Main

Antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) is highly prevalent among individuals with substance dependence (SD), both in clinical samples and in the general population. The National Comorbidity Survey (Kessler et al. 1994, 1996) and the Epidemiological Catchment Area Study (Regier et al. 1990) showed that, although the lifetime prevalence of ASPD ranges from 2.6–3.5%, its prevalence is 9.8–14.3% among those with a lifetime SD diagnosis. In clinical samples, the median prevalence of ASPD is 18% among individuals with alcohol dependence (AD) and 24% among those with cocaine or opioid dependence (Verheul et al. 1998).

Studies of the genetic basis of SD and ASPD have provided evidence both for overlapping vulnerability and for unique genetic factors. Cadoret et al. (1986, 1995), using an adoption study design, found evidence of two genetic pathways to drug abuse/dependence. The first pathway involved a direct contribution from alcoholic biological parents. The second pathway involved the presence in biological parents of ASPD, with the manifestation in the adopted offspring of aggressiveness, conduct disorder, ASPD, and, ultimately, drug abuse/dependence. These two pathways are consistent with recent research that has identified two subtypes of alcoholism (Bohman et al. 1987; Babor et al. 1992) and drug dependence (Ball et al. 1995, 1998; Feingold et al. 1996).

Serotonergic neurotransmission appears to play an important role in mediating impulsive behaviors (Soubrie 1986). Acute administration of ethanol increases the concentration of 5-HT in the brain, whereas chronic administration decreases 5-HT levels. Decreased serotonergic activity following chronic ethanol administration can lead to behavioral disinhibition, including impulsive aggression (Higley et al. 1992, 1996). The serotonergic system also plays a role in the response to a variety of drugs that are self-administered by animals and abused by humans, including cocaine (Parsons et al 1998; Rocha et al 1998a). These drugs have been shown to release dopamine in the nucleus accumbens and ventral tegmental area (VTA), areas that are thought to provide a common pathway for reinforcement (Di Chiara and Imperato 1988). In turn, 5-HT receptors, including 5-HT1B receptors, are involved in the modulation of dopaminergic activity. For example, activation of the 5-HT1B receptor increases striatal dopamine release, which correlates with enhanced locomotor and motor activity, an effect that is blocked by a 5-HT1B antagonist (Kriem et al. 1996). Furthermore, the central infusion of 5-HT potentiates the inhibitory effect of dopamine on neurons of the VTA (Brodie and Bunney 1996).

Data from humans also show inter-individual variation in central serotonergic functioning among patients diagnosed with ASPD, AD, or drug dependence. For example, antisocial alcoholics have lower basal cerebrospinal fluid levels of 5-HIAA than do controls (Limson et al. 1991; Virkkunen et al. 1994). Substance-dependent patients and antisocial men also show blunted cortisol and prolactin responses to a challenge with the 5-HT agonists fenfluramine and d-fenfluramine (O'Keane et al. 1992; Moss et al. 1990).

Studies looking at the effects of tryptophan depletion also support the notion that low 5-HT tone in the CNS facilitates aggressive behaviors. For example, men with high basal levels of hostility or antisocial traits experienced increased hostility following tryptophan depletion (Cleare and Bond 1995). Furthermore, low central serotonergic tone in combination with alcohol administration has been reported to produce independent, but additive, effects on aggressiveness in men (Pihl et al. 1995).

Based on this literature, the genes encoding 5-HT receptors (HTR1B in particular) are likely candidates for studies of both SD and ASPD. Interestingly, a cluster of four high preference sites for alcohol, morphine and cocaine was mapped by QTL analysis to chromosome 9 (42–48 cM) in mice, a region in close proximity to the site for the 5-HT1B receptor gene (Crabbe et al. 1994). Furthermore, transgenic mice lacking 5-HT1B receptors showed increased aggressive behavior (Saudou et al. 1994) and increased spontaneous alcohol drinking (Crabbe et al. 1996). Additionally, this rodent model showed a 3-fold increase in the rate of cocaine self-administration compared with wild-type animals (Rocha et al. 1998b).

