INTRODUCTION

5-HT1A Receptor Function in Impulsive Aggression

Considerable evidence has accumulated showing that impulsive and aggressive behaviors across psychiatric diagnoses are associated with impaired central serotonergic function (reviewed in Lee and Coccaro, 2001). The 5-HT1A receptor in particular has been implicated in rodent models, as administration of selective 5-HT1A receptor agonists (such as the azapirone ipsapirone) leads to decreased aggressive behavior in several rodent models of offensive aggression (Bell and Hobson, 1994; Olivier et al, 1995). This evidence suggests that individuals with impulsive aggressive behavior may exhibit functional impairment of central 5-HT1A receptors. The functional status of central 5-HT1A receptors can be assessed in humans in vivo by means of elevated plasma cortisol and hypothermic responses to exogenous 5-HT1A receptor agonists. The first effect is thought to be due to postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptor activation on hypothalamic cells that release corticotrophin releasing factor (Fuller, 1992). The second effect may be due to activation of either postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors and/or cell-body 5-HT1A autoreceptors that inhibit serotonergic neuronal action potential generation (Cowen et al, 1990). In addition, the azapirones also stimulate increases in plasma prolactin, an effect mediated by either post-synaptic 5-HT1A receptor activation or dopamine-2 (D2) receptor antagonism (Meltzer et al, 1991). Elevations in circulating cortisol and (more variably) prolactin can be reliably demonstrated in healthy human subjects after oral administration of ipsapirone (Lesch et al, 1989, 1990; Kahn et al, 1994; Cleare et al, 1998).

Using this paradigm, self-reported aggressiveness (measured with the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory, BDHI) has been associated with impaired cortisol response to ipsapirone in both healthy subjects (Cleare and Bond, 2000) and personality disordered individuals (Coccaro et al, 1995). In addition, self-reported aggressiveness has been associated with blunted prolactin response to buspirone (Coccaro et al, 1990) and ipsapirone (Cleare and Bond, 2000), disinhibition has been associated with blunted prolactin response to ipsapirone (Netter et al, 1996), and borderline personality disorder (BPD) has been associated with blunted prolactin response to flesinoxan (an agent like the azapirones, with 5-HT1A agonist and D2 antagonist activity) (Hansenne et al, 2002). However, others have found increased cortisol responses to ipsapirone, in association with aggression (Netter et al, 1999) and active mania (Yatham et al, 1999), and to meta-chlorophenylpiperazine (m-CPP, an agent with multiple serotonergic actions (Gatch, 2003)), in association with BPD (Hollander et al, 1994). A few studies have found no association of cortisol response to ipsapirone in relation to either trait anger (Klaasen et al, 2002), disinhibition (Netter et al, 1996), or levels of aggressive responding on a laboratory test of interpersonal antagonism (Moeller et al, 1998), although the first two of these studies may have used suboptimal doses of ipsapirone (Kahn et al, 1994). Overall, this literature suggests that various syndromes of impulsivity and aggressive behavior involve alterations in the function of 5-HT1A and possibly D2 receptors as well. In some of these disorders, it is also possible that other changes (eg, compensatory upregulation) may arise partly in response to deficits elsewhere in the serotonin system.

