Introduction

Dopamine neurons project extensively to striatal and cortical regions where they mediate a diverse array of functions, including reward-related learning, drug-related synaptic plasticity, working memory, and motivation [1,2,3,4,5,6]. Dopamine neurons respond with brief and phasic bursts of action potentials to salient events, such as reward-predictive stimuli and unexpected reward delivery [7, 8], and these bursts cause phasic dopamine release in terminal regions, including the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the nucleus accumbens (NAc) [9,10,11,12]. Influential theories suggest these phasic responses serve as teaching signals in reinforcement learning [8, 13, 14] and/or motivational signals that mediate cue-triggered seeking of rewards [4, 6].

While fast-scan cyclic voltammetry (FSCV) studies have characterized rapid and transient increases in extracellular dopamine concentration ([DA]o) in response to salient events in a behavioral task [15,16,17,18], consistent with phasic bursting of dopamine neurons, microdialysis studies have demonstrated sustained increases in cortical and striatal extracellular dopamine that persist for many minutes (even up to an hour) after the completion of appetitive learning, instrumental behavior, and other cognitive tasks [19,20,21,22,23]. The prolonged post-session dopamine increase occurs even though animals are no longer receiving rewards and/or being presented with salient stimuli. Importantly, dopamine antagonists administered either systemically or directly into PFC and striatum minutes after behavioral training can disrupt memory consolidation, demonstrating the necessity of post-training dopamine signaling [24,25,26]. This slower mode of dopamine signaling has been attributed to the so-called “tonic” dopamine release, which is hypothesized to be mediated by mechanisms that are entirely distinct from those that drive phasic activity of dopamine neurons and release [27,28,29]. But given that learning and most behavioral functions assigned to dopamine depend on both fast and slow modes of neurotransmission, we hypothesized that these phasic and tonic dopamine states might instead comprise a continuum of the same activation process in an awake and active animal. In that case, phasic activation of dopamine neurons could trigger secondary events that enhance the slow (tonic) activation of dopamine levels. Thus, we designed experiments to determine whether bursting of dopamine neurons results in post-burst sustained elevation of extracellular levels of dopamine in terminal regions and, if so, what mechanisms sustain this elevation.

Dopamine’s effects on reward-related learning, motivation, and cognitive constructs such as attention and working memory are primarily mediated by dopamine neurons in ventral tegmental area (VTA) and their projections to the NAc and PFC [2, 5, 30,31,32]. Thus, we assessed [DA]o in these two terminal regions before and after activation of VTA neurons. We found that electrical and optogenetic stimulation of VTA neurons, using patterns of activation that resemble the phasic bursting activity of dopamine neurons during reward-guided behavioral tasks, results in increased [DA]o in the NAc and PFC that persists for many minutes after stimulation is terminated. This sustained post-stimulus elevation of dopamine is blocked by tetrodotoxin (TTX), suggesting that it is not due to dopamine spillover from the initial stimulation; rather, it results from reduced dopamine transporter (DAT)-mediated clearance of dopamine released spontaneously during the post-stimulation period.

Materials and methods

Adult male Sprague-Dawley or Long-Evans rats were used. All experiments were approved by and conducted according to the guidelines and regulations of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of Pittsburgh and the NIH.

Briefly, for microdialysis experiments, animals were implanted with microdialysis probes in the PFC and/or NAc and bipolar-stimulating electrodes (electrical stimulation) or optical fibers (optogenetic stimulation) in the VTA. Microdialysis was conducted in freely moving animals, and dialysate samples were collected every 20 min. Drugs were either delivered intraperitoneally (i.p.) or locally into PFC and/or NAc via the microdialysis probes. VTA was electrically stimulated with a burst of pulses (pulse width = 1 ms, burst width = 200 ms, interburst interval (IBI) = 500 ms) for 20 min. For VTA optogenetic stimulation, blue laser (~473 nm) pulses were delivered for 20 min using one of two burst protocols: (a) 20 pulses (pulse width = 1 ms) at 100 Hz for 200 ms, IBI = 500 ms; and (b) 100 pulses (pulse width = 5 ms) at 20 Hz for 5 s, IBI = 10 s. In the biochemistry experiments, 1 mm midbrain slices were treated with drugs, and biotinylation for membrane-bound DAT and Rho activation assays were performed. Midbrain slices were used because of low signal-to-noise ratio, especially for detection of DAT, in the slices from terminal areas. See Supplementary Methods for details.

