INTRODUCTION

Cannabis sativa remains one of the most widely used psychoactive substances in the world. In the United States alone, over 94 million individuals have used cannabis at least once in their lifetime (approximately 40% of the population; Substance Abuse Mental Health Services Administration, 2004). The principal psychoactive constituent in cannabis, Δ-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC; Gaoni and Mechoulam, 1964), affects the brain via the action of central cannabinoid receptors (CB1; Devane et al, 1988). The subsequent discovery of several endogenous cannabinoid neuromodulators, namely anandamide and 2-arachidonylglycerol (2-AG) has led to an exponential growth in cannabinoid research (Devane et al, 1992; Stella et al, 1997). However, despite substantial advances in the molecular and biochemical aspects of endocannabinoid function, little is known about the specific role that this system plays at behavioral level, particularly in the context of chronic cannabis use.

To date, the majority of studies assessing the effects of cannabis on human brain function have focused on higher sensory, attentional, and memory processes (Patrick et al, 1995, 1997; Iversen, 2003; Skosnik et al, 2001, 2006a, 2006b; Ramaekers et al, 2006; Jager et al, 2007). However, several of these studies have led to ambiguous and equivocal results, which may be due to the fact that tasks evaluating higher cognitive processes probe the integrity of distributed neocortical networks (ie neocortical areas contain relatively moderate CB1 densities compared with structures such as the basal ganglia and the cerebellum). Tasks which examine cerebellar function should be particularly useful indices of the effects of exogenous cannabinoids, as the cerebellum contains the highest density of CB1 receptors in the brain (Herkenham et al, 1990; Pertwee, 1997; Tsou et al, 1998). In addition to its role in motor coordination, the cerebellum is thought to play a key role in temporal operations such as rhythm production, time estimation, and some forms of associative learning (Ivry and Keele, 1989; Katz and Steinmetz, 2002; Leiner et al, 1991). Indeed, altered time perception is one of the most frequently reported subjective experiences associated with acute cannabis intoxication (Hicks et al, 1984; Mathew et al, 1998), which is thought to be modulated by the cerebellum (O'Leary et al, 2003). Thus, studies examining cerebellar-mediated processes may provide important clues in relation to the subjective effects of cannabis and the role of the endocannabinoid system in normal brain function.

Of the experiments which have evaluated cannabinoid effects on time estimation, several have shown that direct cannabis administration induces temporal disintegration, which is typically exhibited in the form of increased internal clock speeds (the experience of time passing more slowly; Hicks et al, 1984; Mathew et al, 1998; O'Leary et al, 2003). In relation to cerebellar modulation of cannabis' effect on time perception, a study integrating behavioral measures of temporal processing with neuroimaging methodologies has yielded positive results. Using PET, O'Leary et al (2003) demonstrated that cannabis-induced alterations in a self-paced timing task were correlated with the increased activity of medial cerebellar cortex. Interestingly, cerebellar metabolism during acute cannabis administration has been shown to correlate with subjective ratings of intoxication (Volkow et al, 1996). While it is clear that cannabis use in humans affects cerebellar function, time estimation tasks such as those described above probably recruit a distributed neural network including such structures as basal ganglia and neocortex (Bengtsson et al, 2005), making specific interpretations regarding cannabinoid effects on cerebellar function difficult. What is needed is a probe specific to cerebellar function that is based upon well-established neural circuitry, while solely dependent on the cerebellum.

One such task which has been shown to be modulated almost completely by the cerebellum is classical eyeblink conditioning (EBC). EBC is an associative motor learning task which involves the paired presentation of a neutral conditioned stimulus (CS), such as a tone, followed by an unconditioned stimulus (US), such as an ocular air puff. The US air puff evokes a reflexive eyeblink, the unconditioned response (UR), and after repeated CS–US paired presentations, a conditioned response (CR), a blink of the eye, forms subsequent to CS presentation. After repeated pairings, the CR is timed so that the peak eyelid closure occurs near the onset of the US (Gormezano et al, 1983).

