INTRODUCTION

The local anesthetic procaine has unique neuropsychopharmacological properties that make it useful as a discrete probe of the limbic system and associated studies of emotion. In this regard, it has been used as an affective challenge in healthy volunteers (Ketter et al, 1996; Servan-Schreiber et al, 1998), and patients with mood disorders (Ketter et al, 1993) and panic disorder (George et al, 1993), as well as in individuals abusing alcohol (George et al, 1990) and cocaine (Adinoff et al, 2001).

Procaine (1.84 mg/kg), when administered intravenously (i.v.) in healthy volunteers (Kellner et al, 1987), results in brief intense emotional (ranging from euphoria to dysphoria) and sensory experiences (visual, auditory, and olfactory illusions/hallucinations) in association with increased relative anterior paralimbic cerebral blood flow (CBF) as measured with [15O] water positron emission tomography (PET) (Ketter et al, 1996). In healthy individuals, procaine also induces the following: hormonal changes such as increased adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH), cortisol, and prolactin (Kling et al, 1994); and increased temporal lobe fast activity electroencephalogram (EEG) (Parekh et al, 1995). Furthermore, the right amygdala blood flow correlated with the degree of affective arousal, while the left amygdala CBF correlated positively with the degree of dysphoria and negatively with the degree of euphoria (Ketter et al, 1996). The experiences described by the subjects in these procaine studies are reminiscent of those reported after direct stimulation of limbic cortex during epilepsy surgery (Gloor et al, 1982; Halgren et al, 1978; Penfield and Jasper, 1954). The emotional and endocrine changes also resemble the effects of acute challenge with the anticholinesterase drug physostigmine (Janowsky et al, 1986).

The neurochemical mechanisms of procaine's emotional, sensory, and endocrine effects could provide valuable insights into the neurobiology of normal and pathological emotion regulation, but have not been clearly elucidated. Procaine is classically associated with the blockade of voltage-gated sodium channels producing the local anesthetic effect for which it was designed and synthesized by Einhorn in 1905. Procaine produces 50% inhibition of the action potential at 1.1 mM in frog and rat sciatic nerve (Butterworth and Strichartz, 1990), by binding in the channel pore and altering the gating mechanism to increase the probability of the inactive state, thereby reducing the likelihood of an action potential (Butterworth and Strichartz, 1990). Site-directed mutations in rat brain sodium channels show reduced use-dependent blockade, confirming this hypothesis (Scholz, 2002). Procaine is also hypothesized to have direct effects on the lipid bilayer and hence indirectly influence components in the neuronal membrane (De Jong, 1994).

Procaine also interacts with many neurochemical systems, but is most potent at the muscarinic cholinergic receptor. Procaine competitively displaces quinuclidinyl benzilate (QNB), a nonspecific muscarinic antagonist at putative M2 receptors in the guinea-pig ileum with a delivery rate constant (Ki) of 4 μM (Hisayama et al, 1989) and inhibits QNB binding (Ki=4 μM) in rat hippocampal membranes known to have M1 and M2 receptors (Sharkey et al, 1988). In contrast, the affinity of procaine for M3 receptor was 5 mM in the guinea-pig mesenteric artery (Itoh et al, 1981). Procaine inhibits methylcarbachol or dimethylphenylpipereeinium (DMPP) binding presumably at nicotinic cholinergic receptors on rat brain membranes at higher concentrations (Ki=50–100 μM depending on the agonist used) than observed in the muscarinic system (Saraswati et al, 1992). Sigma receptors are the only other sites at which procaine binds in the low micromolar range (3.6 μM; Sharkey et al, 1988).

The affinity of procaine for other systems include transporters (dopamine (DA): 104 mM, norepinephrine (NE): 217 mM, and serotonin (5HT): 276 mM) and receptors (serotonin (0.1–10 mM, depending on subtype), neuropeptide Y (5 mM), angiotensin (5 mM), endothelin (5 mM), adrenergic (a2: 5 mM, b1: 100 mM), glycine (1 mM), glutamate (1 mM), and gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) (1.5–5.4 mM per subunit); Aoshima et al, 1992; Cunningham and Lakoski, 1988; Fishlock and Parks, 1966; Itoh et al, 1981; Napier, 1992; Ritz et al, 1987; Sharkey et al, 1988; Sugimoto et al, 2000; Sato et al, 2000). It should be noted that for some ligands affinity constants were not determined, but stated doses had no biological effects, such as D1 and D2 receptors (Napier, 1992). Thus, based on affinities, procaine would be expected to have primary actions on the muscarinic cholinergic and sigma receptor systems that could be important contributors to its clinical effects.

