INTRODUCTION

There is an extensive literature highlighting a close relationship between anxiety-like behavior and the mesolimbic dopamine (DA) system. Dopaminergic mechanisms alter behavioral responses to naturally anxiogenic environmental stimuli (Kienast et al, 2008; Talalaenko et al, 1994). In addition, the DA system and anxiety-like behavior are both dramatically altered by early-life stressors (Higley et al, 1991; Matthews et al, 2001) and withdrawal from chronic drug exposure (Finckh et al, 1997; Nath et al, 2000; Yang et al, 1992). Manipulation of the DA system in the amygdala, a central component of the neural circuitry controlling anxiety-related behavior, recapitulates many of these findings (de la Mora et al, 2005; Weiss et al, 2001), suggesting that the amygdala DA system is critical for the expression of anxiety. The neurobiological mechanisms governing these relationships have not been firmly established.

The rat amygdala, a central regulatory component of the limbic system, contains five major nuclei that coordinate a multitude of brain regions responsible for different aspects of affective behaviors like fear/anxiety. The lateral and basolateral subdivisions (basolateral amygdala (BLA)) are the primary ‘input’ nuclei (Pitkanen et al, 1997) and are composed of large, pyramidal-shaped glutamatergic projection neurons. In addition to their excitatory inputs from diverse cortical and thalamic regions, at least two populations of GABAergic interneurons, the lateral paracapsular cells (LPCs) and scattered ‘local’ cells, tightly regulate the activity of BLA principal neurons. LPC GABAergic neurons are concentrated in ‘islands’ found along the lateral border of the BLA and provide feedforward inhibition in response to excitatory cortical inputs (Marowsky et al, 2005). ‘Local’ interneurons provide synchronous feedback inhibition in response to glutamatergic collaterals arising from the principal BLA neurons themselves (Woodruff et al, 2006). This complex BLA GABAergic circuitry controls the acquisition and expression of different measures of conditioned fear (Davis, 2000), the expression of anxiety-like behavior in response to naturally aversive stimuli (Sanders and Shekhar, 1995), and following drug withdrawal (Isoardi et al, 2007). The parallels between the role of these BLA GABAergic interneuron populations and the dopaminergic system suggest a potential interaction between these neurotransmitters. Recent evidence supports this hypothesis and suggests that DA directly acts upon BLA GABAergic neurotransmission. Activation of D2-like receptors (D2Rs) disinhibits the lateral amygdala (LA) in mice (Bissiere et al, 2003) by suppressing GABAergic neurotransmission. These effects may be complex and dependent on the specific GABAergic interneuron population. For example, BLA D1Rs suppress LPC-mediated GABAergic feedforward inhibition (Marowsky et al, 2005) and simultaneously enhance local BLA interneuron excitability (Kroner et al, 2005).

In addition to the expression of D1R and D2R in the BLA, there is some evidence suggesting that DA D3Rs also play a functional role in this brain region. There is marked expression of D3R binding sites within BLA (Camacho-Ochoa et al, 1995), and microinjection of the D3R antagonist nafadotride into the BLA suppresses amphetamine-enhanced appetitive conditioning (Phillips et al, 2002). These data suggest a functional role for D3Rs in the BLA. Unlike DA D1R and D2R, the physiological role of BLA D3 receptors has not been explored. In this study, we examine the neurobiological mechanisms that underlie the behavioral effects of BLA D3Rs using both behavioral pharmacology and in vitro physiology.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

All experiments used male Sprague–Dawley rats (Harlan, Indianapolis, IN). The WFUSM Animal Care and Use Committee Experimental approved all animal procedures in accordance with the NIH Guidelines for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

Surgical Procedure and Microinjections

Using previously published procedures (Lack et al, 2007), we anesthetized adult rats (300 g, n=24) with pentobarbital and implanted with guide cannulae directed at the dorsal aspect of the LA according to the following stereotaxic coordinates (millimeters relative to Bregma; Paxinos and Watson, 1997): −2.80 anterior/posterior, +5.05 lateral on the right, +5.25 lateral on the left, and −6.20 dorsal/ventral from the top of the brain. During a 5-day recovery period, animals were extensively handled and repeatedly exposed to the manipulations associated with the microinjection procedure. On injection days, injection cannulae were placed such that their tips extended 1 mm below the ventral aspect of the guide cannulae, and animals received 0.5 μl of either of the D3 receptor antagonists, U99194 or GR103691, delivered over a 30 s period. Injection cannulae were left in place for an additional 1 min before being removed. Animals receiving the sham injection were subjected to the same procedure, but were injected with artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF, see below). Animals received three microinjections, once a week, for three consecutive weeks, and were exposed to a different behavioral apparatus each (see below). Animals were counter-balanced across injection days such that they never received the same dose of U99194 or GR103691 (or sham) across the three injection weeks. Placement of the guide cannulae was confirmed post mortem by preparing coronal slices from fixed tissue. Tip placements are shown in Figure 1a. Tip locations from adjacent anteroposterior slices were combined for simplicity. Four out of 24 surgeries resulted in cannulae placements outside the target area for the U99194 experiments and seven out of 22 surgeries in the GR103691 experiments; these ‘miss’ animals are not included in the analysis.

Figure 1
figure 1

U99194 effects on anxiety-like behavior expressed in a light/dark box. The approximate location of guide cannulae tips are indicated by the closed circles (a). Anterior-to-posterior stereotaxic coordinates of representative coronal brain slices are shown at left (Paxinos and Watson, 1997). (b) U99194 did not significantly affect the number of movements in the light/dark arena (n=6 animals for artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) group, n=8 for 1.5 pmol group, and n=6 for 5 pmol group, P>0.05, one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA)). Intra-basolateral amygdala (BLA) U99194 increased time spent in light side compared with vehicle-injected (c) and increased number of transitions from dark to light side (d). U99194 also decreased re-entry latency to the dark side compared with vehicle injected (e). Newman–Keuls post-test compared the individual columns: *P<0.05, **P<0.01, and ***P<0.001 compared with sham; ##P<0.01 compared with the 1.5 pmol dose.