All forms of EBC are dependent on cerebellar function, though some forms, such as trace conditioning, also critically depend on the function of the hippocampus (Steinmetz, 2000). The importance of cerebellum in the acquisition of the nictitating membrane/eyeblink response in nonhuman animals has been clearly demonstrated (McCormick et al, 1981; McCormick and Thompson, 1984a, 1984b; Thompson, 1986; Yeo et al, 1985a, 1985b). Although it is difficult to directly assess the neural substrates of EBC in humans, increasing evidence corroborates the nonhuman animal literature (Daum et al, 1993; Topka et al, 1993; Woodruff-Pak et al, 1996). For instance, a PET study showed activation in the inferior cerebellar cortex and deep cerebellar nuclei in humans during eyeblink classical conditioning (Logan and Grafton, 1995). Furthermore, cerebellar morphology and volume in both animals and humans are related to the magnitude of eyeblink conditioning (Woodruff-Pak et al, 2000).

Eyeblink conditioning methodology is well suited for examining cerebellar-based timing deficits in cannabis users. The neural circuitry associated with conditioning is distinct, well characterized, and includes structures and pathways modulated by the cannabinoid system. For example, at several critical nodes in this circuit, particularly granule cell (parallel fiber) to Purkinje cell synapses, excitatory glutamatergic release is modulated (inhibited) by presynaptic CB1 activation (Kreitzer and Regehr, 2001a). More specifically, Purkinje cells activated by glutamatergic granule cells normally serve to inhibit cerebellar deep nuclei (ie interpositus nucleus) thus modulating the timing of conditioned blink responses (CRs) (Steinmetz, 2000). Chronic cannabis use, which is known to produce behavioral tolerance and may induce long-term endocannabinoid compensatory changes (eg CB1 downregulation; Sim et al, 1996; Romero et al, 1998), could result in increased granule cell glutamate release, and hence overactive inhibitory Purkinje inputs to the interpositus. This position is in agreement with a recent study of CB1 knockout animals, in which mice lacking the CB1 receptor exhibit severely disrupted EBC (Kishimoto and Kano, 2006). However, despite the knowledge of dense CB1 presence in the cerebellum, the effect of cannabis use on learning-related changes in this area in humans is unknown.

To elucidate the long-term effect of cannabinoids on the cerebellum in humans, the current study assessed chronic cannabis users utilizing cerebellar-dependent single-cue delay EBC. Fourteen current cannabis users and 10 healthy drug-free control participants were evaluated. Drug-use status was determined through clinical interviews, drug-use questionnaires, and urine toxicology screens. The primary dependent measures for the eyeblink procedure were percent conditioned responses (% CRs) and CR peak latency. In addition, fronto-central electroencephalographic (EEG) data were collected to assess attention-dependent event-related potentials (ERPs) to the tone CS. An EEG measure was included to address the possible confound that cannabis-related alterations in sensory/attentional function could affect processing of the tone CS. The N100 component was evaluated, as this ERP is known to be reliably elicited by discreet auditory stimuli (ie tones), and is highly sensitive to attentional allocation (Hillyard et al, 1973; Coull, 1998). As previously observed in CB1 knockout mice (Kishimoto and Kano, 2006), it was hypothesized that cannabis users would show deficits in the acquisition and timing of the CR.

METHODS

Participants

Current cannabis users (n=14) and healthy drug-naive controls (n=10) were assessed. This study was approved by the Indiana University Bloomington Human Subjects Committee. Participants were recruited from the local university community, were paid for their participation, and written informed consent was obtained from each. Recruitment consisted of flyers that were posted on the Indiana University campus and in the local Bloomington area which advertised for a paid psychology study for individuals currently using marijuana, but who were free of heavy alcohol use or use of other drugs. The control flyer simply advertised for individuals with no history of illicit drug use and only moderate alcohol use. Potential participants were first screened over the phone to determine if they met our inclusion criteria (see below). After phone screening (of approximately 50 potential subjects), 28 individuals met our inclusion criteria and were invited to participate in the study. Four of these were rejected after the initial clinical interview due to past histories of psychopathology. Table 1 illustrates the basic demographic information as well as drug/alcohol use rates. There were no significant differences between the groups in age, years of education, or Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) scores (Table 1). All participants but two were right handed.