Cholinergic M1 and M2 receptors are present in core limbic areas, such as amygdala and hippocampus, as well as primary sensory regions. While M1 receptors have primarily a cortical distribution, M2 receptors have a more uniform distribution across the brain, cortically and subcortically (Mesulam, 1995; Mesulam et al, 1983; Flynn and Mash, 1993). Telencephalic cholinergic projections originating in the basal forebrain nuclei, such as the nucleus basalis, send efferents to most of the cortex, including the anterior cingulate, as well as to subcortical regions, the basolateral amygdala, and hippocampus (Mesulam, 1995; Mesulam et al, 1983). Of particular interest are the efferents of the basolateral amygdala that heavily innervate the anterior cingulate and nearby medial orbitfrontal cortex (Russchen et al, 1985). These anterior paralimbic regions with direct and indirect cholinergic connections are also the areas that exhibit the most robust activation by procaine in humans (Ketter et al, 1996). Thus, the distribution and the potential functional consequences of the cholinergic receptors in limbic pathways could allow cholinergic mechanisms to play an important role in emotion (Heimer, 2003).

Cholinergic drugs have been shown to influence limbic regions known to mediate mood, reward, and aggression. For example, neuronal activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex, and the amygdala of monkeys has been shown to be altered by iontophoretic administration of cholinergic agents (Aou et al, 1983; Inoue et al, 1983; Lenard et al, 1989). Similar to GABA, procaine microinjection (6–20 mg) into rat nucleus basalis inhibits frontal cortical neuronal firing in response to conditioned stimuli (pairing with medial forebrain stimulation) (Rigdon and Pirch, 1984). Moreover, procaine selectively activates limbic structures electrophysiologically in rats (Munson et al, 1970; Racine et al, 1975, 1979; Wagman et al, 1967). Utilizing 2-deoxyglucose methodology, lidocaine, a closely related amide local anesthetic, selectively activated limbic structures in rats (Post et al, 1984). Local anesthetics, including cocaine, lidocaine and procaine, can produce kindled seizures and aggressive behavior in rodents (Post, 1981). Intra-amygdalar injection of cholinomimetics also induces kindled seizures (similar in appearance to those evoked by electrical amygdaloid kindling), which can be blocked by muscarinic antagonists (Cain, 1981). Furthermore, atropine, a muscarinic antagonist, inhibits and physostigmine weakly facilitates procaine-induced kindling in rats (Heynen et al, 1995).

In summary, cholinergic modulation could contribute significantly to procaine-induced emotional and sensory experiences. This is suggested by procaine's: (1) preferential affinity to M2 muscarinic cholinergic receptors that are localized in key limbic areas; (2) ability to selectively activate limbic structures associated with emotion regulation while producing concomitant clinical electrophysiological effects; and (3) similarities in action to muscarinic agonists. In this study, we specifically explore the potential role of the muscarinic system in procaine's effects by measuring muscarinic receptor binding in anesthetized rhesus monkeys with the PET radioligand [18F]FP-TZTP, 3-(3-(3[18F]fluoropropylthio)-1,2,5-thiadiazol-4-yl)-1,2,5,6-tetrahydro-1-methylpyridine (Kiesewetter et al, 1995) before and after procaine administration.

We hypothesized that the binding of [18F]FP-TZTP would be reduced by procaine as a function of the dose administered. In addition, we hypothesized that the K1 of the ligand, which correlates strongly with CBF (Carson et al, 1998), would have a pattern of specific limbic increases similar to those observed with CBF in human studies (Ketter et al, 1996), although this effect could be attenuated by anesthesia.

METHODS

Subjects

Four adult male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) weighing 11.7, 8.8, 8.0, and 7.1 kg were studied with brain imaging techniques while receiving general anesthesia during the entire procedure. One animal was unable to complete the whole series of studies due to arterial port failure, and thus was not included in the analysis. All studies were performed under a protocol approved by the NIH Clinical Center Animal Care and Use Committee. The monkeys were routinely monitored by veterinary staff, housed according to American Association for Laboratory Animal Care (AALAC) standards in individual cages, allowed ample feed and were provided with psychological enrichment.

Experimental Design

Animals underwent PET with the radiopharmaceutical 3-(3-(3[18F]fluoropropyl)thio)-1,2,5-thiadiazol-4-yl)-1,2,5,6-tetrahydro-1-methylpyridine ([18F]FP-TZTP) while anesthetized with isoflurane. A total of 18 experiments, six with each animal, were conducted on separate days with each experiment separated by 20–180 days. On each study day, two dynamic PET scan sessions were collected, including an initial baseline scanning session with saline administration followed by a second with continuous infusion of procaine, at one of six doses, ranging from zero, 0.015625, 0.03125, 0.0625, 0.125, to 0.5 mg/kg/min (active drug doses referred to as dose 1 to dose 5, respectively). The order of procaine doses was randomized. These doses were chosen based on self-administration studies (Ford and Balster, 1976; Hammerbeck and Mitchell, 1978), while being safely under the seizure-inducing levels (Babb et al, 1979). These infusion rates were determined from procaine infusion rates from self-administration studies of 1 mg/kg per injection at an average rate of 24 injections per hour (Ford and Balster, 1976) and 4 mg/kg per injection at an average rate of nine injections per hour (Hammerbeck and Mitchell, 1978).