Results

Electrical stimulation of VTA produces sustained dopamine release in NAc and PFC

Microdialysis was used to measure [DA]o in NAc and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) of freely moving rats during the active phase of their sleep–wake cycle in a home-cage environment. Microdialysis has a limit of detection in the femtomolar range [33], which is 2–3 orders of magnitude better than the detection limit of FSCV [34]. The improved limit of detection afforded by microdialysis permits measurement of resting baseline [DA]o in behaving animals, including in regions with sparse dopamine innervation such as PFC [22]. We were, therefore, able to detect resting [DA]o in NAc and mPFC before stimulation of VTA neurons and follow [DA]o until its return to a stable pre-stimulation level.

VTA was electrically stimulated after stable [DA]o was measured in NAc and mPFC. The electrical stimulation protocol (100 Hz burst for 200 ms, IBI = 500 ms) mimicked the bursting firing pattern observed in VTA dopamine neurons when rats are engaged in relevant behaviors. For example, in response to reward during a reward-guided operant task, VTA dopamine neurons (identified based on response to apomorphine) fire on average 1.1 bursts per second (IBI = ~900 ms) with a typical intraburst frequency of ~30 Hz, but they can fire as fast as 100 Hz [35]. Hence, our protocol utilized interburst and intraburst frequencies that are within the range of reported frequencies. The stimulation duration was 5–20 min to model the typical duration of reward-driven behavioral paradigms [19, 22]. We found that electrical stimulation of VTA increased [DA]o in NAc and mPFC, and these elevations were sustained from several minutes to more than an hour post stimulation (Fig. 1 and S1, S2). VTA stimulation elicited a larger [DA]0 increase in PFC compared to NAc (brain region × sample interaction: F(9,135) = 5.48, p = 0.00) in the stimulation sample (post hoc independent t-test, p = 0.02). [DA]0 increases were similar in the post-stimulation samples (sample 5, p = 0.09; sample 6, p = 0.98; sample 7, p = 0.79; sample 8, p = 0.48; post hoc t-tests). While the difference between PFC and NAc during stimulation is consistent with previous reports of fewer DATs and reduced clearance of extracellular dopamine in PFC [36, 37], the present study focused on post-stimulation elevations of [DA]0 that were comparable across these regions.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Electrical stimulation of VTA produces sustained dopamine release in NAc and PFC. a Schematic illustrating simultaneous measurement of [DA]o in NAc via a microdialysis probe and electrical stimulation of VTA via a stimulation electrode. b VTA was stimulated using a protocol consisting of bursts of 1 ms pulses delivered at 100 Hz for 200 ms, with an interburst interval of 500 ms and an amplitude of 60 µA, for 20 min (n = 10 rats). VTA activation produced a sustained increase in [DA]o in NAc [F(15,135) = 6.13, p = 0.00]. Post hoc comparisons revealed a significant difference between baseline (sample 3) and sample 4 (p = 0.01), sample 5 (p = 0.01), sample 6 (p = 0.00), sample 7 (p = 0.01), and sample 8 (p = 0.03). The figure inset illustrates dopamine release following a second delivery of 100 Hz stimulation. The second stimulation also significantly increased [DA]o in NAc in samples 13 (p = 0.00) and 14 (p = 0.01). c VTA activation using a stimulus protocol consisting of bursts of 1 ms pulses delivered at 100 Hz for 200 ms, with an interburst interval of 500 ms and an amplitude of 60 µA for 5 min (n = 6 rats) was associated with a sustained increase in [DA]o in NAc [F(15,75) = 4.93, p = 0.00]. Post hoc comparisons revealed a significant difference between baseline (sample 3) and sample 4 (p = 0.03), sample 5 (p = 0.02), sample 6 (p = 0.00), sample 7 (p = 0.04), sample 8 (p = 0.02), and sample 10 (p = 0.01). The figure inset illustrates dopamine release following a second delivery of 100 Hz stimulation. The second stimulation resulted in an immediate elevation of [DA]o in NAc in sample 12 (p = 0.01) and sample 13 (p = 0.03). Data are represented as mean ± SEM. *p ≤ 0.05; **p ≤ 0.01. d Schematic illustrating simultaneous measurement of [DA]o in PFC via a microdialysis probe and electrical stimulation of VTA via a stimulation electrode. e VTA stimulation using a protocol consisting of bursts of 1 ms pulses delivered at 100 Hz for 200 ms, with an interburst interval of 500 ms and an amplitude of 60 µA, for 20 min (n = 7 rats) elevated [DA]o in PFC [F(15,90) = 8.10, p = 0.00]. Post hoc comparisons revealed a significant difference between baseline (sample 3) and sample 4 (p = 0.02), sample 5 (p = 0.01) and sample 6 (p = 0.04). The inset illustrates dopamine release following a second delivery of 100 Hz stimulation, which significantly increased [DA]o in PFC in sample 12 when compared to baseline (p = 0.04). f VTA activation using a stimulus protocol consisting of bursts of 1 ms pulses delivered at 100 Hz for 200 ms, with an interburst interval of 500 ms and an amplitude of 60 µA, for 5 min (n = 6 rats) was associated with sustained elevations in [DA]o in PFC [F(15,75) = 5.15, p = 0.00]. Post hoc comparisons against baseline indicated a significant increase in [DA]o in sample 4 (p = 0.00). The inset illustrates dopamine release following a second delivery of 100 Hz stimulation. Post hoc comparisons revealed that [DA]o in PFC in sample 12 was significantly increased compared to baseline (p = 0.02). Data are represented as mean ± SEM. *p ≤ 0.05; **p ≤ 0.01 (See also Fig. S1, S2)

