Introduction

Kin selection theory predicts that individuals gain inclusive fitness benefits from cooperating with relatives, because the costs of helping is outweighed by the increased fitness of individuals with whom they share genes identical by decent1,2. Coined as Hamilton’s rule, it follows that inclusive fitness gained through helping behaviour will generally increase with the relatedness coefficient between helper and receiver1,2. This should select for kin discrimination, as directing help towards relatives requires the ability to distinguish kin from non-kin, based on either environmentally or genetically determined cues3,4. Indeed, there is wide evidence for kin discrimination in social animals, emphasizing the significance of nepotism, i.e. preferential cooperation with kin, in social evolution5,6. Given that recognition systems are costly to maintain, kin discrimination is expected to be most pronounced when there are significant inclusive fitness benefits of nepotism5. If these conditions are relaxed, or cooperation occurs as a by-product to otherwise selfish actions, selection for kin discrimination may be diminished and recognition systems lost. This could explain the apparent absence of kin discrimination and nepotism in a range of cooperative species5,7,8,9,10. There are several contexts under which we might expect relaxed selection for nepotism in cooperating groups11,12: 1) when benefits of cooperative traits that directly increase group productivity7,13,14, e.g. group foraging and task differentiation15,16, override benefits of nepotism, 2) if there is low variation in genetic relatedness among cooperating individuals, inclusive fitness benefits of cooperation will be retained while the maintenance of a recognition system may be unnecessary5,17,18,19; and 3) in situations where there is little competitive interaction with non-relatives, the cost of maintaining a recognition system may outweigh the benefits10,20.

While the benefits of kin discrimination in directing help towards relatives seem unequivocal in the evolution of cooperation, the role of nepotism in maintaining and structuring social groups once cooperation has evolved remains enigmatic. Using permanently social spiders as our study system, we tested the prediction that nepotism can be lost under conditions where kin recognition may no longer be beneficial. Social spiders form colonies consisting of highly related individuals, owing to within-colony breeding and lack of pre-mating dispersal21,22. This results in highly genetically homogeneous individuals within groups23 and limited gene flow among colonies23,24,25. Colony members cooperate in brood care, nest building, web maintenance, feeding and prey capture21. Sub-social species in the same genus are characterized by extended maternal care and a transient cooperative juvenile stage, followed by pre-mating dispersal and a solitary adult life21,22. Sociality in spiders is suggested to have evolved via the sub-social route26, where ecological constraints on dispersal in sub-social progenitors may have favoured juvenile philopatry and elimination of pre-mating dispersal, leading to the formation of cooperatively foraging groups27,28. Cooperative foraging, where spiders jointly subdue and feed on a captured prey may create the potential for within-group competition and, due to spiders feeding mode, it is open to exploitation. This is because some individuals may invest in prey capture and costly digestive enzymes that serve to feed by liquefying the prey content, while others may feed with little prior investment29, resulting in the depletion of the common resource. Sub-social species may solve this problem, also known as ‘the tragedy of the commons’ and reduce the disadvantages of kin competition through kin-biased cooperation since the costs of cheating relatives through selfish acts are high30,31. Indeed, net benefits of foraging with kin revealed by improved feeding efficiency and growth rates of groups with higher genetic relatedness, are documented in two sub-social species of the genus Stegodyphus, suggesting that kin discrimination is an important factor in the evolution of group living in these spiders32,33. In contrast, benefits of kin discrimination once permanent sociality has evolved are less clear, as high relatedness and genetic similarity among group members18, infrequent encounters with non-kin owing to a sedentary life-style21 and direct benefits of cooperation7,13, may lead to relaxed selection for kin discrimination.

We investigated whether kin discrimination exists in cooperative foraging in the permanently social spider Stegodyphus dumicola (Eresidae). To examine evidence for nepotism, we manipulated relatedness levels among groups of interacting spiders engaged in cooperative feeding in three distinct experiments. In each experiment we used geographical distances between colonies as a proxy for genetic similarity23,26. Spiders that originated from the same colony were considered siblings, since colonies are established by single sib-mated females34. In the first experiment we established three treatments: groups of siblings originating from the same colony, groups of non-siblings from different colonies within the same population and groups of non-siblings from colonies originating from different populations. In a second experiment we used double-mated females to create genetic variability in the offspring35. We established treatment groups consisting of offspring broods of similar or mixed genetic composition36,37. Females were mated with two of their brothers, with two males from different colonies within the same population, or with two males from different colonies originating from different populations. In an additional two groups, females were mated with one brother and one male from within either the same or a different population. In both experiments we assessed spider growth rate over a period of 7–9 weeks, the number of spiders participating in communal feeding and the proportion of prey mass extracted (feeding efficiency) in cooperative feeding events33. In a third experiment, we compared sib groups (same colony) and mixed groups (multiple colonies) of individually marked spiders to examine whether kinship affected individual participation in feeding events.