PowerPoint slide

Behavioral Assays

To assess the effects of U99194 and GR103691 microinjected into the BLA, we employed two different anxiety assays and a separate apparatus to assess locomotion. For the light/dark box (Isoardi et al, 2007; McCool et al, 2003), individuals were placed in the ‘light’ side of a 40 cm × 40 cm plexiglass arena divided equally into ‘light’ and ‘dark’ sides by an opaque plexiglass insert (Rat TruScan Activity Arena, Coulbourn Instruments, Allentown, PA). The ‘light side’ of the arena was illuminated by ambient room lighting to ∼400 lx. Two infrared sensor rings surrounded the entire apparatus, one in the floor plane and one located ∼15 cm above the floor to measure rearing behavior. Data were collected for 5 min at 0.25 Hz and analyzed for the total number of moves, the time to leave the light side following initial placement (egress latency), the time to return to the light side after initial entry to the dark side (re-entry latency), the total time spent in the light and dark compartments, the number of light–dark transitions, and the number of vertical beam breaks. Between animals, the apparatus was cleaned with warm water, 70% ethanol, again with warm water, and thoroughly dried.

In the second microinjection week, sessions were performed using the elevated plus maze (Pellow et al, 1985) with incandescent lighting (∼40 lx on the open arms). The apparatus consists of open and closed runways (20 in × 4 in) that set atop a 28.5 in (72.4 cm) high aluminum frame (Med Associates, St Albans, VT). Animal entry into open and closed arms were detected with infrared sensors attached at the opening to each arm; data acquisition was achieved with a personal computer interfaced with control units and programmed using MED-PC (Med Associates). Dependent variables included the total amount of time an animal spent in each arm, the total amount of time the animal spent at the central junction, and the number of transitions between arms. At the beginning of each 5-min session, individuals were placed at the junction of the open and closed arms facing an open arm. Between animals, the apparatus was cleaned with warm water, 70% ethanol, again with warm water, and thoroughly dried.

Locomotor activity was measured in an open field on the third week (Steiner et al, 1997). Animals were placed in the center of a 40 cm × 40 cm plexiglass arena (Rat TruScan Activity Arena), and the number of movements, movement distance, and move time was recorded in the dark for 5 min using infrared sensor rings.

Slice Preparation

For the in vitro electrophysiology experiments, coronal amygdala slices were prepared from male Sprague–Dawley rats (100–150 g, 3–5 weeks of age) as described previously (Isoardi et al, 2007; McCool et al, 2003). Slices were submerged in room temperature (∼25°C), oxygenated aCSF containing (in mM): 126 NaCl, 3 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 2 MgSO4, 26 NaHCO3, 10 glucose, and 2 CaCl2·2H2O. Slices were maintained in aCSF for ∼1 h before recording. All experiments were performed 1–4 h after preparation of the BLA slices.

Whole-Cell Patch-Clamp Recording

Methods for whole-cell voltage clamp recording BLA GABAergic neurotransmission were similar to those reported previously (DuBois et al, 2006; Isoardi et al, 2007). Briefly, slices were placed in a recording chamber that was continuously perfused with room-temperature aCSF at a rate of 2 ml/min. Input resistance was 8–12 MΩ when electrodes were filled with an internal solution containing (in mM): 122 Cs-gluconate, 10 CsCl, 10 HEPES, 1 EGTA, 5 NaCl, 0.1 CaCl2, 4 Mg-ATP, 0.3 Na-GTP, and 2 QX314-(Cl), pH 7.25 and osmolarity 280–290 mOsm. Data were acquired with an Axopatch 200B amplifier (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA) and stored for later analysis using ClampEx software (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA). GABAA-mediated synaptic currents were pharmacologically isolated using 6,7-dinitroquinoxaline-2,3(1H,4H)-dione (DNQX, 20 μM, an AMPA/kainate receptor antagonist; Sigma-Aldrich, St Louis, MO), DL-2-amino-5-phosphono-pentanoic acid (APV, 50 μM, an NMDA receptor antagonist; Sigma-Aldrich), and the GABAB antagonists SCH50911 (10 μM) or CGP55845 (10 μM). Glutamatergic synaptic currents were pharmacologically isolated using the GABAA antagonist bicuculline methochloride (10 μM) and SCH50911. Only neurons with a high membrane capacitance (>100 pF) and low input resistance (<30MΩ) were considered presumptive principal neurons (Faber et al, 2001; Kroner et al, 2005; Schiess et al, 1993; Washburn and Moises, 1992) and were included in this study.

Electrically evoked GABA synaptic response was stimulated using a range of intensities to achieve an average synaptic response of ∼100 pA with a holding potential at −10 mV. The stimulating electrodes were placed in the external capsule (to activate LPCs) and medial to the recording electrode (to activate local interneurons) as specified within each experiment. For experiments measuring glutamatergic neurotransmission, the holding potential was −60 mV and a stimulating electrode was placed in the LA to electrically activate glutamatergic projections as reported previously (Isoardi et al, 2007). Slices were perfused with a D3R agonist PD128907 and/or the D3-selective receptor antagonists such as U99194 maleate, GR103691, or a D2-selective receptor antagonist such as L741626 (Tocris Bioscience, Ellisville, MO). To block dynamin-dependent endocytosis of GABAA receptors, dynamin inhibitory peptide (p4) or scrambled control peptide (Tocris) was included in the intracellular pipette solution.

Paired-Pulse Ratio

Paired-pulse ratio (PPR) was measured with two equal-intensity electrical stimulations with a 50- or 250-ms inter-pulse interval. A ratio of the amplitudes of the evoked inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSCs) was taken (second amplitude divided by the first). Measurements were taken before (baseline) and during maximal drug effect from both GABAergic interneuron populations. All values are expressed as mean±SEM. PPR data were subjected to a paired t-test with P<0.05 considered statistically significant.

Spontaneous GABAergic Synaptic Events

Miniature IPSCs (mIPSCs) were acquired at 20 kHz, and were filtered at 2 kHz. For this set of experiments, an internal solution consisting of (in mM): 135 KCl, 10 HEPES, 2MgCl2, 0.5 EGTA, 5 Mg-ATP, 1 Na-GTP, and 1 QX314-(Cl), pH 7.25 and osmolarity 280–290 mOsm. Following isolation of GABAergic transmission using DNQX and APV (as used with electrically evoked experiments) and complete attenuation of evoked responses with 1 μM tetrodotoxin (TTX) (94.38±3.53% inhibition), mIPSCs were recorded for 1 min (baseline) and for 1 min after a 10 min PD128907 application. Access resistance was measured throughout recordings and cells with changes in access resistance >20% were not included in the analysis. These experiments were performed at a holding potential of −60 mV. Event amplitude and frequency were analyzed using MiniAnalysis (SynaptSoft Inc). Median values from individual cells were averaged across treatment groups (Isoardi et al, 2007) and reported as mean±SEM.