A second stimulation given 140 min later produced a similar pattern of [DA]o increase (Fig. 1), suggesting that the initial stimulation had not provoked a pathological condition, such as sustained depolarization block of dopamine neurons [38]. In addition, the magnitude and duration of dopamine response in both regions were similar to previously reported microdialysis measures in animals performing a reward-guided reversal learning task [22]. These observations indicate that the pattern of stimulation used here is relevant to dopamine cell firing and dopamine release during reward-directed learning and cognitive behaviors. For subsequent studies, we focused on characterizing the impact of 20-min VTA stimulation on [DA]o in NAc because of higher dopamine signal in this region compared to PFC (mean baseline concentration in PFC = 0.2 ± 0.06 fmol; in NAc = 0.8 ± 0.12 fmol).

Optogenetic VTA dopamine stimulation produces sustained dopamine release in NAc

In addition to dopamine neurons, VTA contains a significant portion of GABA neurons [39], which could have been activated by VTA electrical stimulation. While it is unlikely that GABA neuron co-activation in the VTA produces terminal release of dopamine, we nonetheless compared dopamine responses in NAc after electrical stimulation of VTA neurons to dopamine responses after selective optogenetic stimulation of VTA dopamine neurons in tyrosine hydroxylase (Th)::Cre rats [40]. We have previously verified highly specific expression of channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) in VTA dopamine neurons of this transgenic line [41]. Similar to VTA electrical stimulation, optogenetic stimulation of VTA dopamine neurons elicited prolonged increases in [DA]o in NAc (Fig. 2 and S3). These [DA]o elevations persisted for a shorter duration (post hoc tests yielded significance for up to 20 min post stimulation) compared to [DA]o increases elicited by VTA electrical stimulation (Fig. 1b). This difference may have resulted from a smaller number of neurons being optogenetically stimulated, since only ~60% of dopamine neurons co-express ChR2 according to our previous quantification [41].

Fig. 2
figure 2

Optogenetic stimulation of VTA dopamine neurons produces sustained dopamine release in NAc. a Schematic illustrating simultaneous measurement of [DA]o via a microdialysis probe in NAc and blue laser illumination of ChR2-expressing VTA dopamine neurons via an optical fiber. b Immunofluorescence images from two representative Th::Cre rats illustrate expression of ChR2-eYFP (green) in TH+ (red) dopamine neurons in the VTA. Neurons that co-express ChR2 and TH appear yellow. Arrowheads point to optical fiber tips. Scale bar = 1 mm. c VTA dopamine neurons were stimulated using either a protocol consisting of bursts of 1 ms pulses delivered at 100 Hz for 200 ms, with an interburst interval of 500 ms, for 20 min or a protocol consisting of bursts of 5 ms pulses delivered at 20 Hz for 5 s, with an interburst interval of 10 s, for 20 min (laser power = 5–7 mW) (n = 11 sessions from five rats). Optogenetic VTA dopamine stimulation was associated with a sustained increase in [DA]o in NAc [F(9,90) = 4.47, p = 0.00]. Post hoc comparisons revealed a significant difference between baseline (sample 3) and sample 4 (p = 0.02) and sample 5 (p = 0.02). The dotted line represents electrical stimulation-induced elevations in NAc [DA]o (depicted also in Fig. 1b). Data are represented as mean ± SEM. *p ≤ 0.05 (See also Fig. S3)

Post-stimulation sustained [DA]o increase in NAc and PFC depends on active release