Radiopharmaceutical

[18F]FP-TZTP (Kiesewetter et al, 1995, 1999) was developed as an extension of the structural class of M2 agonists made available by Nova Nordisk (Sauerberg et al, 1992). FP-TZTP exhibits modest selectivity for M2 (2.2 nM) over M1 (7 nM) receptors. Studies of crossreactivity with other neurotransmitter systems showed low affinity for all biogenic amine systems evaluated. FP-TZTP exhibited affinity for sigma-1 (62 nM) and 5HT1 receptor (2 μM) (Kiesewetter et al, 1995). The uniform uptake of [18F]FP-TZTP across the brain resembles the distribution of M2 receptors, which is consistent with M2 selective binding. In rats, [18F]FP-TZTP displays high uptake and high specific binding as determined by ex vivo autoradiography (Kiesewetter et al, 1999). In muscarinic knockout mice, the M2 knockout mouse is the only one that shows significantly reduced [18F]FP-TZTP uptake in all brain tissues regions (Jagoda et al, 2003). Taken together, these data support [18F]FP-TZTP preferential M2 binding in vivo. Tracer kinetic modeling for [18F]FP-TZTP has been developed by Carson et al (1998), so that PET data can be converted into parametric images of total binding (V, volume of distribution) and radioligand delivery (K1). The binding of [18F]FP-TZTP has been shown to be decreased by administration of physostigmine, and thus the binding was sensitive to endogenous acetylcholine (Carson et al, 1998). While other potential ligands, such as the nonselective muscarinic agonist CI-979 (Hartvig et al, 1997) or nonselective muscarinic antagonist [11C]-scopolamine (Frey et al, 1992), and [11C]NMPB (Zubieta et al, 2001) could contribute additional information pertaining to the profile of in vivo actions of potential agents on muscarinic receptors, [18F]FP-TZTP was chosen over others due to its preferential binding to M2 receptors. Furthermore, the study design was conducive to using an 18F compound, particularly because a single synthesis generated the total radiotracer required for both phases of each study day.

Procedure

On the morning of the each procedure, the fasted animal was initially given 0.5 mg ketamine and 5 cc of 2.5% sodium pentathol intramuscularly to produce quasianesthesia for the placement of three i.v. lines, usually in both radial veins and a femoral vein, and intubation for eventual general anesthesia. The animal was transported to the PET suite and placed in the PET scanner and general anesthesia was induced with inhalation of 1–2% isoflurane using a Stevens–Johnson anesthesia machine.

The head was positioned in a stereotactic headholder for coronal image acquisition. An arterial line was attached to a permanent subcutaneous arterial port (Model 21-Y036, Sims Deltec, St Paul, MN) implanted in the femoral artery, a temperature probe was inserted into the rectum, and cardiopulmonary monitoring equipment leads were placed on the animal to monitor their cardiovascular, pulmonary, and thermoregulatory functions throughout the entire procedure (electrocardiogram (EKG), blood pressure, respiration rate, end-tidal pCO2, and temperature). Once cardiopulmonary measures were stable under anesthesia, the experimental procedures began.

After a transmission scan, two [18F]FP-TZTP dynamic scan sessions were acquired. First, a series of dynamic scans were collected over 180 min with saline infusion. After the primary data collection period of 90 min, procaine administration began and continued for the remainder of the experimental procedure. Sterile 10% procaine hydrochloride solution (Sanofi Winthrop Pharmaceuticals) was diluted with saline to desired concentrations on each experimental day and kept in the dark until experiments began, as procaine is light sensitive.

Procaine was administered i.v. in a continuous fashion with a Harvard pump. The syringe and i.v. lines were wrapped with aluminum foil to limit light exposure. The procaine dosing strategy involved a two-step infusion rate; a loading dose, which was double the target dose, was administered for 40 min and was followed by the target or maintenance dose for the remainder of the scanning session. At 40 min after beginning the target dose, the second set of dynamic scans was acquired over 90 min, while the constant procaine infusion continued (mean interval between scans 185±2 min). Upon completion of the experiment, the intravenous and arterial lines and monitoring equipment were removed, the animal was taken out of the PET scanner, returned to its home cage and allowed to recover from anesthesia.

[18F]FP-TZTP was synthesized for each study day according to previously published methods (Kiesewetter et al, 1995, 1999). The product from a single radiosynthesis was split for two injections. The mean activity injected was 1.0±0.1 mCi for the first injection (saline scan) and 3.5±1.6 mCi for the second injection (procaine scan); mean [18F]FP-TZTP mass was 0.4±0.2 nmol for the first injection and 4.5±2.1 nmol for the second injection; mean specific activity injected was 2700±1000 mCi/μmol for the first injection and 840±310 mCi/μmol for the second injection. More radioactivity was given for the second injection to attenuate the effect of the residual radioactivity from the first injection.