If spiders show nepotism in communal feeding, we should detect higher growth rate and feeding efficiency in groups with higher relatedness among individuals as documented in sub-social species32,33, whereas the absence of these effects of kinship would suggest a diminished role of nepotism in the permanently social S. dumicola.

Results

Experiment 1

We detected no differential growth rate among different treatment groups over the course of the experiment (Fig. 1A). If nepotistic cooperation resulted in differential growth rate over time, this would be revealed in a significant interaction term33, however mixed-model analyses of variance strongly indicated that groups show similar growth rates (Treatment x Time interaction F = 0.46, df = 10, n = 256, p = 0.91). While growth rate changed over time (Time (ordinal effect) F = 240.01, df = 5, p < 0.0001), no difference among treatment groups was detected (Treatment (fixed effect) F = 0.67, df = 2, p = 0.51). Feeding efficiency (proportion of mass extracted from prey) varied over time with no treatment effects (Fig. 2A, ANCOVA, Treatment x Time interaction F = 0.27, df = 2, n = 181, p = 0.76; Time F = 46.19, df = 1, p < 0.0001; Treatment F = 0.09, df = 2, p = 0.91). No difference in the maximum proportion of spiders feeding communally was detected among treatment groups (Fig. 3A, GLM binomial error, Treatment x Time interaction χ2 = 1.89, df = 2, n = 181, p = 0.38; Treatment χ2 = 0.23, df = 2, p = 0.88; Time χ2 = 0.0056, df = 1, p = 0.94). Finally, latency to reach the maximum feeding group size observed in a feeding event also did not differ significantly among groups (mean ± SE (minutes), 1st measurement, S 22.4 ± 5.1, NS1 19.3 ± 3.9, NS2, 24.4 ± 4.4; last measurement, S 22.8 ± 6.1, NS1 22.0 ± 7.2, NS2, 15.0 ± 7.2; GLM exponential error, Treatment x Time interaction χ2 = 0.27, df = 2, n = 181, p = 0.87; Treatment χ2 = 0.25, df = 2, p = 0.88; Time χ2 = 0.72, df = 1, p = 0.39).

Figure 1
figure 1

Mean mass of spider groups (mg ± SE) per treatment over the experiment duration (days) in:

(A) Experiment 1, sibs S (n = 19); non-sibs from same NS1 (n = 17) and different populations NS2 (n = 17); (B) Experiment 2, ‘homogeneous’ broods fathered by two sibs HM1 (n = 13), two non-sibs from the same population HM2 (n = 8) and from a different population HM3 (n = 4); and ‘heterogeneous’ broods fathered by a sib and a non-sib from same HT1 (n = 36) and different population HT2 (n = 28). In both experiments there was no difference among treatments in mass increase.

Figure 2
figure 2

Feeding efficiency (proportion of mass extracted from prey) per treatment over the experiment duration (days) in:

(A) Experiment 1, sibs S (n = 19); non-sibs from same NS1 (n = 17) and different populations NS2 (n = 17); (B) Experiment 2, ‘homogeneous’ broods fathered by two sibs HM1 (n = 13), two non-sibs from the same population HM2 (n = 8) and from a different population HM3 (n = 4); and ‘heterogeneous’ broods fathered by a sib and a non-sib from same HT1 (n = 36) and different population HT2 (n = 28). In both experiments there was no difference among treatments in prey mass loss.

Figure 3
figure 3

Maximum proportion of spiders feeding per treatment over the experiment duration (days) in:

(A) Experiment 1, sibs S (n = 19); non-sibs from same NS1 (n = 17) and different populations NS2 (n = 17); (B) Experiment 2, ‘homogeneous’ broods fathered by two sibs HM1 (n = 13), two non-sibs from the same population HM2 (n = 8) and from a different population HM3 (n = 4); and ‘heterogeneous’ broods fathered by a sib and a non-sib from same HT1 (n = 36) and different population HT2 (n = 28). In both experiments there was no difference among treatments in proportion feeding.