Next, we assessed whether the observed sustained elevation of post-stimulation [DA]o in mPFC and NAc resulted from continued active release of dopamine or simply reflected remaining dopamine that was “leftover” after the large burst-induced release. Previous studies have shown that stimulation of dopamine neurons in a burst pattern produces greater release of dopamine than tonic stimulation, even when the number of impulses per second and the total number of impulses are held constant [42, 43]. Thus, burst firing of dopamine neurons can simply produce a large surge of [DA]o that clears away slowly. We found that blockade of voltage-gated sodium channels via local application of TTX to either NAc or mPFC immediately after VTA stimulation drastically reduced [DA]o (Fig. 3 and S4). In fact, [DA]o in NAc after TTX application approached the lower limit of detection in post-stimulation samples (mean of sample 6: 20 ± 7% and sample 7: 16 ± 7%), whereas stimulation in the absence of TTX continued to produce significant elevations in [DA]o (mean of sample 6: 143 ± 8% and sample 7: 123 ± 7%). Similarly, in PFC, mean [DA]o after TTX treatment fell to 19 ± 5% of baseline by post-stimulation sample 6 while stimulation alone still elicited a significant [DA]o increase at that time point (mean: 143 ± 15%). While some of the reduction in post-TTX [DA]o can be explained by TTX’s effect on baseline [DA]o, the fact that NAc and PFC [DA]o dropped close to the detection limit suggests that TTX also prevented most of the sustained increase in [DA]o elicited by VTA stimulation. These results suggest that the sustained post-burst elevation of [DA]o is supported by active (impulse flow-dependent) exocytotic release of dopamine.

Fig. 3
figure 3

TTX blocks sustained dopamine release in NAc and PFC. a Schematic of infusion of TTX via microdialysis probes into NAc or PFC after electrical stimulation of VTA. b TTX infusion in NAc after cessation of VTA electrical stimulation blocked the post-stimulation increase in [DA]o in NAc. Solid line indicates data from sessions in which TTX infusion in NAc followed electrical stimulation of VTA (stim + TTX, n = 7 rats) while the dotted line represents data from rats that received electrical stimulation of VTA only (stim only, presented also in Fig. 1b). Stim only and stim + TTX data were analyzed after applying log transformations (see “Supplementary Methods”). Electrical stimulation of VTA alone produced sustained increases in [DA]o (results after data transformation were identical to the results in Fig. 1b; one-way repeated measures ANOVA: F(9,81) = 5.93, p = 0.00; post hoc tests against baseline: sample 4 (p = 0.01), sample 5 (p = 0.01), sample 6 (p = 0.00), sample 7 (p = 0.01), and sample 8 (p = 0.04)). TTX treatment modulated stimulation-induced increases in [DA]o in NAc (one-way repeated measures ANOVA, F(9,54) = 24.46, p = 0.00). Post hoc comparisons against baseline (sample 3) indicated that VTA stimulation before TTX infusion in NAc significantly increased NAc [DA]o in sample 4 (p = 0.04). Following TTX treatment, [DA]o decreased below baseline in sample 5 (p = 0.02), sample 6 (p = 0.01), sample 7 (p = 0.00), sample 8 (p = 0.00), sample 9 (p = 0.00), and sample 10 (p = 0.00). A two-way repeated measures ANOVA further demonstrated a significant interaction between sample bin and condition (stim + TTX and stim only) [F(9,135) = 33.77, p = 0.00]. c TTX infusion in PFC after cessation of VTA electrical stimulation blocked the post-stimulation increase in [DA]o in PFC. As in b, the solid line indicates stim + TTX condition (n = 6 rats) and the dotted line represents electrical stimulation of VTA only (stim only, presented also in Fig. 2b). TTX treatment after VTA electrical stimulation modulated sustained increases in [DA]o (one-way repeated measures ANOVA: F(9,45) = 45.35, p = 0.00). Post hoc comparisons against baseline (sample 3) indicated that while stimulation increased [DA]o in sample 4 (p = 0.00), [DA]o was no longer significantly different from baseline in sample 5 (p = 0.40) after TTX infusion in NAc. Further, [DA]o in PFC decreased below baseline in the remaining post-stimulation samples 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, and 10 (p = 0.00 for all comparisons). A two-way repeated measures ANOVA also demonstrated a significant interaction between sample bin and condition (stim + TTX and stim only) [F(9,99) = 6.79, p = 0.00]. *p ≤ 0.05 and **p ≤ 0.01 for comparison against baseline within the stim + TTX condition only. Data are represented as mean ± SEM (See also Fig. S4)