Western Blots

Lysis buffer (7 μl/mg tissue; 10 mM Tris, pH 7.4, 2% sodium dodecyl sulfate, 1 mM EDTA, pH 8, and protease inhibitors for mammalian tissue (Sigma, St Louis, MO)) was added to the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) and BLA was dissected from coronal slices, and the tissue was disrupted by brief sonication and incubated at 4°C on a rotisserie mixer for 2 h. Protein yield was quantified using the BCA assay (Thermal Scientific, Rockford, IL). Twenty micrograms of total protein was loaded onto 8–16% sodium dodecyl sulfate precast polyacrylamide gels (Thermal Scientific), separated by electrophoresis, and transferred to a nitrocellulose membrane (Hybond N; Amersham, Piscataway, NJ). The membrane was blocked with 5% non-fat dry milk (NFM) in Tris-buffered saline (TBS-T; 150 mM NaCl, 5.2 mM Na2HPO4, 1.7 mM KH2PO4, 0.05% Tween-20). Blots were incubated overnight at 4°C in TBS-T/1% NFM containing 8.5 μg/ml rabbit anti-rat D3 DA receptor affinity-purified polyclonal antibody (no. AB1786P; Millipore, Temecula, CA). Following extensive washing with TBS-T, the blots were exposed to a goat anti-rabbit, HRP-labeled secondary antibody (1 : 3000 dilution; Sigma) for 1.5 h at room temperature with agitation. Detection of bound secondary antibody was performed using SuperSignal West Dura Extended Duration Substrate (Thermal Scientific). Band intensity was quantified from digital images captured on a CCD camera using a Bio Rad ChemiDoc XRS Imaging System with Quantity One Analysis Software (Hercules, CA).

Statistics

Behavioral data were analyzed using a standard one-way ANOVA. Electrically evoked and miniature IPSC data were analyzed with either un-paired or paired t-tests whenever appropriate. P<0.05 was considered statistically significant.

RESULTS

BLA D3Rs Modulate Anxiety-Like Behaviors

Recent reports of D3R modulation of BLA-dependent conditioned behaviors (Phillips et al, 2002) and the reduced anxiety-like behavior in D3R knockout mice (Steiner et al, 1997) suggested to us that these receptors might modulate native responses to emotionally relevant stimuli. To test this hypothesis, we examined the effects of two D3R-selective antagonists, U99194 (14-fold affinity for D3 : D2) and GR103691 (60-fold affinity for D3 : D2) (Audinot et al, 1998). We microinjected 1.5 and 5 pmol of the U99194 or aCSF bilaterally into the BLA of 24 animals. In a separate set of animals (n=22), we microinjected 0.5 pmol of the GR103691 or aCSF bilaterally into the BLA. Post hoc anatomical analysis revealed that guide cannulae were implanted outside the target area in four out of 23 surgeries in the U99194 experiments (Figure 1a) and in seven out of 22 surgeries in the GR103691 experiments (not shown). These ‘miss’ animals are included as ‘site controls’ in a separate data analysis.

When tested in the light/dark box (Figure 1a), intra-BLA U99194 significantly increased both the total time spent on the light side (Figure 1c: aCSF—15.4±5.6 s, n=6; 1.5 pmol U99194—86.4±24.8 s, n=8; 5 pmol U99194—142.3±16.8 s, n=6; out of 300 s total; one-way ANOVA (P<0.01); F=9.4 (Newman–Keuls P<0.05)) and the total number of transitions between the light and dark sides of the apparatus (Figure 1d: aCSF—1.0±0.3; 1.5 pmol U99194—3.6±0.8; 5 pmol U99194—6.8±0.9; one-way ANOVA (P<0.0001); F=16.5 (Newman–Keuls, aCSF vs 1.5 and 5 pmol P<0.001, 1.5 vs 5 pmol P<0.01)). U99194 modestly increased the egress latency at both the 1.5 pmol (33.8±10.5 s) and 5 pmol doses (36.2±5.0 s) relative to sham (15.7±5.8 s), but this did not reach statistical significance. U99194 had no significant effects on either the total number of moves in the apparatus (Figure 1b) or the number of vertical rears (not shown).

Similar to U99194, intra-BLA microinjection of GR103691 significantly increased the total time spent on the light side of the light/dark box (aCSF—13.7±3.2 s, n=7; 0.5 pmol GR103691—69.5±17.93 s, n=7; out of 300 s total; F=30.65, P<0.01, t-test). GR103691 slightly increased the total number of transitions between the light and dark sides (aCSF—2.4±0.08; 0.5 pmol—4.3±1.3; F=2.26, P>0.05, t-test) without reaching statistical significance, but did significantly increase the egress latency (aCSF—13.3±3.3 s; 0.5 pmol—32.00±4.1 s; F=1.57, P<0.01, t-test). Similar to U99194, GR103691 did not significantly alter the total number of moves or the number of vertical rears (not shown).

The light/dark box data suggested that intra-BLA microinjections of a D3R antagonist reduced anxiety-like behaviors. To confirm this, we also measured the effects of bilateral U99194 and GR103691 BLA microinjection in the elevated plus maze. Intra-BLA U99194 (Table 1) significantly increased the time spent on the open arms (F=15.7, P<0.001) and significantly decreased time on the closed arms (F=9.7, P<0.01) without altering the time spent in the junction between the open and closed arms (F=0.04; P>0.05) during the 300 second assay. Likewise, intra-BLA U99194 significantly increased the number of open-arm entries whether these data were expressed as the total number of open-arm entries (F=20.4, P<0.001) or as the percentage of open-arm entries (open-arm entries/total arm entries; F=10.8, P<0.001). Newman–Keuls post-tests suggested that all these effects were all dose-dependent (see Table 1). Intra-BLA U99194 did not significantly alter the number of closed-arm entries (F=0.08, P>0.05). Intra-BLA GR103691 likewise significantly increased the time on the open arms (aCSF=1.37±1.37 s; 0.5 pmol=19.44±3.58 s; F=9.5, P<0.01, t-test) without changing the time on the closed arms (aCSF=247.8±15.91 s; 0.5 pmol=222.1±15.80 s; F=1.3, P>0.05, t-test), junction time (aCSF=51.02±15.07 s; 0.5 pmol=47.31±10.15 s; F=1.8, P>0.05, t-test), the total number of open-arm entries (aCSF=0.67±0.67; 0.5 pmol=1.75±0.41; F=1.9, P>0.05, t-test), or the total number of closed-arm entries (aCSF=9.16±2.79; 0.5 pmol=8.0±1.76; F=1.9, P>0.05, t-test).