Arterial blood samples were drawn from the indwelling arterial port throughout the scanning procedure. A total of 29 blood samples (0.5 ml) taken over the scanning period were centrifuged and 0.1 ml plasma aliquots were counted in a calibrated gamma counter to generate time activity curves. Seven 1 ml blood samples were obtained at 0,3,8,15,30,50 and 90 min after injection for determination of the unmetabolized radiotracer fractions by thin layer chromatography (TLC) according to methods previously described (Carson et al, 1998).

Procaine levels were determined from three 2 ml blood samples taken at 0, 15, and 45 min after the second injection of the radiotracer (40, 55, and 85 min after maintenance dose was initiated); two drops of sodium arsenite per milliliter blood were added immediately to inhibit procaine metabolism by butyrylcholinesterases in blood. The samples were assayed for procaine levels by gas chromatography measuring the free procaine level in plasma (National Medical Services, Inc., Willow Grove, PA). Steady-state plasma concentrations were achieved through the loading/maintenance dose strategy, with procaine concentrations having a coefficient of variation of 9%. No trends were observed over time among the three procaine samples. The mean plasma concentrations achieved are presented in Table 1.

Table 1 The Effects of Procaine Administration on the Functional Response of the Cortex

Imaging Data Collection and Analysis

Image collection and analysis followed the methods of Carson et al (1998). Images were acquired with the General Electric Advance tomograph (DeGrado et al, 1994) in three- dimensional mode, which collects 35 slices simultaneously with a 4.25 interslice distance and a 6 mm isotropic reconstructed resolution. Reconstructed scans were corrected for attenuation, scatter, random emissions, and deadtime, and calibrated in nCi/ml.

[18F]FP-TZTP functional images of delivery rate from the plasma (K1 (ml plasma/min/ml tissue)) and equilibrium volume of distribution (V (ml plasma/ml tissue)) or total binding were computed from the dynamic scans collected over the initial 45 min, utilizing a kinetic model with an arterial input function that has been corrected for metabolites (Carson et al, 1998). Initially, the time delay between brain and blood sampling (Δt) was determined by a one-compartment model fitting three parameters (Δt, V, K1). Functional images of V and K1 were created by first generating a pixel by pixel time activity curve adjusting for the global Δt, and then fitting to a two-parameter (K1 and V) model.

For the second injection, the data were adjusted for residual radioactivity and residual metabolites remaining from the first injection (baseline scanning). Images were corrected for residual activity and metabolites from the first injection with identical methodology as that of Carson et al (1998). Briefly, the model equation for the second injection was modified to include a nonzero initial radioactivity concentration (C0), which clears exponentially (exp(−k2t)), where k2 is the clearance rate constant of the second injection. For each pixel, C0 was estimated by averaging the pixel value for the 30 min preceding the second injection (corrected for decay). This extrapolated background radioactivity amounted to less than 10% of total activity for the second injection. The magnitude of metabolites remaining from the first injection was estimated in a similar manner, and affected the early portion of the input function for the second injection.

Regions of interest (ROIs) were defined on coronal magnetic resonance imaging scans (MRIs) using the rhesus monkey atlas of Paxinos et al (2000). T1 weighted images were acquired on a GE Signa (1.5 Tesla), with a sequence 3D SPGR (X=1 mm, Y=1 mm, Z=0.39 mm), while sedated with ketamine and robinol on a separate day from the PET studies. The V and K1 images were coregistered to the structural MRIs using the Automated Image Registration (AIR) algorithm (Woods et al, 1998) that transformed and resliced the functional images into the coordinate system of the MRIs. Primary ROIs were anterior cingulate, amygdala, and basal forebrain structures ventral striatal and pallidal nuclei that were particularly activated in the human blood flow studies. Additional ROIs were defined in prefrontal, parietal, occipital, temporal cortices, thalamus, striatum, posterior cingulate, hippocampus, cerebellum, and brainstem. Primary sensory (V1, A1 and S1) and motor cortex (M1) were also measured, but not included into statistical inference testing as they were encompassed in some of the previously mentioned regions. The mean V and K1 levels were calculated from baseline and procaine scans for each ROI and a cortical average (prefrontal, anterior cingulate, posterior cingulate, parietal, occipital, and temporal cortices) was obtained.

The V values measured at baseline (Vbase) and with procaine (Vproc) administration were corrected for nonspecific binding by subtracting a uniform value of 7 ml/ml (rationale explained below) taken from preblocking studies with [18F]FP-TZTP (Carson et al, 1998), yielding binding potential (BP) values for each scan (BP1=Vbase−7; BP2=Vproc−7). Percent blockade was calculated by

The relationship of average cortical blockade (ΔBP) to procaine dose was evaluated with repeated measure ANOVA with procaine dose as the one within factor, (SuperAnova, v1.11, Abacus Concepts, Berkeley, CA).