Blockade of cannabinoid and nicotinic receptor activity does not impact post-stimulation increase in NAc [DA]o

The outcome of the TTX experiment suggested that the burst-induced increase in [DA]o triggers secondary events that enhance active exocytotic dopamine release and/or reduce the clearance of dopamine actively released in the post-stimulation period. In the striatum, several secondary mechanisms that locally regulate release of dopamine have been identified [44, 45]. For example, an increase in synaptic dopamine release and subsequent dopamine receptor stimulation mobilizes the release of endocannabinoids [46] that could activate type-1 cannabinoid receptors (CB1Rs) on GABA or other neurons that may modulate dopamine release [45]. We found that inhibition of CB1R activity via systemic injection of rimonabant did not change spontaneous [DA]o or the pattern of stimulation-induced increase in [DA]o in NAc (Fig. S5).

Another potential mechanism is secondary activation of cholinergic neurons and subsequent stimulation of presynaptic nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (nAChRs) on dopamine axons, evoking dopamine release that is independent of dopamine somal activity [47, 48]. To test this, the effect of local inhibition of nAChRs by mecamylamine on NAc [DA]o was examined. Our results did not provide support for this mechanism (Fig. S6a,b). Mecamylamine treatment in combination with VTA stimulation also elicited a large variability in [DA]o in post-stimulation samples, which could be due to the impact of mecamylamine treatment itself on basal [DA]o (Fig. S6c,d).

Post-stimulation sustained increase in [DA]o results from reduced DAT-mediated clearance of dopamine

Next, we considered mechanisms that are involved in the clearance of [DA]o. Previously, it has been shown that amphetamine, which acts as a substrate of DAT and causes a large surge of [DA]o, induces transient internalization of DAT [49]. Although microscopy studies have not confirmed this internalization [50], other work indicates that the process is mediated, in part, by activation of Rho-GTPases, because selective inhibition of Rho, or stimulation of protein kinase A (PKA)-coupled receptors such as D1/5 dopamine receptors, prevents amphetamine-induced DAT internalization [51]. We hypothesized that a surge in synaptic dopamine release caused by burst activation of cell bodies also results in DAT internalization. Such a mechanism would then lead to an increase in [DA]o even after cessation of burst activation of dopamine neurons by reducing the rate of clearance of newly released dopamine. As FSCV studies have characterized transient surges in [DA]o in NAc in the range of 1–10 μM upon burst dopamine stimulation [10], we applied a comparable concentration of dopamine (10 μM) in rat midbrain slices to mimic burst-induced dopamine release in vivo. We found that 10 μM dopamine decreased cell surface expression of DATs in midbrain slices (Fig. 4a). The same concentration of dopamine increased the amount of activated Rho-GTPase (Fig. 4b). We then tested the effects of inactivating Rho on dopamine-mediated DAT internalization. To inactivate Rho, we stimulated D1/5 receptors in midbrain slices with SKF 38393. D1/5 receptors are G-protein-coupled receptors (GPCRs) present on dopamine neurons, and stimulation of these GPCRs increases adenylyl cyclase activity that activates PKA [52]. Activated PKA phosphorylates and thus inactivates Rho [53]. We found that SKF 38393 blocked dopamine’s effects on Rho activation and DAT internalization (Fig. 4a, b and S7).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Stimulation of D1/D5 receptor blocks dopamine-induced decrease in cell surface expression of DAT and increase in Rho activation in vitro as well as attenuates sustained dopamine release in NAc in vivo. a Rat midbrain slices were treated with dopamine (10 μM), SKF 38393 (100 nM), or dopamine and SKF 38393. Cell surface proteins were biotinylated, isolated, and probed for cell surface expression. The effect of dopamine on membrane-localized DAT depended on SKF treatment [F(1,8) = 13.72, p = 0.01, dopamine X SKF 38393 interaction, n = 3 slices for all groups]. Post hoc comparisons against control slices treated with vehicle revealed that membrane-localized DAT was significantly decreased in response to dopamine (p = 0.04), but this effect was blocked by SKF 38393 (p = 2.01). SKF 38393 treatment alone did not significantly increase DAT levels but resulted in a non-significant trend toward a decrease in membrane-localized DAT (p = 0.06). b Tissue lysates were then assessed for activated Rho. A two-way ANOVA revealed a significant interaction between dopamine and SKF 38393 treatments [F(1,12) = 10.00, p = 0.01] (n = 4 slices all groups). Post hoc comparisons against control slices treated with vehicle indicated that dopamine increased the amount of activated Rho-GTPase in midbrain slices (p = 0.01), and this effect was blocked by SKF 38393 (p = 0.75). SKF 38393 alone did not alter the amount of activated Rho-GTPase (p = 0.12). *p ≤ 0.05. The displayed blot images are cropped from full-length blots, which are included in Fig. S7. c In vivo treatment of SKF 38393 (10 µM) in NAc 10 min after the start of electrical stimulation of VTA attenuated post-stimulation increase in [DA]o in NAc. Filled triangles represent data from sessions with electrical stimulation of VTA only (stim only, n = 5 rats) and filled circles represent data from sessions with SKF 38393 infusion in NAc and electrical stimulation of VTA (stim + SKF 38393, n = 5 rats). For these sessions, VTA was stimulated using a protocol consisting of bursts of 1 ms pulses delivered at 100 Hz for 200 ms, with an interburst interval of 500 ms and an amplitude of 6 µA (see “Supplementary Methods”), for 20 min. Electrical stimulation of VTA increased [DA]o in NAc (one-way repeated measures ANOVA, F(6,24) = 3.53, p = 0.01). Post hoc comparisons against baseline (sample 2) demonstrated significant elevations of [DA]o in sample 3 (p = 0.05), sample 4 (p = 0.03), and sample 5 (p = 0.05). Electrical stimulation of VTA in combination with SKF 38393 treatment also resulted in a significant modulation of NAc [DA]o (one-way repeated measures ANOVA, F(6,24) = 6.29, p = 0.00). Post hoc comparisons against baseline (sample 2) demonstrated that even in the presence of SKF 38393 in NAc, electrical stimulation of VTA produced a significant elevation in NAc [DA]o in stimulation sample 3 (p = 0.04) and resulted in a trend toward a significant [DA]o increase in post-stimulation sample 4 (p = 0.08). However, SKF 38393 treatment prevented a sustained stimulation-induced [DA]o increase in sample 5 (p = 0.58) and resulted in a trend toward a decrease in [DA]o below baseline in sample 6 (p = 0.06). * and # indicate p ≤ 0.05 for comparisons against baseline within stim only and stim + SKF 38393 conditions, respectively. Further, a two-way repeated measures ANOVA confirmed that the local blockade of D1/5 receptors in NAc by SKF 38393 changed the pattern of [DA]o increase elicited by electrical stimulation of VTA (sample X condition: F(6,48) = 4.23, p = 0.00). Data are represented as mean ± SEM (See also Fig. S8, S9)