Table 1 Demographic and Drug-Use Histories for the Cannabis Group (n=14) and Control Participants (n=10)

The inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) For the cannabis group: current cannabis consumption (smoked joints) at the rate of at least once per week during the past month, a positive urine toxicology screen for THC, no other illicit substance use during the past 6 months (including a negative urine toxicology screen for other illicit drugs), and no DSM-IV diagnosis of bipolar or schizophrenia spectrum disorders except cannabis abuse or dependence; (2) For the control group: no history of illicit substance use, a negative urine toxicology screen for all drugs tested, and no history of psychiatric illness (Axis I or II); (3) For all participants: ages 18–35, completion of high school education, no family history of bipolar or schizophrenia spectrum disorders, no history of cardiovascular disease, hearing problems, neurological disease, learning disability, or head injury resulting in loss of consciousness. In addition, participants were excluded if they reported consumption of more than two alcoholic drinks per day (one per day for females). The cannabis group drug-use inclusion criteria (cannabis use at least once per week; 24 h abstinence) were chosen to eliminate acute cannabis effects, while retaining neurophysiological effects from altered CB1 activity. Human studies indicate that 80–90% of the total amount of THC is excreted within 5 days, so a minimum use of once per week enabled the detection of THC metabolites.

Clinical Interviews, Questionnaires, and Drug-Use Assessment

The following psychometric questionnaires and clinical interviews were administered to all participants: a substance-use questionnaire, The Drug Abuse Screening Test (DAST; Skinner, 1982), and The Short Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (SMAST; Selzer et al, 1975). In addition, The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis-I and -II Disorders (SCID I and SCID II; First et al, 1997, 2002) and subscales of the WAIS III (Picture completion, Digit Symbol, Similarities, and Digit Span; Wechsler, 1997) were used to rule out previous psychiatric conditions and general deficits in neuropsychological function.

The clinical interview was used to ascertain current and past diagnoses for alcohol and substance-abuse and dependence. Measures of frequency, quantity, and density of cannabis consumption were determined via the interview for the past 6 months, then for 1 month before the test session. Participants were instructed to consider each day of the week and indicate, for an average week, how much they consumed per drug-use occasion over the past 6 months, and then for the month before testing. Recency and density of last use was assessed using the past month section of the interview. Overall chronicity was assessed using a time-line follow-back procedure (Skosnik et al, 2006a), total months of active use since the onset of initial drug use (of any drug), which accounts for different periods of nonuse, and a measure of overall density of use (total amount ever used/total months of active use).

Urine screens (Q10-1, Proxam) were administered immediately preceding all testing to corroborate self-reports from the drug questionnaire and clinical interviews. The Q10-1 kit screens for cannabis, opiates, amphetamines, cocaine, MDMA (ecstasy), tricyclic antidepressants, phencyclidine, benzodiazepines, methamphetamines, and barbituates.

EBC Stimuli and Procedure

Participants engaged in a 148-trial delay EBC paradigm similar to Sears et al (2000) and Brown et al (2005). A visual overview of the experimental set-up can be seen in Figure 1. Initially, eight US alone trials were presented, with an intertrial interval (ITI) of 15 s. Subsequently, the acquisition phase commenced consisting of 10 blocks of trials (mean ITI=15 s, range=10–20 s). Each block contained nine CS–US paired trials and one CS-alone trial, which were randomly presented within the last five trials of each block. Paired CS–US trials consisted of a 400 ms, 1000 Hz tone (80 dB SPL) with a coterminating 50 ms air puff. The extinction phase included 25 CS alone and 25 US alone trials, randomly presented across five additional blocks with a mean ITI of 15 s (range=10–20 s). To maintain attention throughout the procedure, participants were asked to rate the pleasantness of neutral pictures selected from the International Affective Picture System (Lang and Greenwald, 1988). Pictures were presented for 2 s in between each trial, and participants rated the images on a scale of 1–10 using a button response pad.

Figure 1
figure 1

Experimental setup. Visual overview of the experimental setup showing method and timing of unconditioned stimulus (US) and conditioned stimulus (CS) presentation, and electroencephalographic (EEG)/electromyographic (EMG) recording.