Table 1 Effects of Intra-BLA U99194 Microinjection in the Elevated Plus Maze

Data from both the light/dark box and the plus maze suggest that intra-BLA U99194 did not substantially alter locomotor behaviors. To examine this directly, we tested the effects of U99194 microinjection on open-field behavior (Table 2). Intra-BLA U99194 did not alter the total number of moves (F=0.06, P>0.05, one-way ANOVA), the total movement distance (F=1.00, P>0.05), the amount of time spent moving (F=0.33, P>0.05), or the number of vertical rears (F=0.9, P>0.05). Likewise, GR103691 had no effect on the number of moves (aCSF=78.17±3.41; 0.5 pmol=73.16±3.55; F=1.08, P>0.05, t-test), the total movement distance (aCSF=1613.95±59.81 cm; 0.5 pmol=1820.46±102.69 cm; F=2.94, P>0.05), the amount of time spent moving (aCSF=230.91±4.12 s; 0.5 pmol=238.58±5.26 s; F=1.62, P>0.05), or the number of vertical rears (aCSF=30.67±3.99; 0.5 pmol=26.5±3.34; F=1.43, P>0.05). Taken together, these data suggest that blockade of BLA D3Rs with the selective antagonists U99194 and GR103691 can produce anxiolytic effects without significant alterations of locomotor-related behaviors.

Table 2 Effects of Intra-BLA U99194 Microinjection on Open-Field Behavior

In a separate analysis, we analyzed data from ‘miss’ animals to further determine the contribution of the BLA to the behavioral measures. When comparing sham-treated to ‘miss’ animals, we found no significant difference between the two groups in time in the light side of the light/dark box (P>0.05, F=1.56, t-test) or time in the open arms of the plus maze (P>0.05, F=4.84, t-test). Misses were typically located dorsal and lateral to the BLA (striatum and entorhinal cortex, respectively, not shown). This analysis suggests that the anxiolytic effect of the D3R antagonist microinjection is specific to intra-BLA delivery.

LPC and Local GABAergic Synapses can be Functionally Distinguished

Recent results suggest that DA-dependent signaling in the BLA specifically targets GABAergic transmission (Bissiere et al, 2003; Kroner et al, 2005; Marowsky et al, 2005). However, BLA GABAergic neurons are represented by ‘local’ feedback interneurons scattered throughout the nucleus (McDonald et al, 2005) and ‘islands’ of feedforward interneurons scattered along the lateral/paracapsular border of the BLA (Fuxe et al, 2003; Marowsky et al, 2005). Anatomical data suggests that the local feedback interneurons synapse on the soma and proximal dendritic compartments of principal neurons (McDonald and Betette, 2001), whereas LPC feedforward interneurons synapse onto distal dendritic processes (Marowsky et al, 2005; Muller et al, 2007). This anatomical arrangement suggests that these synapses might be functionally independent. To test this, we preformed a synaptic ‘collision’ experiment (Szinyei et al, 2000; Xiang et al, 2002) using whole-cell patch-clamp electrophysiology of BLA principal neurons. When paired electrical stimuli are delivered to BLA GABAergic inputs using a long inter-stimulus interval (250 ms in this case; Silberman et al, 2008), the second synaptic response is typically smaller than the first. This ‘paired-pulse depression’ is thought to reflect feedback inhibition by presynaptic autoreceptors (Silberman et al, 2009). If local and LPC GABAergic inputs are functionally distinct, the mechanisms controlling paired-pulse depression at one synapse should not affect the other. When stimulating either the local or the LPC GABAergic synapses (Figure 2a), paired electrical stimuli caused substantial inhibition of the second synaptic response relative to the first (Figure 2b1 and c). Conversely, stimulation of one GABAergic input followed 250 ms later by stimulation of the other input caused no paired-pulse depression (Figure 2b2 and c; n=7 cells at each stimulation site or group). These data suggest that the GABAergic synapses from the two interneuron populations can be functionally separated by the placement of the stimulating electrode.

Figure 2
figure 2

γ-Aminobutyric acid (GABA)ergic synaptic responses from different basolateral amygdala (BLA) interneuron populations can be functionally distinguished. (a) Schematic diagram of the BLA indicating the approximate anatomical relationships between stimulating electrodes (black bars) placed near the external capsule to stimulate feedforward inhibitory interneurons (lateral paracapsular cells (LPC)) and within the nucleus to stimulate local feedback inhibitory interneurons (L). The recording electrode (hollow pipette) is placed between the stimulating electrodes. The coronal slice was adapted from Paxinos and Watson (1997). Sample traces illustrate paired-pulse depression of inhibitory postsynaptic current (IPSC) amplitude (b1; L2/L1 and LPC2/LPC1) during a paired stimulation of the same anatomical site using an inter-stimulus interval of 250 ms. Pronounced paired-pulse depression characterized both the LPC and local GABAergic synapses. In contrast, when these anatomical sites are stimulated consecutively (b2; un-paired stimulus), there was little paired-pulse depression. Both traces are from the same cell and exemplify the experimental approach of alternating stimulation sites to calculate the paired-pulse depression ratio. A graphical summary (c) of the percent suppression of the second response compared with the first response during the PP250 when a single anatomical site is stimulated (‘paired’; n=7 cells at each stimulation site) compared with sequential stimulation of the two different stimulation sites (‘un-paired’; n=7 in each group). *P<0.05, t-test between the different stimulation sites.

PowerPoint slide

To further confirm that we can independently activate GABAergic synapses arising from these two interneuron populations, we employed BRL 37344, a selective β3-adrenergic receptor agonist, which has recently been shown to robustly potentiate LPC GABAergic synapses without affecting local synapses (Silberman et al, 2010). We found that BRL 37344 (10 μM) significantly potentiated the LPC GABA-IPSC (39.93±8.24% from baseline, n=6). In contrast, BRL 37344 did not significantly alter the local IPSC compared with baseline (6.72±7.12% from baseline, n=6, P<0.05, t-test; data not shown). These pharmacological data provide further evidence that LPC and local GABAergic synapses can be independently activated using placement of the stimulating electrodes.