IC50 (inhibitory concentration for 50% blockade) values were calculated by two methods using Graphpad Prism software (v3.0a for Macintosh, Graphpad Software, Inc., San Diego, CA), and were compared for best fit. The first method determined IC50 from the percent blockade measures (ΔBP) with the two-parameter model equation:

where ΔBPmax is the maximum percent blockade achievable with procaine. Ligand concentrations [L] were based on mean procaine plasma levels acquired during each scanning period. The second fitting method used a sigmoidal dose-response model fitting three parameters to the equation

where BPmax is the binding potential in the absence of procaine. These models differ in that the three-parameter model only used data from the second scan of each day and assumed a fixed value for BPmax on all days for all animals. The two-parameter model used the baseline results (BP1) measured for each animal on each experimental day to directly calculate ΔBP. The three-parameter model also included the second-scan information on the zero-dose experimental day. Regional variations of IC50 values were assessed with repeated measure ANOVA with two within factors, region and procaine dose.

The statistical inferential methods for K1 values were essentially identical to those described to assess BP measures. K1 ROI values (without any corrections) measured at baseline (K1-base) and with procaine (K1-proc) administration were used to calculate percent change by

The relationship of average cortical change of K1 to procaine dose was evaluated with repeated measure ANOVA with one within factor, procaine dose. Regional variations of K1 change were assessed with repeated measure ANOVA with two within factors, region and dose. When appropriate post hoc means comparisons were conducted, as in the case of testing the a priori hypothesis of selective anterior paralimbic, K1 increases. Variability of peripheral measures was also examined with repeated measures ANOVA, with two within factors of dose and times.

Owing to the relative uniformity of muscarinic M2 receptor distribution across the brain, nonspecific binding measures were estimated in the following way. Since individual values were not available, a constant value for the nonspecific distribution of volume was used. Several constants were evaluated (6, 7, 8, and 8.7 ml/ml (the mean cortex value reported in Carson et al (1998) from preblocking studies)), but the IC50 estimates, regardless of constant value, remained essentially identical. For example, for the mean cortex IC50, a value of 1.34 μM was obtained when using a constant value of 8.7 ml/ml, as compared to 1.31 μM obtained with 7 ml/ml for nonspecific distribution volume estimates. The constant value of 7 ml/ml, approximately the mean value of the cerebellum from preblocking studies was chosen for the analyses, under the assumption that nonspecific binding is uniform across the brain. Note that the underestimation of nonspecific binding in this study would have the effect of underestimating the maximum percent blockade. Again using the cortex average as an example region, the ΔBPmax using 8.7 ml/ml was 100.1%, while 7 ml/ml produced a value of 88.4%.

RESULTS

Control Values

On one day, each animal received saline administration during the second scanning period, in lieu of procaine, to establish test–retest reliability. Cortical [18F]FP-TZTP BP did not differ significantly on retest (mean±SD saline1, 21.4±2.9 ml/ml; saline2, 23.1±3.4; F=1.48, df=1,2, p=ns). However, there was a slight tendency for regional [18F]FP-TZTP BP changes from saline1 to saline2, as indicated by a region × scan interaction (F=2.28, df=12,24, p=0.04). Most regions increased slightly 1.0–2.4 ml/ml (average change 5.9%), but the thalamus, cerebellum, and the brainstem remained at the same level (<0.8 ml/ml change). This slight trend of BP with time on anesthesia was also seen in the work of Carson et al (1998) and will tend to result in an underestimation of ΔBP.

Cortical values for ΔK1 also showed a slight, but nonsignificant increase (5.7%) from saline1 to saline2, a nonsignificant increase (mean±SD saline1, 0.42±0.09; saline2, 0.45±0.13; F=0.53, df=1,2, p=ns). Again there was significant regional variation (F=2.32, df=12,24, p=0.04) from saline1 to saline 2. Most regions increased 0.02–0.133 ml/min/ml (average change 11.2%), but parietal and occipital cortex remained at the same level. These increases in K1 are consistent with the work of Carson et al (1998) and most likely reflects time-related CBF increases associated with isoflurane administration (McPherson et al, 1994). These small increases in K1 measures indicate that any procaine-induced flow increases may be slightly overestimated.