Eyeblinks were recorded using pairs of electromyographic (EMG) electrodes (8 mm AG/AGCl; Model TD-23; MedAssociated, St Albans, VT). The electrodes were placed on the orbicularis palpebrarum muscle below each eye, with a ground electrode on the forehead. All electrode impedances were maintained below 5 kΩ. The US consisted of a 10 p.s.i. (50 ms duration) puff of medical-grade air presented to the left eye with copper tubing affixed to eye-glass rims and placed 1 cm away from the inner canthus of the eye. Foam ear inserts were used for presentation of the CS tones (E-A-RLINK—Aearo Company Auditory Systems, Indianapolis, IN). EMG data were continuously recorded at 2.5 kHz with a Sensorium EPA-6 bioamplifier (highpass filter=1 Hz, 12 dB/octave; lowpass filter=300 Hz, eighth order elliptic; gain=5000) and acquired using the software Neuroscan (v. 4.2, El Paso, TX).

EBC Data Processing

Individual trials were epoched (1086 ms) from the continuous EMG data file beginning 500 ms before presentation of the CS (using Neuroscan Edit software), and high pass filtered (10 Hz, 6 dB/octave) before being rectified and smoothed using a 41-point Gaussian weighted moving average. Data were then entered into the software DataMunch for further analysis (King and Tracy, 1999; Tracy et al, 2001). For each subject, responses were recorded as blinks if the amplitude exceeded five standard deviations above the baseline (baseline window for each trial=125 ms before CS presentation). CRs were recorded if the blink occurred between 100 and 350 ms after CS onset (corresponding to a period beginning 250 ms before US onset). Trials in which spontaneous blinks occur within a window from 75 ms before CS presentation to 25 ms following the CS were labeled bad trials and excluded from further analysis.

EEG Recording and Processing

Electroencephalogram data were concurrently collected during the EBC paradigm. An examination of the N100 ERP component was assessed, which is sensitive to selective attention. This EEG measure was utilized to rule out the possibility that group differences might be due to altered CS encoding resulting from cannabis-related attentional impairments (Skosnik et al, 2001). The EEG was recorded continuously (band pass 0.1–100 Hz; sampling rate 1000 Hz) from the scalp at electrode site FCz with a nose reference, along with additional electrodes to record the vertical electrooculogram. The recorded EEG was segmented into epochs consisting of the 400 ms during stimulus presentation, along with a 50 ms baseline, with any epoch containing a voltage greater than±100 μV excluded. Ocular movement artifact correction and averages were computed for each frequency block using commercially available software (Neuroscan). For analysis of the transient-evoked response (N100), epochs were low-pass filtered at 20 Hz (24 dB/octave) before averaging, and baseline corrected (50 ms prestimulus/pre-CS baseline) after averaging. The time window for the N100 component was defined as the largest peak occurring between 90 and 150 ms after the onset of tone CS. Finally, all experimenters responsible for EBC and EEG data analysis were blind to which experimental group each subject was assigned.

Statistical Analysis

The primary dependent measures for the eyeblink procedure were % CRs and CR peak latency. For % CRs and CR Peak latency during the acquisition phase (10 blocks of paired CS–US trials), a repeated-measures ANOVA was used to assess the between participants effect of group (2) and the within participants effect of block (10). During the five-block extinction phase, a repeated-measures ANOVA was used to assess the between-participants effect of group (2) and block (5). For the 10-block acquisition phase, a similar repeated-measures ANOVA was used to assess the effect of group (2) and block (10) on CR and UR amplitude, and UR onset latency. For the primary dependent variables of % CRs and CR peak latency, effect sizes are reported (partial η2), where small effect sizes are less than 0.06, moderate effect sizes range from 0.06 to 0.14, and large effect sizes are greater than 0.14 (Cohen, 1973). Possible group differences in EEG N100 amplitude/latency and questionnaire scores were determined using one-way ANOVAs. All statistical tests used an alpha level of p<0.05 to determine significance (two-tailed), and all tests were performed using the software package SPSS 14.0.

RESULTS

Raw EBC Data

The primary dependent variables of interest were % CRs and CR peak latencies during paired trials across the duration of the experiment. Plots of the grand-averaged trial-by-trial EMG data from all the paired CS–US trials can be seen in Figure 2. As can be seen in the outlined sections representing the CR time window (approximately 220–350 ms), cannabis use was associated with a pronounced deficit in the acquisition of the CR compared to the control participants, particularly from about block 3 to block 10. Both groups showed comparable UR amplitudes (at approximately 400 ms), which steadily decreased in amplitude across the acquisition phase due to habituation.