To establish the relevance of a Rho-mediated DAT internalization mechanism in freely moving animals, we examined the effect of systemic or intra-NAc application of SKF 38393 on [DA]o and found that both treatments attenuated sustained increases in [DA]o in post-stimulation samples (Fig. 4c and S8). These slice and in vivo findings, combined with the results from the TTX experiment, support the following mechanism for sustained post-stimulation elevation of [DA]0: burst activity of dopamine neurons promotes DAT internalization and thus decreases clearance of dopamine actively released in the post-burst period, leading to a large post-burst [DA]o even though dopamine neurons have returned to tonic mode of firing. Consistent with previous work [54, 55], SKF 38393 also decreased basal [DA]o in NAc (Fig. S9). This suggests that D1/D5 receptor activation may impact DAT internalization in both baseline and stimulation conditions in vivo. Importantly, however, there was no difference in [DA]o between SKF only and SKF+ stimulation conditions in post-stimulation samples.

Discussion

We found that phasic burst activity of VTA dopamine neurons in awake rats in a home-cage environment is followed by a sustained post-burst elevation of [DA]o in both NAc and PFC terminal regions that persisted for at least 20 min. This increase is not due to dopamine spillover from the initial stimulation but depends on continued active release of dopamine in terminal regions. We identified Rho-mediated internalization of DATs as a potential mechanism responsible for increased post-burst availability of actively released dopamine, based on evidence that experimental blockade of Rho activation prevented both DAT internalization and the sustained post-stimulation elevation of [DA]o. These results suggest that phasic surges of dopamine release produced by bursting of dopamine neurons result in DAT internalization that reduces the clearance of actively released dopamine in the post-burst period, causing a sustained post-burst elevation of tonic [DA]o.