Peripheral Measures

There was no significant effect of procaine administration dose on heart rate (HR: F=1.21, df=5,10, p=ns), systolic or diastolic blood pressure (SYS: F=0.90, df=5,10, p=ns; DIA: F=1.04, df=5,10, p=ns;), respiration rate (RR: F=1.26, df=5,10, p=ns), or pCO2 (pCO2: F=1.46, df=5,10, p=ns). Although most measures significantly decreased with time (HR: F=11.27, df=5,10, p=0.000; SYS: F=6.47, df=5,10, p=0.000; DIA: F=2.45, df=5,10, p=0.030; RR: F=6.64, df=5,10, p=0.000; pCO2: F=8.37, df=5,10, p=0.000), a drug × time interaction was only observed with pCO2 which was significantly increased on the highest dose (0.500 mg/kg/min) after procaine administration and returned to baseline levels after cessation of the loading phase (HR: F=0.51, df=5,10, p=ns; SYS: F=0.64, df=5,10, p=ns; DIA: F=0.94, df=5,10, p=ns; RR: F=0.83, df=5,10, p=ns; pCO2: F=1.59, df=5,10, p=0.016).

Global Effects

The IC50 values generated by the two fitting methods (two-parameter and three-parameter) differed by approximately a factor of 2. For example, the average cortical ROI IC50 values were 1.31 μM and 0.75 μM for the two- and three-parameter models, respectively. The percent coefficient of variation of IC50 from the fits was comparable at 30%, thus model selection could not be performed based on goodness-of-fit. In subsequent results, the two-parameter results were chosen over the three-parameter results, because the former model directly accounted for day-to-day and animal-to-animal variability of baseline [18F]FP-TZTP binding potential (Table 1).

Figure 1 depicts (top and middle panels) representative [18F]FP-TZTP V images with and without procaine coadministration in a single animal at the highest dose of 0.5 mg/kg/min. Baseline and procaine global [18F]FP-TZTP specific binding is reported in Table 1. Procaine, in a dose-related manner, blocked average cortical [18F]FP-TZTP total specific binding (F=51.45, df=5,10, p=0.0001; Figure 2).

Figure 1
figure 1

(a) Coronal slice presentation of volume of distribution images (V) from a single animal displaying the significant reduction in specific binding of [18F]FP-TZTP by procaine. Global reduction in specific binding in this subject was 86.9%. Upper panel—baseline binding is fairly uniform; lower panel—procaine administration (0.5 mg/kg/min) is essentially identical to preblocking studies with FP-TZTP (Carson et al, 1998). (b) Procaine increased K1 measures the greatest in the anterior cingulate, striatum, and basal forebrain nuclei. Data represents a subtraction image in a single monkey (0.016 mg/kg/min procaine minus baseline). The PET images were registered to the animal's MR images; coordinates are mm rostral (+) and caudal (−) to ear canals.

Figure 2
figure 2

Procaine blocks cortical [18F]FP-TZTP binding potential in a dose-related fashion that follows the saturation binding curve (solid line) with an IC50 of 1.4 μM with an estimated accuracy of ±0.41 μM. The maximum blockade was 87% and minimum was 23%.

Procaine significantly increased average cortical [18F]FP-TZTP ΔK1 values (F=6.23, df=2,10, p=0.0071), however, not in a sigmoidal dose-related manner (Table 1). The peak change (52%) in K1 occurred at the low dose of 0.016 mg/kg/ml (mean procaine plasma level=0.65 mM; see Figure 1 bottom panel) and had successively smaller increases with each higher dose, although still increased by 20% at the highest dose of 0.5 mg/kg/min. The post hoc analysis of dose revealed all doses combined resulted in significantly higher ΔK1 values compared to the baseline study (baseline: 5.7±17.0%; procaine: 28.6±17.3; F=11.28, df=1, 10, p=0.007). This effect was attributable primarily to 0.016 mg/kg/min dose with a 51.7% increase (dose 1 vs baseline: F=27.16, df=1, 10, p=0.002) and also to the 0.031 mg/kg/min dose with a 31.5% increase (dose 2 vs baseline: F=23.54, df=1, 10, p=0.02).

Regional Effects

The dose–response relationship of [18F]FP-TZTP specific binding across the regions mirrored the global findings (Table 2). The regional IC50 did not vary significantly across the areas measured and ranged from 1.00 to 2.25 μM (F=0.88; df=12,24; p=ns).

Table 2 Regional [18F]FP-TZTP Parameter Values

[18F]FP-TZTP ΔK1 (Table 2) varied significantly across ROIs (F=5.58; df=2,12; p=0.0001) and across procaine doses (F=5.61; df=4,8; p=0.02); however, there was no interaction of region × dose (F=0.79; df=60,120; p=ns). In the regional post hoc analysis, the ΔK1 of the anterior paralimbic areas (including the amygdala, anterior cingulate, basal forebrain nuclei, hippocampus, and prefrontal cortex, mean ΔK1=34.7%) was significantly increased compared with other regions (including posterior cingulate, occipital cortex, parietal cortex, cerebellum, and brainstem; mean ΔK1=21.0%, F=29.05, df=1,24, p=0.0001).