Experiment 2

We detected no differential growth rate of the different treatment groups over the course of time (Fig. 1B, mixed-model ANOVA, Treatment x Time interaction F = 0.60, df = 28, n = 586, p = 0.94). Growth rate changed over time (Time (ordinal effect) F = 36.63, df = 7, p < 0.0001) and a marginally significant effect indicated that treatment groups varied slightly in growth rate (Treatment F = 2.41, df = 4, p = 0.048). Feeding efficiency (arcsine-transformed proportion of mass extracted from prey) varied over time with no consistent treatment effect (Fig. 2B, ANCOVA, Treatment x Time interaction F = 4.03, df = 4, n = 563, p = 0.003; Time F = 1.76, df = 1, p = 0.18; Treatment F = 1.04, df = 4, p = 0.38). The maximum proportion of spiders feeding communally varied among treatment groups over the course of the experiment (Fig. 3B, GLM binomial error, Treatment x Time interaction χ2 = 15.21, df = 1, n = 456, p = 0.004; Treatment χ2 = 17.1, df = 4, p = 0.0018; Time χ2 = 3.57, df = 1, p = 0.058). Contrast analysis showed that these differences were not attributed to more related groups feeding in larger proportions (HM versus HT groups, χ2 = 2.92, df = 1, p = 0.08). Latency to reach the maximum feeding group size observed in a feeding event differed significantly among groups (mean ± SE (minutes), 1st measurement, HM1 17.3 ± 3.8, HM2 20 ± 10, HM3 18.6 ± 3.4, HT1 26.8 ± 4.6, HT2 31.5 ± 6.04; last measurement, HM1 41 ± 14.6, HM2 45 ± 25, HM3 26.7 ± 6.7, HT1 25 ± 3.9, HT2 25.2 ± 4.2; GLM exponential error, Treatment x Time interaction χ2 = 9.40, df = 4, n = 499, p = 0.052; Treatment χ2 = 30.51, p < 0.0001, df = 4, Time χ2 = 5.30, p = 0.021). Contrast analyses showed that these differences were not driven by more related groups reaching maximum feeding group size faster than less related groups (HM versus HT groups, χ2 = 3.04, p = 0.08).

Experiment 3

Relatedness among group members did not affect individual participation in cooperative feeding. The proportion of individuals taking part in feeding events did not differ between sib (S) and non-sib (NS) treatments (S, 0.39 ± 0.04; NS, 0.31 ± 0.04; GLM binomial error, χ2 = 2.77, df = 1, n = 28, p = 0.096) and the latency to form feeding groups was similar in both treatments (Wilcoxon test, χ2 = 0.0025, df = 1, n = 27, p = 0.96). Within the NS treatment, spiders originating from two different colonies joined feeding groups in equal proportions (GLM binomial error, χ2 = 0.13, df = 1, p = 0.71).

Discussion

We found no indication that social Stegodyphus dumicola spiders bias cooperation towards close relatives during feeding. In three different experiments, individuals took part in cooperative feeding events regardless of the level of relatedness of their group members. They aggregated to feed in similar numbers and exploited food with similar feeding efficiency, as shown by comparable rates of food extraction and increase in body weight in the different treatment groups. This suggests lack of nepotism via kin discrimination. Indeed, the finding that individuals participate indiscriminately in feeding events in groups of high and low relatedness suggests overall low discrimination.

In contrast to social S. dumicola, kin discrimination has been shown to occur in sub-social species, which are considered a transition stage in the evolution of permanent sociality in spiders32,33. In sub-social Stegodyphus, individuals direct aggression and cannibalism predominantly towards non-sibs38 and young in sib groups experience higher foraging efficiency than those in non-sib groups (S. lineatus33 and S. tentoriicola32). Similar kin recognition has been shown in other sub-social spider species (Diaea ergandros39 and Delena cancerides40). The presumed loss of kin-biased cooperative behaviour in social S. dumicola may be attributed to social or ecological contexts: firstly, a high level of relatedness among colony members reduces both the need for kin recognition and the effectiveness of kin-biased cooperation. Secondly, a sedentary foraging mode and life style should reduce the potential for competitive interactions with non-kin and hence the benefit of maintaining a presumed costly recognition system; and thirdly, strong direct benefits of cooperation within the group can outweigh any potential advantages of directing cooperation preferentially towards close kin. Any of these factors could change the cost-benefit ratio of exerting nepotistic behaviour in the transition from sub-social to permanent social behaviour and result in a reduced role of kin discrimination once permanently sociality had evolved.