Sustained post-burst increase in [DA]o is consistent with behavioral evidence and volume transmission

The sustained increase in [DA]o after periods of burst activity is functionally significant, because dopamine perisynaptic architecture in cortical and striatal regions is well suited to accommodate a slow and spatially distributed signal via volume transmission [45, 56]. A considerable density of dopamine receptors, both in the PFC and striatum, is localized extrasynaptically [57,58,59], and more than half of dopamine release sites in the striatum are reported to lack post-synaptic specializations [60]. Hence, dopamine that slowly accumulates in the extracellular space on the order of minutes to hours could modulate the activity of many pre- and post-synaptic sites within a large area in the striatum and the PFC via volume transmission [28]. This sustained and diffuse dopamine signal might maintain and stabilize/consolidate networks activated during learning and working memory [61]. In addition, persistent dopamine signaling could lower the threshold for long-term potentiation in all synapses for a certain period of time, allowing for adaptive modulation of memory encoding [62].

The mechanisms identified here might be especially critical for explaining dopamine’s role in memory consolidation during reinforcement-guided learning. Previous microdialysis studies in animals performing reward-driven learning or working memory tasks have demonstrated that [DA]o in PFC and striatal regions remain elevated for many minutes after completion of the training phase [21,22,23]. In fact, the duration and magnitude of the post-training increase in [DA]o in these studies were similar to what we observed in the present study after electrical or optogenetic burst stimulation of dopamine neurons. The importance of this post-training dopamine signaling has been recognized [24,25,26, 63], but the underlying mechanisms are unknown. Dopamine neurons fire phasically in response to salient stimuli during a behavioral task, but these phasic increases in activity return to baseline levels after hundreds of milliseconds [7, 8, 64,65,66,67]. This raises the question of how elevated dopamine levels can be sustained post training, when dopamine neurons are presumably firing at baseline levels. Our data obtained under electrical and optogenetic stimulation conditions suggest that repetitive stimuli-evoked bursting of dopamine neurons during behavioral training may produce changes in DAT internalization processes, resulting in elevated extracellular accumulation of spontaneously released dopamine post training. This finding also reconciles rapid dopaminergic transmission during learning with slow and sustained dopamine response after learning.

Prolonged dopamine responses have been implicated in sustained motivation [6, 68,69,70,71]. For example, sustained increases in [DA]o that continue after the end of behavior are observed in the striatum when rats lever press for food or lick a spout for sucrose solution, and these prolonged dopamine elevations are modulated by shifts in motivational state [20, 70, 72]. Furthermore, a computational model has indicated that a slow increase in striatal dopamine levels, which might carry an averaged reward signal during a behavioral task, predicts motivational state [73]. Hence, sustained increases in [DA]o, as observed in the current study, might be critical for plasticity mechanisms that include memory consolidation and adaptive learning as well as persistent functions such as working memory and motivation.

Proposed mechanism of sustained dopamine release following burst activation

While phasic and sustained (or tonic) modes of dopamine neurotransmission are often hypothesized to be driven by distinct mechanisms and to affect different post-synaptic processes and behaviors [27,28,29, 74], the present findings suggest that, in awake and active animals, these two modes of signaling may be complementary (Fig. 5). Ongoing activity of dopamine neurons in awake animals maintains impulse flow-dependent resting extracellular levels of dopamine in terminal regions (Fig. 5a) [75]. These levels are regulated by DAT, since the local application of DAT inhibitors profoundly enhances [DA]o [76, 77]. In response to salient events during behavioral tasks, dopamine neurons respond with brief bursts of spikes [7, 8, 64, 65, 67] that generate a large phasic surge of [DA]o [42, 43]. Our data indicate that large increases in [DA]o may cause DAT internalization via activation of Rho-GTPases. Similar to mechanisms described for amphetamine-mediated internalization of DATs and EAAT3 glutamate transporters on dopamine neurons [51, 78, 79], we propose that dopamine enters the presynaptic terminal via DATs to initiate a Rho-dependent intracellular signaling cascade (Fig. 5b). The mechanism via which intracellular dopamine activates Rho is unclear. Candidate mechanisms include influx of calcium and subsequent calmodulin/CamkII activation or activation of intracellular GPCRs [79]. DAT internalization has important consequences for the period following burst stimulation, which would be analogous to the immediate post-task period in learning and cognitive tasks. In the post-task period, dopamine neurons return to their spontaneously firing activation level, which generates a low level of synaptic dopamine. However, because of DAT internalization, dopamine is cleared at a much slower rate, leading to a sustained accumulation of dopamine in the extracellular space in terminal regions (Fig. 5c).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Mechanism for sustained increase in extracellular dopamine following burst activation of dopamine neurons. a (1) Baseline spontaneous firing of dopamine neurons causes the release of low levels of dopamine from the terminals. (2) Dopamine diffuses into the extrasynaptic space where it is taken up by DATs, resulting in a low level of extracellular dopamine. b (1) Dopamine neurons fire in phasic bursts in response to behaviorally salient stimuli. Phasic bursting results in increased release and higher synaptic levels of dopamine. (2) A high concentration of extracellular dopamine enters the cell via DATs. (3) Intracellular dopamine activates Rho. (4) Rho activation mediates internalization of DATs. c (1) Upon cessation of phasic bursting, dopamine neurons return to baseline firing and release low levels of dopamine into the synapse. (2) Synaptic dopamine diffuses into the extrasynaptic space, where very few DATs are present following DAT internalization. As a result, extrasynaptic dopamine is cleared away slowly and dopamine accumulates in the extracellular space