DISCUSSION

In this PET study of anesthetized monkeys, procaine blocked the binding of a muscarinic ligand in a dose-related manner, globally and uniformly across the primate brain. The IC50 for the cortex was estimated to be 1.31 μM, which corresponds to plasma levels achieved between infusion doses 0.0312 and 0.0625 mg/kg/min procaine. This is in the proximity of in vitro assessments of M2 muscarinic receptor Kd of 4 μM as determined in rat hippocampal slices and guinea-pig ileum (Hisayama et al, 1989; Sharkey et al, 1988). Given the nearly 100% blockade achieved, these results support a direct interaction of procaine with the same muscarinic receptors to which [18F]FP-TZTP is binding. Furthermore, the V images of procaine administration at the maximal dose in this study were essentially identical to the V images obtained during the preblocking experiments with [18F]FP-TZTP studies (Carson et al, 1998).

Although procaine is known to selectively increase limbic electrophysiologic activity in animals and anterior paralimbic perfusion in humans (Heynen et al, 1995; Ketter et al, 1996; Munson et al, 1970; Parekh et al, 1995; Post, 1981; Post et al, 1984; Racine et al, 1975, 1979; Wagman et al, 1967), the lack of regional variation in IC50 values suggests that the competition of procaine with [18F]FP-TZTP binding sites is similar across the brain. Furthermore, these data suggest that procaine's limbic selectivity is most likely not a result of limbic muscarinic receptors having enhanced regional sensitivity to procaine over that of other brain regions.

Nonetheless, the selective activation of paralimbic structures by procaine could still involve cholinergic mechanisms. Amygdalar neurons via muscarinic mediation have been associated with bursting phenomena (Yajeya et al, 1997), postsynaptically (Washburn and Moises, 1992) and presynaptically (Sugita et al, 1991). Bursting activity was further tied to a complement of presynaptic muscarinic receptors that were estimated to be comprised of 50% M3, 30% M2, and 20% or less M1, but most likely not due to the well characterized M-current (Yajeya et al, 1997). Moreover, similar neuronal depolarization responses due to cholinergic mechanisms have been implicated in other paralimbic structures such as hippocampus, entorhinal, and piriform cortices and anterior cingulate (Benson et al, 1988; Colino and Halliwell, 1993; Hasselmo and Bower, 1992; Klink and Alonso, 1997; McCormick and Prince, 1986), as well as septum, basal forebrain, striatum, thalamus, and neocortical structures (Hasuo et al, 1988; Hsu et al, 1995; McCormick and Prince, 1987; Szerb et al, 1994). Thus, it is possible that procaine acting on M2 receptors could initiate bursting activity and enhance firing in limbic regions. These results suggest that the interaction of muscarinic receptors with the local neuronal environment could contribute to procaine's limbic effects.

It is likely that procaine is acting as an agonist since: cholinomimetics induce kindling similar to procaine (Wasterlain et al, 1981); and physostigmine weakly facilitates procaine-induced kindling while atropine substantially slows this process (Heynen et al, 1995). Moreover, the clinical effects of physostigmine share similarities to procaine (Janowsky et al, 1986).

Conversely, however, antagonist activity is suggested by procaine competitively inhibiting acetylcholine-induced contraction (Ishii and Shimo, 1984) of guinea-pig cecum. Furthermore, procaine inhibits opening of cation channels on guinea-pig ileal smooth muscle cells thus affecting the acetylcholine-induced cationic currents; GTPγS currents are inhibited in a similar manner (Chen et al, 1993). Lastly, procaine is known to have a biphasic effect on neuronal excitation with anticonvulsant activity at lower concentrations and proconvulsant activity at higher concentrations (De Jong, 1994; Foldes et al, 1960, 1965), which could reflect both agonist and antagonist cholinergic activity.

Cerebral blood flow, as measured by K1, significantly increased globally and regionally in the anterior paralimbic regions with all procaine doses, but the relationship of procaine dose and K1 increase appears to be more complex than that observed with the binding data. The most robust increase was observed at the lowest dose of procaine and all subsequent higher doses resulted in less of an increase above baseline saline condition. The largest K1 changes occurred at plasma levels near the IC50. This suggests that the K1 effects associated with the lower doses could possibly be related to M2 muscarinic blockade. However, the apparent reduction of the K1 increase at the subsequently higher doses might be explained by procaine's effects on sigma or other receptors, or be a direct effect of the M2 receptors yielding a biphasic relationship of procaine dose on K1. Allosteric modulation of the muscarinic receptor has been documented with cocaine (Flynn et al, 1992), and given the structural similarities between procaine and cocaine, such modulation could account for a smaller change in K1 at higher doses. Another potential contributor to the complex K1 changes observed in this study could be the reduced ability of the model to fit K1 with higher receptor occupancy (Carson et al, 1998), resulting in an underestimation of K1. This would yield lower flow changes than would be expected with a direct receptor blockade to flow relationship.

