). In support of these findings, we found that D. alfa enhanced N staining intensity in the ventral hippocampus and amygdala (Figure 2).

Although Epo appears to enhance cognitive function in psychiatric illness, it can also have serious adverse effects. For example, chronic Epo treatment significantly increases the risk of thrombosis (Singbartl, 1994). Furthermore, many malignant tumors express both Epo and Epo receptor, and Epo-sensitive tumor growth is supported by Epo both in vitro and in vivo (Yasuda et al, 2003). Unfortunately, Ehrenreich et al, (2007) reported that chronic administration of Epo (40 000 U per week for 12 weeks) significantly increased hematocrit and that it was necessary to perform bloodletting on several subjects. In what may be a resolution of this problem, Miskowiak and coworkers reported that a single injection of Epo (40 000 U) did not increase hematocrit within 3 or 7 days in their studies (Miskowiak et al, 2007, 2008a, 2008b, 2009) suggesting that it may be possible to improve cognition at doses that do not raise hematocrit at least 7 days after a single administration of this therapeutic. In this study, we did not determine the duration for which a single injection of D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) improved the NORT performance of STOP null mice. In view of the findings by Miskowiak and coworkers, it would be useful to determine the duration of the effects of D. alfa on the NORT performance of STOP null mice.

In summary, we show that a single injection of D. alfa (25 μg/kg, i.p.) improved the behavioral performance of STOP null mice in the NORT. This improvement was associated with an elevation of tissue levels of NOx in regions of the brain implicated in NORT performance. Furthermore, inhibition of NOS by injection of the inhibitor L-NAME, at a dose that did not impair the basal NORT performance of WT mice, blocked the ability of D. alfa to enhance the NORT performance of STOP null mice. These findings suggest that erythropoietin may improve the cognitive performance of schizophrenics by enhancing NO signaling in regions of the brain associated with learning and memory.