Genetic relatedness within colonies

The breeding system and modes of colony establishment in social spiders result in high genetic homogeneity within colonies. There is no juvenile natal dispersal and spiders mate within the parent colony over multiple generations, resulting in extreme inbreeding21,41. New nests are established by dispersal of mated females; in S. dumicola, single, mated females disperse on silk by ballooning or bridging42 and if successful, will produce a clutch of siblings to form the start of a new colony21. An alternative mode of colony establishment is by budding or fission of large nests, whereby groups of large juveniles split off the parent nest to form daughter nests that may then become separated from the parent nest21. The daughter nests thus contain a subset of the original inbred colony. Both modes of colony establishment result in high relatedness among colony members and the perpetuation of the inbred parental lineage. Movement between colonies is uncommon and restricted to nearby nests41, which are likely derived from the same female lineage and therefore composed of genetically similar individuals34. Colony establishment by either method involves small breeding populations and this can lead in the long run to overall loss of genetic variation in the population. This process is further enhanced by the female-biased sex ratio and consequent decrease in effective population size. Overall low genetic variation within colonies, across populations and indeed across the entire species’ range characterizes the social spider species that have been investigated to date21,22,43. Given this population genetic structure of inbred colonies with extremely low genetic variation, kin-biased cooperation would seem unlikely to provide any additional benefit to that derived from cooperating indiscriminately within the colony.

Ecological context

Direct fitness benefits of group living do not require that cooperation be directed preferentially toward kin7,13. In social spiders, group foraging increases the probability of gaining a successful meal, as prey capture rate increases44,45 and spiders can subdue and capture larger prey46,47,48. In addition, cooperative foraging reduces per capita energy expenditure on web-construction44 and possibly on the production of digestive enzymes. Fitness benefits also come through allomaternal care, as young that are raised cooperatively are larger and have greater survival than those raised solitarily49. If the magnitude of these direct benefits increases with increasing group size, processes such as group augmentation may further select for group cohesion13,50 and override any benefits of kin-biased cooperation. Social spiders have a sedentary foraging mode and lifestyle, they build capture webs surrounding the colony that intercept mobile insect prey and they undergo the entire life cycle within the colony. Therefore social spiders do not experience competitive interactions with other colonies, which might favour group closure and nest-mate recognition systems as seen in most social insects20,51,52.

Indiscriminate cooperation does not necessarily imply that social spiders lack a colony or nest recognition mechanism. In social insects, for example, nest-mate or kin recognition cues are mediated in part by distinct cuticular hydrocarbon fingerprints53 and this may be the case in sub-social spiders as well. Cuticular hydrocarbons were identified as potential kin recognition cues carrying information about family identity in sub-social S. lineatus54. In social spiders it is frequently noted that unrelated individuals from different colonies and even heterospecific members of the same genus, are readily accepted into a colony with no obvious aggression55. This suggests that social spiders do not respond to cues borne on the spider’s body. However, other sources of information regarding nest or colony identity might be available to spiders, for example silk56. Thus, whether colony-related cues occur in social spiders and if so, what might be their possible functions, remain unanswered questions.

Transition to permanent sociality

Our results are in accordance with a scenario where kin discrimination may have had an important role in the evolution of cooperative group living, particularly in sub-social species that lack the inbred colony structure. At the transition to permanent, inbred sociality, kin-biased cooperation was no longer beneficial, while traits that confer direct benefits were favoured. Kin discrimination and nepotism may be costly traits to maintain; kin discrimination requires sensory capabilities of detection and discrimination among chemical cues, while nepotism would result in fewer foraging opportunities in non-kin environments. To obtain direct benefits of group foraging, individuals only need to cooperate with others in performing a particular task. Direct benefits of cooperation may also be obtained in permanent social groups by means of task differentiation and there is accumulating evidence for task differentiation within social spider colonies57,58. Task specialization in highly social insects increases group productivity15,59 and this may be the case in social spiders. In social spiders, greater group productivity results in rapid colony growth and the production of more females that will disperse to establish new colonies21,22. Collectively, these features suggest that permanent sociality is associated with the emergence of complex social organization where nepotism plays a diminishing role, while direct benefits of task differentiation, cooperative foraging and group defence favours group productivity50,60.