The Present Study

In the present study, we tested the hypothesis that impulsive aggression in personality disorder patients is associated with impaired function of central 5-HT1A receptors. Among personality disorders as a group of diagnoses, some, such as BPD, are characterized by hallmark clinical features of impulsive aggression. Overall, there is a significant range of severity of these symptoms in patients with personality disorders, permitting an adequate test of the relationship of these symptoms to underlying biological measures, such as 5-HT1A receptor function. The relative association of impulsivity and hostile/antagonistic behavior was evaluated in order to distinguish between related clinical features that are often confounded in measures of impulsive aggression. We also evaluated the relationship of personality disorder and comorbid Axis I diagnoses with altered 5-HT1A receptor function, with post hoc tests where appropriate to consider whether these relationships are mediated by trait measures of impulsive aggression. Childhood maltreatment history was also evaluated for association with cortisol response to ipsapirone, given the high rates of reported childhood maltreatment in personality disorder samples (Battle et al, 2004) and the association of maltreatment with HPA axis disturbances (Heim and Nemeroff, 2001). Dimensional and categorical (diagnostic) measures of depression and anxiety were also assessed, because of the association of these symptoms with altered 5-HT1A receptor function (Lesch, 1991; Meltzer and Maes, 1995; Drevets et al, 1999), and their frequent appearance in personality disorder patients. We chose cortisol as an outcome measure rather than ACTH (although ACTH is more directly related to hypothalamic 5-HT1A receptor activation) in order to resolve the results of this study with the foregoing literature on aggression and impulsivity, and because we have found previously that cortisol elevations are more reliably demonstrated than ACTH after ipsapirone administration to healthy subjects (Kahn et al, 1994).

SUBJECTS AND METHODS

Subjects

Individuals with DSM-defined personality disorders (n=52) were recruited primarily from the community but also by referral from clinician colleagues in the greater New York City region. This group had a mean age of 37.3±9.4 years and was 69% male. Inclusion criteria included ages >18 and <60, no significant medical illness, including no history of neurological illness or head injury. All subjects were within 20% of their ideal body weight (according to a standard health information chart published by Signa Health). All female subjects were studied in the first 10 days of their menstrual cycle, and none were currently taking oral contraceptive medication. A single female subject was postmenopausal at study; all results remained unchanged upon repeat analyses with this subject excluded (data not shown). Personality disorder subjects were excluded if they had lifetime co-morbid diagnoses of a primary psychotic disorder, bipolar affective disorder type I, or substance dependence. No subjects met substance abuse criteria within 6 months of study. Over 80% of this sample had never been treated with psychiatric medication. All subjects who were in current treatment with psychiatric medication at study entry had a minimum 2-week (or five half-lives) medication wash-out period prior to participation in the experimental protocol. Subjects were also queried about recent alcohol use and excluded from participation if they endorsed use just prior to the study procedure. All subjects underwent diagnostic assessment with the Structured Interview for DSM-III-R Personality Disorders (SIDP-R) (Pfohl et al, 1989). A total of 26 subjects met criteria for one or more cluster A personality disorders, 19 for cluster B disorders other than BPD, and 51 for cluster C disorders; the mean number of personality disorder diagnoses was 3.2±1.7. Axis I disorders were evaluated using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I (SCID-I) (First et al, 1995). In addition, a module was employed for intermittent explosive disorder-revised (Coccaro et al, 1998). Consensus diagnoses were arrived at with the assistance of an expert diagnostician (JS). Frequencies of BPD and Axis I diagnoses are indicated in the Statistical Analysis section. Clinical traits of Impulsivity, Hostility, Depression, and Anxiety were assessed with the Barratt Impulsivity Scale—11th version (Patton et al, 1995), Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory (BDHI) (Buss and Durkee, 1957), Hamilton Depression Scale (Hamilton, 1960), and Spielberger State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (Spielberger et al, 1983), respectively. Childhood maltreatment history was assessed with the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ, 25-item version) (Bernstein and Fink, 1998). Informed consent was obtained prior to all study procedures, subject confidentiality was maintained throughout, and procedures were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the Mount Sinai IRB.

Ipsapirone Probe

Subjects were admitted in a fasting condition to the Mount Sinai General Clinical Research Center at 0800, and maintained on NPO status from midnight of the day prior to the study, through the completion of the procedure. The room temperature was maintained between 66 and 75°F. Following placement of an intravenous line, kept patent by a slow infusion of normal saline, the subjects were placed on bedrest in a quiet room with the only activity being the entry and procedural activity of the investigator. At 1000, participants were administered a 20 mg dose of ipsapirone orally. Blood samples and vital signs were collected at 15 min intervals from minus 60 min to plus 210 min following administration of ipsapirone. Samples were stored in a −80°C freezer until their analysis for cortisol and prolactin via standard radioimmunoassay technique.