D3-Like DA Receptors Modulate BLA GABAergic Transmission

Given the anxiolytic effects of the D3R antagonists when injected into the BLA, we first examined the effects of D3R-like activity on synaptic transmission within the BLA. The D3-selective agonist PD128907 (Audinot et al, 1998) significantly inhibited the amplitude of electrically evoked GABAergic IPSCs elicited from both the LPC and local synapses (Figure 3a). The inhibition of GABAergic transmission by 10 μM PD128907 did not recover during an extensive washout period (Figure 3b). Despite this, the pipette resistance did not significantly change during the duration of these recordings. At time=5 min, series resistance was 24.4±3.1 MΩ, and at time=35 min, it was 24.8±3.0 MΩ (P>0.05, paired t-test). These data together suggest that PD128907 modulation of LPC and local GABAergic synaptic responses was irreversible. When measured at the end of the drug application period (dashed line, Figure 3b), 10 μM PD128907 inhibited LPC IPSCs by 35.7±4.1% (n=9) and local IPSCs by 33.3±5.2% (n=6). PD128907 (1 μM) inhibited LPC GABAergic responses by 23.0±6.6% (n=6) and local responses by 25.8±6.6% (n=5). And, 10 nM PD128907 inhibited LPC synapses by 4.1±3.9% (n=5) and local synapses by 5.5±1.9% (n=6). One-way ANOVA analysis of these data found that the magnitude of PD128907 inhibition was significantly concentration dependent (Figure 3c) at both the local (F=8.3, P<0.01) and LPC synapses (F=10.9, P<0.001), with the modulation by both the 1 and 10 μM being significantly greater than the 10 nM concentration (*P<0.05, **P<0.01; Newman–Keuls post-test).

Figure 3
figure 3

A D3 dopamine receptor agonist suppresses γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA)ergic synaptic transmission at both local (left) and lateral paracapsular cell (LPC) (right) interneuron synapses in the basolateral amygdala (BLA). Exemplar traces (a) illustrate the robust modulation of GABAergic inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSCs) by the D3R agonist PD128907. The effects of 10 μM PD128907 (b) were slow onset and did not appear to be reversible during a prolonged washout period (n=6 cells for both 0.01 and 10 μM PD128907 at the local synapses, n=5 for 0.01 μM and n=9 for 10 μM at the LPC synapses). Regardless, inhibition of both local and LPC GABAergic responses by PD128907 was concentration dependent (c), measured at the end of the drug application period indicated by the dashed vertical line in (b). Averaged data in bar graphs represents data from the last 5 min (averaged into 1 min bins) of the drug application. *P<0.05 for comparison between 0.01 μM and 1 μM; **P<0.01 between 0.01 μM and 10 μM. (d) Robust immunoreactivity from BLA samples (n=4) using a specific D3R antibody. **P<0.01. Samples from nucleus accumbens (NAcc) (n=4) had a significantly stronger immunoreactive signal. Bands from western blots are shown below the corresponding column in the bar graph.

PowerPoint slide

This D3-like modulation of GABAergic transmission within the BLA suggests that the D3 DA receptors are expressed in this brain region. To confirm this, we used a D3R-specific antibody with no known cross-reactivity with D2 DA receptors and found robust D3R-like immunoreactivity from BLA samples (Figure 3d, n=4). We compared BLA D3R immunoreactivity with that found in the NAcc (n=4), as it has been shown to have high expression of D3Rs (Guitart-Masip et al, 2006; Le Foll et al, 2005). Consistent with this, we found significantly higher levels of D3-like immunoreactivity in the NAcc compared with the BLA (F=1.57, P<0.01, t-test). Along with the PD128907 modulation, these biochemical data suggest that D3-like DA receptors can be found within the BLA.

To confirm the receptor responsible for PD128907 modulation, we employed the two D3 receptor antagonists used in our behavioral experiments along with a highly potent D2-selective antagonist. In the presence of U99194, PD128097 inhibited local synapses by only 5.4±7.6% (Figure 4e, n=6) compared with 33.3±5.2% inhibition with PD128907 alone (n=9, data from Figure 3c; one-way ANOVA, F=5.58 (Newman–Keuls P<0.05)). When slices were pretreated with GR103691 (Figure 4a and b), PD128907 inhibited the local synapses by 8.9±5.6% (n=6; one-way ANOVA, Newman–Keuls P<0.05). In contrast, the D2R antagonist, L741626, only slightly diminished the PD128907-mediated inhibition to 26.8±4.4% (Figure 4c and d; n=5, one-way ANOVA, P>0.05). Similarly, at LPC synapses, PD128907+U99194 (Figure 4e, n=6) inhibited GABAergic responses by 12.5±4.6% compared to 35.7±4.1% with PD128907 alone (one-way ANOVA, F=11.26 (Newman–Keuls P<0.01)). GR103691 almost completely blocked the effect of PD128907 at LPC synapses (Figure 4a and b), only permitting PD128907 to inhibit 1.8±3.5% (n=6; one-way ANOVA, F=8.59 (Newman–Keuls P<0.001)). As with the local synapses, L741626 could not block the inhibitory effect of PD128907 at LPC synapses (Figure 4c and d; 27.1±5.9%, n=6, P>0.05 compared with PD128907).

Figure 4
figure 4

Dopamine D3, but not D2, receptor antagonists attenuate the effect of PD128907 at both local and lateral paracapsular cell (LPC) interneuron synapses in the basolateral amygdala (BLA). Exemplar traces (a) show that PD128907 has little to no effect on γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA)ergic inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSCs) when pretreated with the selective D3R antagonist GR103691. With GR103691 on the slice, PD128907 (10 μM) had no inhibitory effect on GABAergic IPSCs following a 14 min wash (b; n=6 for both local and LPC synapses). Exemplar traces (c) show that in slices pretreated with the selective D2R antagonist L714626, PD128907 robustly modulated GABAergic IPSCs. In contrast to GR103691, with L714626 on the slice, PD128907 (10 μM) inhibited GABAergic IPSCs following a 14 min wash (d; n=5 for local and n=6 for LPC synapses). PD128907 (10 μM) inhibition (e) was significantly attenuated by the D3R antagonists U99194 (3 μM) and GR103691 (1 μM), but not by the D2R antagonist L741626 (50 nM). Averaged data in bar graphs represents data from the last 5 min (averaged into 1 min bins) of steady drug effect. *P<0.05 between PD and PD+U99194 groups; **P<0.01 between PD and PD+GR for LPC IPSC.