Methods

Population genetic studies of social spiders show that they are characterized by strong population genetic structuring due to inbreeding within colonies, metapopulation dynamics and lack of gene flow among local populations23,25,34. Studies of genetic diversity and population structure in social Stegodyphus showed that populations are highly differentiated. Fst (or phist) estimates are 0.36–0.51 for S. sarasinorum in India (geographic distances range between 7 and 1465 km)25 and averaged 0.31 for populations of S. dumicola in Namibia (geographic distances range between 82 and 515 km)26. In order to obtain sufficient genetic differentiation among individuals from different populations we took samples from populations 100–500 km apart.

Experiment 1: groups of mixed genetic composition

We collected 26 Stegodyphus dumicola colonies consisting of juveniles from two populations, (Polokwane and Modjadjiskloof) in South Africa during November 2009. Colonies were transported to Ben-Gurion University of the Negev (Israel) and spiders were kept under laboratory conditions (25°–27 °C, 40% humidity and 13:11 light: dark cycle), sprayed with water and fed houseflies (Musca domestica) and cricket nymphs (Acheta domestica) twice a week. To create groups of spiders of homogeneous size, we weighed spiders and formed groups of individuals with similar body mass, creating groups of six spiders in total. All six spiders originated from either the same colony or from two different colonies in a combination of three individuals per colony. Treatments consisted of: (i) sibs from the same colony (S; n = 19); (ii) non-sibs from two colonies from the same population, at most 12 m apart (NS1; n = 17); and (iii) non-sibs from two colonies from different populations, (NS2; n = 17). Varying the genetic composition of group members allowed us to create decreasing relatedness levels (S > NS1 > NS2).

Experiment 2: broods of mixed genetic composition

We collected 50 colonies consisting of late sub-adult spiders from two populations (Seeis and Otavi) in Namibia during January 2010. Colonies were brought to a field laboratory near Otavi and spiders were raised outdoors under natural conditions, sprayed with water and fed once a week with wild-caught insects (mainly moths, grasshoppers and crickets). Each colony was opened and the sexes separated; males were kept together whereas females were placed inside mesh boxes (12.5 × 12.5 × 10 cm) in groups of four consisting of one reproducing female and three helpers. The reproducing female mated sequentially with two males: a sib from the same colony, a non-sib from a colony of the same population approximately 100 m apart, or a non-sib from a different population approximately 500 km away. Males were presented in alternating order (i.e., one male at a time) and data from reciprocal mating orders were combined in the analysis. By mating females with a different combination of males we produced either ‘homogeneous’ or ‘heterogeneous’ broods. Homogeneous broods shared identical parental genotypes and were sired by a female mated with two of her sibs (HM1, n = 13), two non-sibs from different colonies of the same population (HM2, n = 8) or two non-sibs from different colonies of different population (HM3 = 4). Heterogeneous broods varied in their genetic composition due to different parental genotypes and were sired by a female mated with a sib male and a non-sib male from the same population (HT1, n = 36) and a sib and a non-sib from a different population (HT2, n = 28). This double-mating design allowed us to control for the number of matings while varying the genetic relatedness structure of the offspring as we can exclude fertilization biases36,37. In this way, we created broods of decreasing within-group genetic relatedness (HM1, HM2, HM3 > HT1, HT2). Females of this species do not readily remate, so obtaining double matings required multiple trials and females that rejected a mating partner were subsequently presented with a different male of the same relatedness level.

Females with egg sacs were transferred to Aarhus University (Denmark) and were kept in laboratory conditions at room temperature and natural photoperiod. Once the eggs hatched and the young had consumed two of the four adult females, each brood was divided into 5 to 7 groups consisting of five spiders with homogeneous body mass (as above).

Experiment 3: Individual participation

We collected 10 colonies consisting of sub-adult spiders from one population (Otavi) in Namibia during April 2010 and transported them to Aarhus University (Denmark). Colonies were maintained in the laboratory (as above), each inside a transparent plastic terrarium (24.5 × 15 × 15 cm). We marked spiders with non-toxic watercolour dots on the dorsal side of the opisthosoma using an identical colour for individuals from the same colony and different colours for the different colonies. We formed groups of 14–20 individuals to form (i) a control group consisting of siblings originating from the same colony (S, n = 12) and (ii) an experimental group consisting of non-sibs originating from two distant colonies combined in a 1:1 ratio (NS, n = 16).