Figure 2
figure 2

Grand averaged trial-by-trial EMG data from all paired conditioned stimulus–unconditioned stimulus (CS–US) trials. The 400-ms tone CS (1 kHz, 80 dB) co-terminates with a 50 ms (10 p.s.i.) air puff. The outlined sections and panels display the time window where conditioned responses (CRs) occurred (approximately 220–350 ms). At around block 3, the control participants (top) begin to show robust CRs, as evidenced by the increased amplitude in the period shortly before US onset. Conversely, cannabis users (bottom) are impaired in the acquisition and timing of the CR. Both groups show decreased unconditioned response (UR) amplitudes as the experiment progresses due to habituation.

EBC Acquisition Phase

Percent CRs and CR peak latency during each 10-trial block (plus extinction) were calculated for each group. For % CRs (Figure 3), a repeated-measures ANOVA revealed a main effect of block (F(9, 198)=8.67, p<0.0001; partial η2=0.85) and a main effect of group (F(1, 22)=14.36, p<0.001; partial η2=0.40). However, there was no group × block interaction (F(9, 198)=1.89, p=0.14; partial η2=0.55). Given that the cannabis group exhibited slightly higher alcohol use rates than controls (see Table 1), the average number of drinks per week (during the past month) was included in the repeated-measures ANOVA as a covariate. The main effect of group remained significant after covarying for alcohol use (F(1, 21)=9.63, p<0.005; partial η2=0.31). Likewise, the group differences also remained significant after covarying for tobacco use per day (F(1, 21)=12.80, p<0.002; partial η2=0.38). For CR peak latency, there was also a main effect of block (F(9, 198)=5.17, p<0.003; partial η2=0.77) and a main effect of group (F(1, 22)=7.23, p<0.01; partial η2=0.25). There was also a trend toward a group × block interaction (F(9, 198)=2.31, p=0.08; partial η2=0.60). The main effect of group remained significant after covarying for tobacco use (F(1, 21)=7.85, p<0.01; partial η2=0.27), and fell to a trend level difference after covarying for alcohol use (F(1, 21)=2.79, p<0.10; partial η2=0.12). For UR amplitude, no group differences were observed during paired trials (F(1, 22)=0.71, p=0.41) or in UR latency during paired trials (F(1, 22)=0.02, p=0.9). Taken together, the significant group differences in % CRs and CR peak latency during acquisition demonstrates that the cannabis group showed impaired learning of the conditioned eyeblink response compared to controls (Figure 3), and shorter (less adaptive) peak CR latencies.

Figure 3
figure 3

Mean % conditioned responses (CRs) across blocks (1 block=9 CS–US paired trials) for cannabis users vs controls. Both groups displayed approximately equal % CRs in the first block of the learning phase, with controls showing greater learning across time. Both groups show similar declines in the percentage of CRs across the extinction blocks, indicating similar rates of extinction of the CR. Error bars indicate±SEM.

EBC Extinction Phase and Auditory Tone CS ERP

A repeated-measures ANOVA on % CRs during the last five extinction blocks revealed a main effect of group (F(1, 22)=4.23, p<0.05; partial η2=0.16) and a main effect of block (F(4, 88)=3.61, p<0.02; partial η2=0.43), which demonstrates that while both groups displayed decreasing % CRs across extinction, the cannabis group remained significantly below control levels. However, there was no group × block interaction (F(4, 88)=0.35, p=0.84; partial η2=0.07), demonstrating that extinction rates for the two groups were equal.

For CR peak latency during extinction, there was no effect of group (F(1, 22)=1.8, p=0.21; partial η2=0.12), block (F(4, 88)=0.27, p=0.89; partial η2=0.10), or group × block (F(4, 88)=0.74, p=0.59; partial η2=0.23), indicating that timing for the remaining CRs was the same in both groups. Likewise for CR amplitude, there was no effect of group (F(1, 22)=0.28, p=0.6), block (F(4, 88)=1.3, p=0.32), or group × block (F(4, 88)=1.2, p=0.36).