Role of D1/5 receptor activation in dopamine-induced DAT internalization

While multiple mechanisms may activate PKA and block Rho-mediated DAT internalization, our experimental approach was driven by data showing that D1/5 receptors are physiologically relevant in dopamine-related behaviors. For example, mice lacking D5 receptors display a greater locomotor response to methamphetamine, a dopamine-activating stimulant, compared to wild-type mice [80], suggesting that D5 receptor activation might serve to curb excessive dopamine release and accumulation. Hence, we speculate that when there is a burst-induced release of a large amount of dopamine, activation of D1/5 receptors by extracellular dopamine counteracts, to some extent, Rho activation and DAT internalization mediated by dopamine transported into the cell. In other words, in dopamine terminals at baseline conditions, there might be a balance between processes that promote and interrupt DAT internalization. We speculate that the former could be mediated by intracellular dopamine that activates Rho and/or synaptic dopamine that activates D2 receptors and potentially inhibits PKA activity [52, 81] whereas the latter by extracellular dopamine that activates presynaptic D1/5 receptors. When a large surge of dopamine occurs during burst activation of dopamine neurons, the balance might be shifted toward Rho-mediated DAT internalization. Accordingly, when most D1/5 receptors are stimulated by SKF 38393, activation of D1/5-coupled PKA signaling cascades could outcompete ongoing Rho activation. Consistent with this idea, our data indicate that SKF 38393 not only prevents burst-induced increases in [DA]o but also decreases baseline [DA]o.

Additional considerations

The DAT-mediated clearance mechanism we have identified here is likely only one of the mechanisms that contribute to the sustained post-stimulation activation of [DA]o. Other mechanisms may include activation of feedback loops, such as stimulation of NAc medium spiny neurons that inhibit GABA neurons in VTA, resulting in the disinhibition of VTA dopamine neurons [82], and/or local signaling via non-dopamine receptors, including glutamate, GABA, and nAChRs [44, 45, 47, 48]. Long-range interactions with the ventral subiculum of the hippocampus can also elicit sustained dopamine release in NAc [83]. Likewise, the sustained elevations of [DA]o observed upon stimulation of the subiculum may be mediated, in part, by the proposed DAT internalization mechanism.

While most experiments in the current study focused on NAc, the proposed Rho-mediated DAT internalization is relevant for sustaining [DA]o elevations in PFC. Although there are fewer DATs in PFC and norepinephrine transporters (NETs) play a significant role in dopamine uptake in this region [84], DAT inhibition significantly reduces [DA]o in PFC [85]. We do not, however, discount the potential involvement of NETs in the observed sustained increase in post-stimulation [DA]o.

Although local and systemic application of SKF 38393 elicited similar reduction in post-stimulation [DA]o, thus reducing the possibility of the observed effects being mediated by D1/5 receptors at VTA cell bodies, future studies should examine whether local stimulation of D1/5 receptors in terminal regions can indirectly modulate the firing of VTA neurons via long-range loops or can affect dopamine terminal activity through intra-striatal loops mediated by post-synaptic receptors. Finally, future studies should also determine whether burst firing of dopamine neurons at lower frequencies (15–60 Hz) could result in the observed post-burst-sustained dopamine release via the proposed mechanism.

Conclusion

The present findings indicate that a critical function of burst activity of dopamine neurons may be to produce a persistent elevation of [DA]o in terminal regions via an intracellular mechanism that promotes DAT internalization. The sustained post-burst increase in dopamine levels may be essential for consolidation of memories during associative learning, maintenance of active networks in working memory, and sustenance of motivational states. Hence, identification of a mechanism that supports this persistent increase in [DA]o has important implications for understanding learning and memory processes in the brain and may provide insights into dysfunction of dopamine systems in disorders such as ADHD, addiction, and schizophrenia.