5-HT1B receptors are located both postsynaptically and presynaptically and include autoreceptors that regulate the release of 5-HT (Zifa and Fillion 1992). These receptors have a putative secondary structure of seven transmembrane domains common to G protein-coupled receptors (Julius 1991). 5-HT1B receptors are negatively coupled to adenylate cyclase (Zifa and Fillion 1992). The gene encoding the 5-HT1B receptor was initially cloned in rat (Adham et al. 1992; Voigt et al. 1991) and mouse (Maroteaux et al. 1992), and subsequently mapped to chromosome 6 in humans (locus HTR1B; Lappalainen et al. 1995). Direct analysis of its sequence variation has also been conducted (Sidenberg et al. 1993; Lappalainen et al. 1995; Nöthen et al. 1994; Cargill et al. 1999). A common polymorphism was identified in the gene by Sidenberg et al. (1993) using HincII restriction enzyme digestion; quite possibly this is the same polymorphism later identified and characterized by Lappalainen et al. (1995) using a PCR-RFLP method (G861C). Nöthen et al. (1994) identified four polymorphisms in the gene, including one that results in an amino acid exchange and three located in the 5′ untranslated region. Of these polymorphisms, only the T to G transversion at nt -261 (T-261G) was found in more than one of the 46 individuals screened. Cargill et al. (1999) identified single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in a range of genes of neuropsychiatric interest, including HTR1B. One of the polymorphisms they identified was C129T, a common, though silent, polymorphism in the coding sequence. Huang et al. (1999) also identified C129T, which they genotyped by SSCP, and found to be in complete linkage disequilibrium (LD) with G861C.

Lappalainen et al. (1998) tested for linkage with HTR1B in two groups: 640 Finnish subjects with a high prevalence of alcoholism and ASPD, and a large multi-generational Southwestern American Indian family with a high rate of alcoholism (n = 416). Among the Finns, sib pair analysis showed significant evidence of linkage of antisocial alcoholism to HTR1B G861C (p = .04) and weak evidence (p = .06) of linkage with a nearby short-tandem repeat marker (D6S284). There was also evidence of association, with the 183 Finnish antisocial alcoholics having an 861C allele frequency of 0.32, compared with 0.23 for the other Finns studied, a significant difference (p = .005). In the Southwestern American Indian tribe, sib pair analysis showed linkage of antisocial alcoholism to both G861C (p = .01) and to D6S284 (p = .01).

Huang et al. (1999) examined allele frequencies for the G861C polymorphism using genomic DNA isolated from postmortem brain tissue obtained from 64 alcohol-dependent patients, 36 subjects with pathological aggression, and 102 controls. These investigators found no evidence for allelic association to either AD or pathological aggression. Furthermore, binding indices of the 5-HT1B receptor in prefrontal cortex did not differentiate between controls and alcoholics or between controls and subjects with pathological aggression (Huang et al. 1999). However, Bmax values for 5-HT1B binding were significantly higher among individuals homozygous for the G861 allele (Huang et al. 1999), which was interpreted to mean that this allele may be in LD with a variant that affects 5-HT1B receptor expression. Mundo et al. (2000) reported linkage disequilibrium of the 861G allele with obsessive-compulsive disorder.

In light of these findings, we examined LD across three polymorphic systems in the 5-HT1B gene, as well as evidence for allelic association to SD generally and AD specifically, either with or without comorbid ASPD. We hypothesized that a functional effect was more plausible for G-261T than for G861C (because, based on its 5′ location, the former was more likely to have an effect on transcription than the latter), that LD exists between the G-261T and the G861C polymorphisms, and that both would be associated to antisocial SD.