PowerPoint slide

U99194 (3 μM) alone did not alter synaptic responses at either the LPC (2.9±6.4% inhibition, n=5) or the local (0.0±7.4% inhibition, n=5) GABAergic synapses. GR103691 (1 μM) also did not affect synaptic responses at LPC (−3.7±2.8% inhibition, n=6) or local (6.2±5.5% inhibition, n=6) synapses. Likewise, 50 nM L741616 did not alter synaptic transmission at LPC (2.4±7.1% inhibition, n=6) or local (8.4±11.6 inhibition, n=6) synapses. These data together support the hypothesis that D3Rs, but not D2-like, modulate GABAergic synapses at two BLA interneuron populations.

D3Rs Modulate BLA GABAergic Synapses via both Post- and Presynaptic Mechanisms

To examine the mechanism responsibe for the D3-like modulation of the BLA GABAergic system, we examined the effects of PD128907 on paired-pulse facilitation at LPC and local GABAergic synapses. Using the dual stimulation protocol (Figure 5), we found that PD128907 had no significant effect on PPRs from LPC synapses (Figure 4c and d; PP50: baseline=0.07±0.19, PD128907=0.02±0.19; PP250: baseline=−0.52±0.03, PD128907=−0.50±0.09, n=6, P>0.05, paired t-test at both inter-stimulus intervals). However, PD128907 increased the PPRs at the 50-ms stimulus interval at the local synapses (Figure 4a and b; PP50: baseline=0.05±0.15, PD128907=0.18±0.16; n=7, P<0.05, paired t-test), but did not significantly alter the ratio at the 250-ms stimulus interval (baseline=−0.24±0.15, PD128907=−0.17±0.10; n=7, P>0.05, paired t-test). PD128907 inhibition of the initial synaptic response of the paired stimuli was 28±2% at local synapses and 37±4% at LPC synapses in this experiment. These data suggest that D3R modulation of GABAergic transmission might involve presynaptic mechanisms at the local feedback synapses, but not at the feedforward LPC synapses.

Figure 5
figure 5

Basolateral amygdala (BLA) D3-like receptors (D3Rs) differentially modulate γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA)ergic release at BLA local and lateral paracapsular cell (LPC) GABAergic synapses. Paired electrical stimuli were delivered to either local (left, a) or LPC (right, c) GABAergic synapses using either 50- or 250-ms (not shown) inter-stimulus intervals. In the insets, the amplitude of the first synaptic response in the presence of PD128907 has been normalized to the baseline inhibitory postsynaptic current (IPSC) amplitude to illustrate the relative effects of the D3 receptor agonist on the two synaptic responses. At the 50-ms stimulus interval, the ratio of the IPSC amplitude generated from the second stimulus relative to the first stimulus was significantly altered by PD128907 at local synapses (b; n=7, *P<0.05, t-test), but not at LPC synapses (d; n=6, P>0.05). The IPSC amplitude ratios were not altered by PD128907 at the 250-ms stimulus interval at either synapse.

PowerPoint slide

To test this hypothesis, we measured the effect of PD128907 on miniature spontaneous IPSCs using 1 μM TTX in the extracellular solution. Recent pharmacological evidence suggests that mIPSCs recorded from principal BLA neurons arise primarily from local, but not LPC synapses (Silberman et al, 2009). We therefore recorded mIPSCs before and after the addition of 10 μM PD128907 to the extracellular solution (Figure 6a). PD128907 significantly reduced the mIPSC interevent interval (Figure 6c) from 226±20 to 280±29 ms (n=6; P<0.05, paired t-test). PD128907 also significantly inhibited mIPSC amplitude (Figure 6b) in these same neurons, reducing amplitude from 25.2±2.0 to 17.3±0.7 pA (P<0.01, paired t-test). The mIPSC frequency data may be consistent with the paired-pulse data and suggests that D3Rs reduce GABA release at the local synapses. However, the mIPSC amplitude data, along with the absence of any effect on PPRs at the LPC synapses, suggests that D3Rs may also modulate local and LPC synapses via a postsynaptic mechanism as well.

Figure 6
figure 6

The D3 receptor agonist PD128907 alters both the frequency and the amplitude of spontaneous, tetrodotoxin-resistant inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSCs). Miniature IPSCs (mIPSCs) (a) were recorded in the presence of tetrodotoxin and high concentrations of intracellular chloride (see Materials and Methods). For an exemplar cell, PD128907 significantly shifted the cumulative probability distribution of mIPSC amplitude to the left (b1; KS statistic=0.23, P<0.01) and the mIPSC interevent interval to the right (c1; KS=0.18, P<0.05). Summary graphs across all cells (n=5) shows that PD128907 significantly inhibited both mIPSC amplitude (b2; **P<0.01, paired t-test) and mIPSC frequency (c2; *P<0.05, paired t-test).

PowerPoint slide

It has been previously reported that D3R activation can increase GABAAR endocytosis in the hippocampus (Hammad and Wagner, 2006; Swant et al, 2008) and the NAcc (Chen et al, 2006). To test whether similar mechanisms are responsible for D3R postsynaptic modulation in the BLA, we dialyzed cells with either a dynamin-inhibitory peptide (‘P4’, Gln-Val-Pro-Ser-Arg-Pro-Asn-Arg-Ala-Pro; 50 μM) or with a control ‘scrambled’ peptide (Gln-Pro-Pro-Ala-Ser-Asn-Pro-Arg-Val-Arg; 50 μM) added to the intracellular recording solution. The ‘P4’ peptide is analogous to the region in dynamin that acts as a binding site for amphiphysin (Grabs et al, 1997) and can block clathrin-dependent internalization of GABAA receptors (Kittler et al, 2000). Following opening the cell membrane in the whole-cell configuration and a brief 10 min period to allow distribution of a peptide throughout the intracellular compartment, we collected baseline-evoked responses for 7 min and subsequently measured the modulation of local and LPC-evoked IPSCs by the application of PD128907 (1 μM). In different groups of cells, we found that intracellular dialysis with the ‘P4’ peptide attenuated PD128907 inhibition of GABA-IPSCs relative to the control peptide. PD128907 inhibited ‘local’ IPSCs (Figure 7a) by 23.1±3.7% (n=8) with the control peptide in the intracellular solution, but only by 5.5±2.6% (n=8, P<0.01, t-test) when BLA neurons were perfused with the ‘P4’ peptide. Similarly at the LPC synapses, ‘P4’ peptide significantly reduced the inhibition by PD128907 (Figure 7a) from 30.3±4.4% (n=8 with control peptide) to 4.6±4.4% (n=7, P<0.01). These data, along with the irreversible inhibition by PD128907 (Figure 3), suggest that D3-like modulation of BLA GABAergic responses involves dynamin-dependent process, presumably related to the internalization of GABAA receptors by D3-mediated signaling. To test this, we reasoned that PD128907 modulation should NOT be reversed with a subsequent application of an antagonist. In a different set of cells, we established a stable PD128907 inhibition of GABA-IPSCs and then applied U99194 for 30 min. We found that PD128907 inhibition could not be reversed by the antagonist at either local (Figure 7c, PD128907=28.22±3.31% baseline, U99194=38.97±6.43% baseline, n=4) or LPC synapses (Figure 7d, PD128907=33.75±5.34% baseline, U99194=42.94±7.03% baseline, n=4). These data are consistent with a long-lasting D3-like modulation related to dynamin-mediated endocytosis of postsynaptic GABAA receptors.