Finally, for the ERP measure, a one-way ANOVA revealed that there were no differences in N100 amplitude between the groups (F(1, 23)=0.11, p=0.75). The grand-averaged EEG waveforms illustrating the N100 ERPs for each group can be seen in Figure 4.

Figure 4
figure 4

Grand-averaged transient ERPs elicited by tone conditioned stimulus (CS) onset for both cannabis users and control participants at electrode FCz. The similar N100 component in the cannabis users and controls provide evidence of intact acoustic stimulus processing and CS encoding in both groups.

DISCUSSION

The current study was the first to assess cannabinoid modulation of cerebellar-dependent delay EBC in human volunteers. These results are in agreement with previous human work showing altered cerebellar activity during cannabis administration (Mathew et al, 1998; O'Leary et al, 2003). In addition to providing further evidence that cannabis use disrupts cerebellar function, the current findings provide an important clue about the specific role endocannabinoids may play in normal cerebellar-dependent learning, particularly in light of the recent study of delay EBC in CB1 knockout animals (Kishimoto and Kano, 2006). Using an animal analogue of the delay EBC learning paradigm, Kishimoto and Kano (2006) have shown that mice lacking the CB1 receptor display highly impaired acquisition of the CR compared to wild-type animals. Interestingly, CB1 knockouts showed normal UR amplitudes, CS processing, and unaltered EBC extinction phases, patterns which are also identical to the current human EBC results. It, therefore, appears that the cannabinoid system is primarily involved in the acquisition phase of EBC, with little involvement in CS sensory processing or UR blink generation.

As to the mechanism of CB1 effects on EBC, both the well-established circuitry mediating conditioning (Steinmetz, 2000) and the specified role of endocannabinoids in cerebellar synaptic function allow for specific hypotheses regarding the role of cannabinoids in EBC. It is thought that information about the CS is projected into the cerebellum via mossy fibers which in turn activate granule cells, while the US is projected into the cerebellum along climbing fibers. Plasticity is thought to occur at neuronal junctions that receive convergent CS–US input (the interpositus nucleus and Purkinje cells in cerebellar cortex). The Purkinje cells that receive convergent CS–US input from granule cells (ie CS) and climbing fibers (ie US) project to the neurons in the interpositus nucleus that also receive convergent CS–US input. The inhibitory Purkinje cells tonically suppress interpositus activity, and it is thought that for conditioning to occur, the GABAergic Purkinje cells must themselves be inhibited, which in turn disinhibits interpositus activity, thus affecting the timing and gain of CRs (Steinmetz, 2000).

In regard to cannabinoid modulation of this circuit, CB1 receptors are known to reside on glutamatergic granule cell axons (parallel fiber terminals). Purkinje cells synthesize and release retrograde endocannabinoids following bursts of parallel fiber stimulation (Brown et al, 2003). Thus, during EBC, endocannabinoids may operate by activating presynaptic CB1 receptors on parallel fiber terminals, which reduce Ca2+ influx, ultimately inhibiting Purkinje cell activity by decreasing glutamate release from parallel fiber terminals. Indeed, it has recently been shown that concurrent parallel fiber/climbing fiber activation of Purkinje cells (which is analogous to convergent CS–US input) leads to short-term depression of the parallel fiber synapse, an effect which is mediated by endocannabinoids (Brenowitz and Regehr, 2005). Such an effect may be crucial for the induction of longer-lasting synaptic plasticity, such as endocannabinoid-mediated long-term depression (LTD; Safo and Regehr, 2005), which itself may represent the neural substrate of EBC (Thompson and Kim, 1996).

In the context of chronic cannabis use, long-term activation of presynaptic parallel fiber CB1 by exogenous THC may act to desensitize or downregulate the receptors, which could disrupt the short-term plasticity and/or LTD necessary for learning in the cerebellum. It has already been established that chronic cannabis users assessed during periods of abstinence demonstrate hypoactive cerebellar activity, which could be interpreted as neuroadaptive endocannabinoid downregulation (Volkow et al, 1996; Block et al, 2000; Chang et al, 2006). Indeed, animal studies have shown that behavioral tolerance to the motor effects of THC is likely mediated by CB1 downregulation throughout the cerebellum (Sim et al, 1996; Romero et al, 1998). More recently, it has been found that chronic cannabis use in humans does in fact lead to significant CB1 downregulation as determined via post-mortem [3H]SR141716A binding (Villares, 2007). These data provide indirect evidence that cerebellar changes in CB1 induced by chronic cannabis intake may partly explain the impaired EBC performance observed in the current study.