METHODS

Subjects

We examined allele frequencies in 370 European Americans (EAs) and 123 African Americans (AAs). Of this number, 278 EAs and 69 AAs met lifetime DSM-III-R criteria (American Psychiatric Association 1987) for alcohol, cocaine, or opioid dependence or a combination of these disorders. Diagnoses were made using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (SCID; Spitzer et al. 1992), the computerized Diagnostic Interview Schedule for DSM-III-R (C-DIS-R; Blouin et al. 1988), or a checklist comprised of DSM-III-R symptoms. A subsample of 90 patients (25.9%; 68 EA and 22 AA) comprised the antisocial SD group; these individuals met either full criteria for ASPD (40 EA and 16 AA) or adult criteria only (i.e., no childhood conduct disorder; 28 EA and 6 AA). Among the SD patients, a total of 278 (80.1%; 243 EAs and 35 AAs) met lifetime criteria for AD. There were 59 EAs and 11 AAs in the antisocial AD group. The majority of control subjects (104, or 69.8%) were screened using either the SCID, the C-DIS-R, or the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (Spitzer and Endicott 1975) to exclude alcohol or drug dependence; the remaining 45 controls (30.2%) were screened using an unstructured interview.

Subjects were recruited at the Alcohol Research Center of the University of Connecticut Health Center and the VA Connecticut Healthcare System, West Haven Campus. All subjects gave written informed consent before participating in the study, which was approved by the Institutional Review Board at each of the participating institutions.

Genotyping

G861C

Genotyping for G861C was accomplished according to the PCR-RFLP method of Lappalainen et al. (1998), but with very slight modifications in the primer sequences (the 5′ primer was extended by one base; resulting sequence: 5′ - gaa aca gac gcc caa cag gac c - 3′; and the 3′ primer was extended by two bases; resulting sequence: 5′ - cca gaa acc gcg aaa gaa gat tc - 3′) (the variant site is detected by HincII, which digests the “C” allele; there is also a constant HincII digestion site in the PCR product).

G-261T

Genotyping for the G-261T system was done using the PCR-RFLP method of Nöthen et al. (1994), which uses a modified reverse primer that introduces an artificial restriction site (the variant site is detected by BsmA1, which digests the “T” allele).

C129T

Genotyping for the C129T system was done using primers SNP3artU2 (5′ -ca agg act aca ttt acc agg acG c - 3′) and SNP3D2 (5′ - ggt gcg gta cac tgt ggc a - 3′), which yield a 127-bp PCR product. The primers were designed with reference to the Whitehead SNP database (Cargill et al. 1999) and to Entrez sequence accession M75128 (Demchyshyn et al. 1992). An artificial site is introduced via the forward primer (uppercase “G”) (the variant site is detected by BsaHI, which digests the “C” allele).

Statistical Analysis

Initially, deviation from the genotype counts predicted by Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium expectations was tested using an exact test, as described by Weir (1996) and implemented in software written by Lewis and Zaykin (2001; Genetic Data Analysis (GDA), version 1.0 (d16c)).

Allele frequencies were compared separately by population group. Chi-square analysis was used to compare allele frequencies for three diagnostic groups: control subjects, subjects with a lifetime SD diagnosis (alcohol and/or drug dependence) but no antisocial diagnosis, and subjects with both SD (alcohol and/or drug dependence) and antisocial diagnoses. A comparable analysis was then conducted in which the only SD diagnosis examined was AD (either with or without an antisocial diagnosis). Given the comparatively small number of subjects with both SD and an antisocial diagnosis, overlapping analyses served to maximize the likelihood of finding differences by diagnostic group, should such differences exist. Because the C129T system is in near-complete linkage disequilibrium with the G861C system, and because a basis for comparison exists in the literature for the G861C system, of those two polymorphic systems, statistical comparisons are reported only for the G861C system to avoid redundancy.

Haplotype analysis was performed using version 5.0 of the 3LOCUS program (Long et al. 1995), which computes estimated haplotype frequencies using an expectation maximization (EM) algorithm, as well as pairwise disequilibria. Values of D′, a standardized measure of LD (Lewontin 1988), were also computed. LD was further evaluated using an exact test implemented with GDA version 1.0 (d16c) software; probability levels as computed by GDA are estimates of significance based on a shuffling test (Lewis and Zaykin 2000, 2001). Both haplotype frequencies and LD were examined separately by diagnosis and population group.

RESULTS

There was no deviation from HW expectations at any of the loci for any of the diagnostic groups in either of the populations. Frequencies for HTR1B alleles by population and diagnosis are shown in Table 1 . Among both EAs and AAs, there was no evidence of allelic association with either SD or AD, with or without a comorbid antisocial diagnosis.