While it would be enticing to suggest that these K1 changes reflect actual blood flow alterations, caution should be exercised. Despite the strong correlation of cerebral blood flow and K1 (r=0.85; Carson et al, 1998), the relationship of blood flow changes to procaine administration in this study can only be inferred. However, the regional pattern of K1 increases in this study is similar to the blood flow increases observed in humans (Ketter et al, 1996). The K1 data indicate that procaine binding to cholinergic M2 receptors may contribute to the overall activation of anterior paralimbic regions. This suggestion is supported by preferential reductions in prefrontal cortex blood flow after scopolamine injection in humans in xenon-133 PET studies (Honer et al, 1988), which are reversed by physostigmine (Prohovnik et al, 1997). Also, carbachol injection into the substantia inominata increases blood flow globally and in many limbic areas in rats (Barbelivien et al, 1999).

Procaine can have autonomic effects, such as increases or decreases in heart rate or blood pressure, when administered i.v. in humans (Foldes et al, 1965, 1960; Haasio et al, 1988; Scott, 1975). In this study, the minimal autonomic decreases observed are consistent with known effects related to time on anesthesia. It is notable that significant cerebral effects occurred while the peripheral effects were maximal.

There are several limitations to this study. Few animals were studied; despite this, the dose–response relationship of procaine blockade of [18F]FP-TZTP was robust and consistent across each monkey (Figure 2). Further, the use of only rhesus monkeys suggest caution in extrapolation to man.

General anesthesia precluded the collection of behavioral measures during procaine administration restricting the scope of the study; the ability to relate the degree of blockade to behavioral measures would be a key element in determining a clearer role of the muscarinic system in the emotional and sensory effects of procaine. The general anesthesia (Durieux, 1996) may have confounded the K1 changes observed, or interacted with procaine to alter the muscarinic activity (Brett et al, 1988; Dilger et al, 1992, 1993). Ketamine–muscarinic interactions (Durieux, 1995) may have also confounded the binding changes observed, but these effects would be present at baseline and with procaine. Anesthesia may have also limited the physiological changes potentially induced by procaine, either directly or indirectly.

Finally, different methods from those used in the human studies were necessary for the acquisition of the binding potential data, including the [18F]FP-TZTP measurement after plasma procaine levels had leveled off, as well as the use of a constant infusion paradigm rather than a bolus administration. This may have resulted in assessing the procaine-flow relationship under different physiological conditions than in the human studies, limiting the ability to extrapolate from this study to the human studies.

With these caveats in mind, considerable evidence suggests a role for the cholinergic system in normal and pathological emotions. An enhanced ratio of cholinergic to adrenergic function has been hypothesized to play a role in mania and depression (Fritze, 1993; Janowsky et al, 1972b). Cholinergic challenge studies lend support to this theory. For example, physostigmine can reduce manic symptoms and exacerbate depressive symptoms in bipolar patients (Janowsky et al, 1972a). Furthermore, physostigmine administration results in relapse of depressive symptoms in bipolar patients successfully treated with lithium, while healthy controls do not develop depressed mood. These patients have concomitant hormonal disturbances and emotional arousal expressed as dysphoria (Janowsky et al, 1986).

Arecoline, a nonselective muscarinic agonist, has also induced dysphoria in mood disorder patients whether or not they were currently depressed; this was greater in individuals with a family history of depression compared to those without such history (Gillin et al, 1991; Nurnberger et al, 1989). Both arecoline (Sitaram et al, 1980) and donepezil, a cholinesterase inhibitor (Perlis et al, 2002) decrease REM latency in patients with major depression, but not in healthy controls.

In a [15O] blood flow study analyzing the functional associativity of cholinergic forebrain regions with and without procaine administration, some abnormalilties were found to normalize with procaine (Benson et al, unpublished data). At baseline, patients with mood disorders compared to healthy controls show significantly weaker positive relationships among the cholinergic forebrain regions, which became stronger and similar to controls with procaine. Thus, commonly reported alterations in prefrontal, anterior cingulate, temporal, striatal, and cerebellar brain regions in mood disorders (see reviews; Dougherty and Rauch, 1997; Ketter et al, 1997; Drevets, 1998) may have a cholinergic component to their dysregulation.

In conclusion, these data demonstrate that intravenous procaine administration in anesthetized monkeys was associated with a dose-related blockade of the M2 muscarinic ligand, [18F]FP-TZTP. This is the first demonstration of muscarinic cholinergic binding of procaine in primates, in vivo. In addition, cerebral blood flow (K1) was significantly increased in limbic regions of the brain. If significant receptor occupancy occurs at comparable plasma levels in humans, these data suggest a possible cholinergic contribution to the robust emotional and sensory effects of procaine and its ability to selectively activate amygdala and closely related anterior paralimbic structures. Thus, based on the binding and flow data presented here, procaine could prove to be another way of assessing muscarinic receptor tone in limbic areas in patients compared to healthy volunteers. Further studies to explore the relationship of muscarinic receptor activity with behavioral assessments, and use of agonist and antagonist ligands could delineate a clearer role of the cholinergic system in the effects of procaine on emotion.