Several limitations of the current study warrant further discussion. First, since participants were tested after 24 h of abstinence, it is unclear whether THC withdrawal may have accounted for the observed findings. However, most heavy cannabis users do not display peak withdrawal symptoms until 2–3 days following the cessation of use (Budney et al, 2004), which most likely precludes the possibility that the current results were due to cannabis withdrawal. However, a related confound is that participants in the current experiment were not monitored during the 24 h abstinence window. Thus, the veracity of participants' self-reported cannabis use 24 h pre-experiment could be unreliable, meaning that acute intoxication or residual THC may have contributed to EBC learning deficits.

A second limitation of the current study is that even though both groups displayed moderate-to-low levels of alcohol and tobacco use, the cannabis group had slightly increased rates of consumption of these substances. However, current weekly levels of alcohol and tobacco use were included as covariates in each of the analyses of EBC performance (% CRs and CR peak latency), which in turn, did not influence the observed effects. In relation to tobacco use, nicotine administration has actually been shown to increase EBC performance in animals (Woodruff-Pak et al, 1994, 2000). This, along with the fact that the onset of nicotine withdrawal is 2 h and peaks around 36 h (tobacco use was permitted in our subjects up to 1 h before testing; Hughes et al, 1994; Kenny and Markou, 2001), most likely rules out that possibility that tobacco use or withdrawal contributed to the current findings.

Thirdly, since the current study did not directly administer cannabis or THC, it remains possible that some preexisting condition contributed to both cannabis drug-seeking and altered EBC performance (eg premorbid alterations in CB1 or endocannabinoids). However, since all of the current cannabis subjects tested positive for THC, and had a mean of 5.6 years of cannabis use, the informed assumption of CB1 downregulation discussed above is likely more proximal and salient than subtle preexisting individual differences in endocannabinoid activity. Nonetheless, since there was no way of directly assessing CB1 densities in the current sample, possible preexisting differences cannot be ruled out using the current methodology.

Several additional limitations of the current experiment exist which could be addressed by future studies including the small sample size, the generalizability of the findings given the homogeneity of the groups, and whether the observed deficits would normalize following longer periods of abstinence. This last issue regarding whether EBC performance is recoverable will be important to elucidate from a public health perspective. Current neuropsychological data suggest a recovery of cannabis-induced cognitive impairments after 1 month of abstinence (Pope et al, 2001; Harrison et al, 2002). However, these neuropsychological tasks are typically sensitive to distributed cortical networks not specific to cerebellar processes, as is the EBC procedure. It therefore remains feasible that compensatory mechanisms of diverse brain regions (ie neocortical) may partially explain the recovery of function on classical neuropsychological tasks. Future studies are planned which will attempt to address these issues, including assessing past cannabis users on EBC, and the use of alternate cerebellar tasks (eg paced finger tapping and postural sway) which could corroborate the finding of altered cerebellar-dependent learning using EBC. Prospective experiments on chronic cannabis users would also benefit from identified behavioral patterns of CB1 knockout animals. Given that CB1 knockout mice exhibit alterations on tests assessing extinction of fear conditioning, pain thresholds, locomotor activity, and spatial memory, developing human analogues of these tasks for application in cannabis users would seem prudent.

In summary, the overall implications and relevance of the current study are twofold: first, the observed effects provide evidence that endocannabinoids may be critically involved in cerebellar EBC. Second, the fact that chronic cannabis users exhibit learning patterns similar to CB1 knockout animals suggests that long-term use may produce physiological alterations (ie CB1 downregulation) of a magnitude previously unreported. Given that the cerebellum is reciprocally connected to vast regions of brain (Dum and Strick, 2003) and is now implicated in a host of nonmotor functions including perception and cognition (Katz and Steinmetz, 2002), it is imperative that future human and animal studies further assess the role of cannabinoids in cerebellar function.