Table 1 HTR1B Allele Frequencies (Number of Alleles) in Patients with Substance Dependence or Alcohol Dependence [Without or With a Comorbid Antisocial Personality Disorder (ASPD) Diagnosis], by Population Group

Among EAs, all comparisons between controls and diagnostic groups for the two polymorphic systems were non-significant. For the G861C polymorphism: SD subjects without an antisocial diagnosis, χ2(1) = 1.04, p = .31; SD subjects with an antisocial diagnosis, χ2(1) = 0.72, p = .40; AD subjects without an antisocial diagnosis, χ2(1) = 1.17, p = .28; AD subjects with an antisocial diagnosis, χ2(1) = 0.72, p = .40. For the G-261T polymorphism: SD subjects without an antisocial diagnosis, χ2(1) = 0.56, p = .45; SD subjects with an antisocial diagnosis, χ2(1) = 0.63, p = .43; AD subjects without an antisocial diagnosis, χ2(1) = 0.69, p = .40; AD subjects with an antisocial diagnosis, χ2(1) = 0.35, p = .55.

Similarly, among AAs, all comparisons by phenotype for the two polymorphic systems were non-significant. For the G861C polymorphism: SD subjects without an antisocial diagnosis, χ2(1) = 0.41, p = .52; SD subjects with an antisocial diagnosis, χ2(1) = 0.64, p = .42; AD subjects without an antisocial diagnosis, χ2(1) = 0.97, p = .33; AD subjects with an antisocial diagnosis, χ2(1) = 0.08, p = .78. For the G-261T polymorphism: SD subjects without an antisocial diagnosis, χ2(1) = 1.10, p = .30; SD subjects with an antisocial diagnosis, χ2(1) = 0.72, p = .40; AD subjects without an antisocial diagnosis, χ2(1) = 1.27, p = .26; AD subjects with an antisocial diagnosis, χ2(1) = 0.10, p = .75.

Haplotype frequencies are shown in Table 2 . In both populations, irrespective of diagnosis and consistent with the report of Huang et al. (1999), there was almost complete LD between the G861C and the C129T systems (p < .0001). Among EAs, both in control and substance-dependent subjects, there was also strong evidence of LD between the G-261T system and the other two polymorphic systems (p < .001)(see Table 3) .

Table 2 Estimated Haplotype Frequencies
Table 3 Pairwise Linkage Disequilibrium (D) and Standardized Linkage Disequilibrium (D’) Values and Probabilities for the Three Polymorphic Systems

DISCUSSION

The findings reported provide evidence for near-complete LD between two presumably nonfunctional HTR1B polymorphisms (G861C and C129T) in both EA and AA samples, consistent with the report of Huang et al. (1999) and irrespective of diagnosis. There was also strong evidence in EAs for LD between each of these polymorphic systems and the G-261T polymorphism in the 5′ flanking region of HTR1B. In AAs, there was weak evidence for LD between G-261T and the C129T system, but only in substance-dependent subjects. As indicated by the magnitude of the D′, this is due only in part to the smaller sample sizes in this population. Evidence of LD between G861C and G-261T is of interest in view of the possibility that G-261T, given its location, could affect the transcriptional activity of HTR1B. Such a finding would help to explain the allelic association of G861C with antisocial alcoholism reported by Lappalainen et al. (1998) and the greater 5-HT1B receptor density among individuals homozygous for the G861 allele reported by Huang et al. (1999). However, our data did not provide evidence for association of this polymorphism to SD or AD, with or without a comorbid antisocial diagnosis. The lack of association is consistent with the findings of Huang et al. (1999), who found no association of G861C alleles to either pathological aggression or AD.