Figure 7
figure 7

D3-like receptor (D3R) modulation of basolateral amygdala (BLA) γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA)ergic inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSCs) is dependent on a dynamin-mediated process that is not readily reversible. At both local (left, a) and lateral paracapsular cell (LPC) (right, b) inhibitory synapses, 1 μM PD128907 substantially suppressed the amplitude of electrically evoked responses (a1 and b1) when a control ‘scramble’ peptide was included in the intracellular recording solution. In a separate group of neurons, intracellular perfusion with a peptide (P4) that disrupts dynamin-mediated endocytosis disrupted PD128907 inhibition at both local (a2) and LPC GABAergic synapses (b2). Summary graphs of these data for both local (a3) and LPC (b3) responses show that the P4 peptide significantly attenuated PD128907 inhibition relative to recordings with the control peptide (**P<0.01, t-test). The numbers of cells in each group are indicated within parentheses. In a separate experiment, the inhibitory effect of PD128907 was not reversed by a 30 min application of U99194 at both local (c1 and c2) and LPC (d1 and d2) synapses following the agonist application. Summary graphs for these data indicate that U99194 had no significant effect on the inhibitory effect of PD128907 (P>0.05, t-test).

PowerPoint slide

D3Rs Do Not Modulate BLA Glutamatergic Synaptic Transmission

To examine any potential involvement of glutamate synaptic transmission with the anxiolytic effects of D3 antagonist microinjection, we measured the effects of 10 μM PD128907 on pharmacologically isolated, AMPA-mediated EPSCs evoked by ‘local’ electrical stimuli. PD128907 inhibited EPSC amplitude by only 3.8±2.9% (P>0.05, paired t-test, data not shown). Likewise, the D3R antagonist U99194 did not alter EPSC amplitude (1.0±3.9% facilitation; P>0.05, paired t-test, not shown) under our recording conditions. These data suggest that D3R activation specifically modulates GABAergic, but not glutamatergic synaptic transmission in the BLA.

DISCUSSION

This study provides convincing evidence that BLA DA D3Rs play a prominent role in modulating anxiety-like behaviors. These data are consistent with previous findings, which showed that D3 knockout mice were less ‘anxious’ than wild-type controls, spending significantly more time in the open arms of an elevated plus maze and in the center of an open field (Steiner et al, 1997). In addition, Phillips et al (2002) showed that microinjections of the D3R antagonist nafadotride into the rat amygdala reduced taste-related associative learning in isolation reared animals. Our data compliment and extend these findings to specifically implicate BLA D3Rs in the modulation of innate anxiety-like behaviors. Specifically, we found that rats microinjected with low doses of either U99194 or GR103691 spent significantly more time on the light side of a light/dark box compared with vehicle-injected animals. Likewise, U99194- and GR103691-injected animals spent significantly more time on the open arms of an elevated plus maze than the closed arms compared with vehicle-injected animals. Importantly, the effects of the D3R antagonists on anxiety-like behavior were not due to significant alterations in locomotion as there were no significant differences in movement time or distance measured in an open field. Because U99194 has only a 14-fold higher affinity for D3R over D2R, we also tested a more selective D3R antagonist, GR103691, which has a 60-fold higher affinity for D3R over D2R (Audinot et al, 1998). Given that the use of both antagonists resulted in very similar modulation of BLA-mediated behaviors, our data strongly suggest that activation of BLA D3Rs by the novel environments represented by the light/dark box and the elevated plus maze helps to increase anxiety-like behavior in these apparati. Both novel environmental cues (Inglis and Moghaddam, 1999) and conditioned cues (Yokoyama et al, 2005) increase extracellular DA levels in the BLA, presumably by activation of VTA DA neurons, which provide the majority of dopaminergic input into this brain region (Carter and Fibiger, 1977). This suggests that exposure to the light/dark box or elevated plus maze would increase DA levels in the BLA and increase the expression of anxiety-like behaviors at least in part by activating D3Rs.

Importantly, our electrophysiological findings illustrate the potential physiological mechanism by which DA might influence anxiety-like behavior. Specifically, we show that D3R activation suppresses inhibitory GABAergic synapses, but not glutamatergic synapses, onto BLA projection neurons. The GABAergic system in the BLA is comprised of at least two populations of interneurons that differ with respect to their forms of inhibition (feedforward and feedback) as well as their neuroanatomical placements (Marowsky et al, 2005; McDonald and Betette, 2001; McDonald et al, 2005; Muller et al, 2007). This paper and work by others have shown that GABAergic synapses arising from these populations are functionally independent (Silberman et al, 2008) and experimentally separable merely by the placement of the stimulating electrode. Similar experimental manipulations have also been shown in the hippocampus where one stimulating electrode was placed in stratum lacunosum-moleculare (distal stimulation) and a second electrode in the stratum radiatum (proximal stimulation) to activate two independent GABAergic interneuron populations/pathways converging onto the same glutamatergic pyramidal neuron, albeit at distinct anatomical compartments (dendritic—distal; somatic—proximal) (Weiner et al, 1997; Wu et al, 2005). Furthermore, we and others have now shown that these two populations can also be pharmacologically manipulated using selective adrenergic drugs (Silberman et al, 2010). These findings not only validate that these two GABAergic interneuron populations are functionally independent, but also opens up the doors to future research on other systems that may exclusively target one of these populations vs the other.