Feeding Trials

Spiders were kept in transparent plastic boxes (10 × 5 × 5 cm, Experiments 1 and 2; 17 × 17 × 11 cm, Experiment 3) covered by mesh and containing two crossed wooden sticks to enable web building. Feeding trials started once spiders produced capture webs.

In Experiments 1 and 2 we weighed the prey consisting of either a fly (Musca domestica in Experiment 1, Calliphora sp. in Experiment 2), or a grasshopper (Locusta migratoria in Experiment 1) to the nearest 0.1 mg before introducing it on the capture web. Once the first spider attacked the prey, the number of individuals feeding (i.e. with chelicerae fixed on prey) was scored every 10 minutes during the entire duration of the feeding trial (60 and 120-minute trial, Experiment 1 and 2 respectively). We recorded the maximum feeding-group size (i.e. maximum number of spiders feeding cooperatively) and the duration of time spiders used to reach it. Once the feeding trial terminated, we removed and weighed the prey remains to calculate both the total amount of prey consumed and the amount per individual. If spiders did not attack the prey within 30 minutes the observation was discarded and the prey was left inside the box for spiders to feed on. For each group we repeated the trials every 10 and 4 days until we obtained 6 and 8 trials in total in Experiment 1 and 2, respectively. Spiders were weighed individually to the nearest 0.1 mg every 14 days to assess growth rate, the change in weight and the variance in weight between treatment groups, over time.

In Experiment 3, we conducted a single feeding trial. We introduced a fly on the web and scored the number of spiders feeding every 5 minutes during a 90-minute trial to record the maximum feeding-group size formation and the proportion of spiders from each colony engaged in communal feeding.

Statistical analysis

We analysed growth rates (mass was log transformed to meet assumptions of normality of residuals) in Experiments 1 and 2 using mixed model ANOVA, where the interaction between treatment and time tests the prediction that relatedness results in differential growth rates among groups33. Feeding efficiency (proportion of mass extracted from prey) was analysed using ANCOVA and where needed data were transformed to meet assumption of normality of residuals. The maximum proportion of spiders participating in feeding events was analysed with GLM using binomial errors. Latency to reach maximum group-size formation was analysed with GLM using exponential errors. In Experiment 3, to test for the effect of relatedness on individual participation in feeding, we compared the proportion of individuals originating from each of the two colonies that took part in feeding events using binomial tests. The time until the maximum group size gathered was tested using a Wilcoxon test. All statistical analyses were carried out with JMP 10 (SAS Software Inc. Cary, NC, USA).

To estimate the statistical power of our tests we carried out a power analysis on each of the three experiments using G*Power 3.1.9.2. For Experiment 1 and 2 power was calculated for mass increase of groups (growth rate) using the sample size and SD obtained in this study (Experiment 1: N = 46, SD = 4.5; Experiment 2: N = 92, SD = 1.9). The effect size used was estimated using the data in Schneider and Bilde (2008) which tested kin discrimination during foraging in the sub-social spider Stegodyphus lineatus using a comparable experimental set up. An effect size of 2.5 mg/group was adapted to a value of 3 mg/group and 2 mg/group for mass increased examined in Experiment 1 and Experiment 2, respectively. For experiment 3 power was calculated for proportion of spiders feeding using the sample size and SD we obtained in this study (n = 16, SD = 0.188). We estimated an effect size based on a single additional spider participating in a foraging event from either colony, corresponding to the smallest possible effect of kinship on foraging participation. We detected a power of 0.67, 0.89 and 0.65 respectively for Experiment 1, Experiment 2 and Experiment 3, at alpha = 0.05. While for each individual experiment there is a risk of 33%, 11% and 35% respectively of not detecting a significant effect (Type II error), we can combine the three power analyses because we have three independent experiments. This gives us a risk of only 1.27% (0.33*0.11*0.35) of a Type II error.

Additional Information

How to cite this article: Berger-Tal, R. et al. Evidence for loss of nepotism in the evolution of permanent sociality. Sci. Rep. 5, 13284; doi: 10.1038/srep13284 (2015).