In both populations studied, the three most common haplotypes (-261G/129C/861G (GCG), -261T/129T/861C (TTC), and -261T/129C/861G (TCG)) account for >96% of the observations. Consistent with this haplotype distribution, in both populations the 861C allele is seen exclusively, or nearly exclusively, on haplotypes with the -261T allele, whereas the 861G chromosomes are divided by G-261T genotype. While for each individual polymorphic system, the major allele is similar between AAs and EAs, for haplotypes, TCG is most common in AAs, and GCG is most common in EAs (close to 50% in each case). Thus, especially where the allele of interest for genetic association was 861G (as for possible linkage disequilibrium with obsessive-compulsive disorder, reported by Mundo et al. (2000))), genotyping the G-261T system as well is likely to provide additional information. Although these data do not bear on the question of whether either the G861C system or the G-261T system is functional, if the latter is functional and the former is not, the strong LD seen between the systems could provide an explanation for observed positive association findings. The LD data show that genotyping the T129C system is much less likely to provide additional useful information.

Differences in the definition of the phenotypes in the populations examined both in the present report and in the study by Huang et al. (1999) may have contributed to the failure to replicate the findings of Lappalainen et al. (1998). Huang et al. (1999) did not study subjects with comorbid AD and pathological aggression. In the present study, antisocial SD was defined by the presence of SD and a comorbid antisocial diagnosis. Lappalainen et al. (1998) studied only antisocial alcoholism, which was determined by the concurrent presence of AD and either ASPD or intermittent explosive disorder. It is possible that the association reported by Lappalainen et al. (1998) was limited to alcohol-dependent patients with particularly high levels of impulsivity (i.e., those with intermittent explosive disorder). The inclusion in the present study of antisocial individuals based on the presence of either full diagnostic criteria (i.e., including childhood conduct disorder) or adult antisocial features contributes to diagnostic heterogeneity both in present study and in relation to the studies of Lappalainen et al. (1998) and Huang et al. (1999). The studies also differed with respect to the process and source of subject recruitment and the populations studied. In the present study, EA and AA subjects were recruited primarily from a substance abuse treatment setting. In contrast, Lappalainen et al. (1998) recruited subjects from criminal/forensic settings in Finland and from a Native American (Amerindian) community. It is plausible that a physiologically meaningful association is confined to certain populations, or is detectable through linkage disequilibrium with G861C only in certain populations (the Finns and Southwestern American Indians are both special populations, from a genetic perspective). The present results showing differences in haplotype frequency between AA and EA populations, and specifically, different allocation of promoter system alleles with G861C alleles by populations, is consistent with this possibility.

It is also possible that we failed to detect an association due to statistical power limitations imposed by the sample size. The effect size (w) for in the study by Lappalainen et al. (1998) was 0.08 (which is in the small range of effect sizes) for the comparison of controls with antisocial alcoholics. Based on this estimate, and α = .05, among EAs the comparison of controls with antisocial substance-dependent subjects in the current study provided power of 30% to detect such an effect. However, the effect size for the comparison of non-antisocial alcoholics with antisocial alcoholics in the study by Lappalainen et al. was 0.11. Based on this effect size, the sample in the present study yielded power = 0.74 (with α = .05) for the comparison within the sample of EAs with substance dependence (i.e., those with or without an antisocial diagnosis). Although we did not compare these groups statistically in the present study, Table 1 shows an identical allele frequency for the two substance-dependent groups. This suggests that the failure to replicate the findings of Lappalainen et al. (1998) is not due to inadequate statistical power. We conducted multiple statistical comparisons in order to maximize the likelihood of replicating results reported by Lappalainen et al. (1998). The potential problem with such an approach is that it increases the Type 1 error rate. However, since we found no significant association, this concern does not appear to be relevant in the present context.

Because of its important theoretical and clinical implications, further examination of the relationship of HTR1B alleles with antisocial alcohol and/or drug dependence in larger samples is warranted. Given the potential for population stratification to produce a spurious association, it would be important for subsequent studies to examine evidence of such an effect (Pritchard and Rosenberg 1999) and, when identified, to control for it (Pritchard et al. 2000). Alternatively, a family-based approach, such as the haplotype relative risk method (Falk and Rubinstein 1987) or transmission disequilibrium test (Spielman et al. 1993) might be used. In addition, refinement of the diagnosis of antisocial SD (e.g., based on electrophysiologic or neuroendocrine markers of alcohol dependence or impulsivity) may provide a better phenotype for examination of the association to alleles of HTR1B.