Our data suggest that DA release within the BLA activates, among others, D3Rs, which inhibit GABAergic transmission at both feedforward and feedback inhibitory interneurons. Yet, it is worth noting that while we did not observe any significant effect of the D3R antagonists alone on GABAergic transmission, we did find a robust effect of the D3R antagonist on anxiety-like behavior. These data can be interpreted by the mere fact that the DA circuitry is intact in vivo, but not in vitro. Specifically, as discussed previously, DA levels in the BLA are robustly elevated in vivo following exposures to novel environments (Inglis and Moghaddam, 1999; Yokoyama et al, 2005), presumably resulting in a phasic activation of D3Rs (in addition to other DA receptors that will be later discussed). In contrast, in our in vitro slice preparation, endogenous DA levels are apparently not significantly elevated with the electrical stimulation that we use, at least as suggested by the absence of any significant effect of the antagonists themselves. However, stimulated DA released has been previously measured in the BLA following much stronger electrical stimulations (Jones et al, 1994). Regardless, the D3R antagonists only function to prevent an exogenous D3R agonist, such as PD128907, under our recording conditions.

The effects of D3R modulation also appear to differ between these GABAergic populations. Although PD128907 inhibits both local feedback and LPC feedforward IPSCs by a dynamin-dependent mechanism, paired-pulse data and measures of spontaneous TTX-resistant IPSCs suggest that D3Rs may also suppress presynaptic GABAergic function at synapses from local interneurons. However, interpretation of the mIPSC frequency data is confounded by the significant PD128907 modulation of mIPSC amplitude. Regardless, any direct presynaptic effect on local interneurons must be relatively modest given that the P4 dynamin inhibitory peptide suppressed the vast majority of PD128907 inhibition at local synapses. A major role for D3Rs expressed by projection neurons is consistent with the neuroanatomical data, which suggests that tyrosine hydroxylase-positive (TH+) terminals contact a relatively limited population of local GABAergic interneurons (Pinard et al, 2008), whereas over 90% of the TH+ terminals in the BLA make contacts with the principal glutamatergic neurons (Muller et al, 2009) from which our recordings were made. The relative density of TH+ terminals found associated with the feedforward LPC GABAergic neurons has not been measured directly. The absence of any significant presynaptic effect by PD128907 at LPC synapses might suggest a minimal role for dopaminergic modulation at these cells as well. However, recent studies have uncovered robust D1 receptor modulation of both local feedback (Kroner et al, 2005) and LPC feedforward (Marowsky et al, 2005) GABAergic interneurons. Given that BLA D1 receptors strongly modulate drug-seeking (Berglind et al, 2006; Gremel and Cunningham, 2009) and anxiety-related behaviors (de la Mora et al, 2005), one should not minimize potential contributions by dopaminergic terminals onto BLA GABAergic interneurons.

The postsynaptic cellular mechanisms mediating the majority of D3-like inhibition of BLA GABAergic transmission seems to involve the activation of dynamin-dependent signaling pathways. This is consistent with recent data showing D3R-mediated, dynamin-dependent inhibition of GABAA receptors in the NAcc (Chen et al, 2006) and the hippocampus (Hammad and Wagner, 2006; Swant et al, 2008). In both brain regions, D3 receptor activation is believed to reduce the activity of protein kinase A via inhibition of adenylyl cyclase. This ultimately decreases the phosphorylation of GABAA subunits making the receptor complex a substrate for clathrin-mediated endocytosis (Chen et al, 2006). Although we did not specifically examine PKA signaling pathways in this study, the dependence on dynamin-mediated processes strongly suggests a common mechanism for D3R-mediated decreases in GABAergic synaptic transmission in three distinct brain regions.

Despite our findings that D3Rs modulate BLA GABAergic transmission, we cannot rule out a direct modulatory effect of D3Rs on the principal neurons themselves. We did not observe any consistent D3-dependent modulation of principal neuron membrane properties, like changes in resting current or alterations in series resistance. However, our recording conditions, particularly loading the intracellular compartment with cesium and using whole-cell voltage clamp, would tend to minimize our ability to measure such phenomena. D2Rs can increase the excitability of principal neurons in other brain regions. For example, the D2/D3 agonist quinpirole increases spontaneous action potential frequency of glutamatergic subthalamic nuclei neurons by reducing calcium entry through voltage-gated calcium channels and subsequently suppressing calcium-dependent potassium currents (Ramanathan et al, 2008). In contrast, quinpirole does not appear to appreciably alter BLA principal neuron excitability (Kroner et al, 2005). Furthermore, our data complement the findings by Kroner et al (2005) and clearly indicate that the effects of PD128907 are not mediated through D2Rs, given that the highly selective D2R antagonist L714626 was unable to attenuate the effects of PD128907 at either local or LPC synapses. Nevertheless, any direct effects of D3Rs on BLA principal neuron membranes cannot be ruled out.

Although we did not investigate the effects of exogenous DA on GABAergic transmission within the BLA, the variety of receptor-specific studies suggest that any effects of endogenous neurotransmitter in this brain region are likely to be complex. GABAergic inputs arising from lateral paracapsular interneurons are suppressed by both D3Rs—but not D2R (this study)—and D1R (Marowsky et al, 2005) activation. This suggests that DA itself would suppress LPC neurons and would presumably reduce feedforward inhibition of the BLA. This robust, multi-faceted suppression of BLA feedforward inhibition can be contrasted with a primarily post-synaptic D3R inhibition and a ‘presynaptic’ D1R facilitation (Kroner et al, 2005; Marowsky et al, 2005) of local feedback GABAergic function. Changes in the relative expression or function of these different DA receptors may therefore significantly alter BLA function and contribute to the pathophysiology of different disorders, such as drug and alcohol abuse (Di Ciano, 2008; Le Foll et al, 2005; See et al, 2001; Vengeliene et al, 2006). Regardless, the spatial and temporal character of DA release in the BLA is likely to ultimately modulate the complexities associated with DA modulation of BLA output during an environmental stimulus or stressor.

In summary, we show that BLA DA D3Rs can help modulate the expression of anxiety-like behaviors. We also show that the most likely neurobiological mechanism controlling this process involves a long-lasting, postsynaptic, dynamin-dependent downregulation of GABAergic control over BLA principal neurons. This downregulation occurs at both feedforward LPC and local feedback inhibitory synapses. Overall, this dopaminergic modulation has the potential to tightly regulate the output from the BLA to downstream projection areas.