The intra- and interassay coefficients of variation for assay of these hormones are 6 and 10% respectively for cortisol, and 1.8 and 3.9% respectively for prolactin (Koenigsberg et al, 2004). Our dependent measures were maximum delta-peak for both cortisol and prolactin (Delta-CRT and Delta-PRL), that is, peak hormone level following ipsapirone minus baseline. Area under the curve was also calculated for hormone levels following ipsapirone and results were similar to delta-peak hormone responses (data not shown). Temperature response to ipsapirone was computed as the maximal change in temperature (baseline minus nadir, °F) following ipsapirone administration.

Statistical Analysis

We first assessed the effects of age, sex, and body weight on the outcome measures of temperature and hormone levels, each at baseline and in response to ipsapirone, in order to address potential confounding factors. We then conducted a series of stepwise multiple regression analyses to evaluate the association of both dimensional (trait) and categorical (diagnostic) measures as independent variables, each first with baseline measures and then with responses to ipsapirone. We have chosen the present analytic approach for several reasons: (1) with repeated-measures ANOVA, the variance is typically misestimated due to the variable correlations between repeated measures typically found in physiology research (Kristensen and Hansen, 2004); (2) change scores are also commonly utilized in longitudinal treatment response designs in psychiatry (Ekstrom et al, 1990), and, as summary measures, have an advantage of ease of interpretability (Hennen, 2003); and (3) stepwise regression allows for an ‘omnibus’ test of association of a number of predictor variables that are hypothetically related to the dependent variable, which we are compelled to consider given the extensive diagnostic comorbidity inherent in this personality disorder sample. This approach also allows for the simultaneous evaluation of diagnostic and trait/symptom measures, and estimates of the relative portion of the variance accounted for in the dependent variables by these clinical measures. We therefore believe that this approach is reasonably valid, conservative, and comprehensive with the present data set. Trait measures included Impulsivity (mean 38.7±13.0), Hostility (mean 35.3±14.3), Depression (mean 15.0±7.8), Trait Anxiety (mean 50.5±13.2), and State Anxiety (mean 42.8±13.0). Childhood maltreatment (CTQ total score, mean 49.8±17.6) was also considered in this analysis. In the stepwise regression analysis of diagnoses, current DSM-III-R diagnoses of BPD (n=21), IED-R (n=21), major depressive disorder (n=25), dysthymic disorder (n=10), depression not otherwise specified (n=1), post-traumatic stress disorder (n=6), obsessive-compulsive disorder (n=6), panic disorder (n=6), and generalized anxiety disorder (n=12) were evaluated, although the full range of Axis I disorders were addressed in the diagnostic evaluation (15 subjects were comorbid for BPD and IED-R). Total numbers of diagnoses per subject of personality disorders (mean 3.2±1.7), mood disorders (mean 0.7±0.6), and anxiety disorders (mean 0.6±0.8) were also considered in this analysis. Where demographic variables were associated with outcome measures, or where diagnostic or trait variables were associated with baseline measures, the demographic variables or baseline measures were then entered as independent variables to the regression analyses of ipsapirone response measures. This strategy was used to control for possible confounding effects of either demographic differences or varying baseline measures on the ipsapirone response measures. All independent variables were entered in a forward manner into their respective regression models, with probabilities to enter and remove variables set at p=0.05 and 0.10, respectively, for all analyses.

RESULTS

Cortisol

The mean baseline cortisol level was 12.1±4.8 μg/dl, and mean peak cortisol response to ipsapirone was 13.5±8.0μg/dl. Age was associated with neither baseline cortisol (Pearson's r=−0.03) nor cortisol response to ipsapirone (r=−0.02). Body weight was associated with neither baseline cortisol (r=−0.03, p=0.9) nor cortisol response to ipsapirone (r=−0.07, p=0.6). In addition, female subjects were not significantly different from male subjects on either baseline cortisol (12.7±4.5 vs 11.8±5.0 μg/dl; t=0.61, df=50, p=0.55) or cortisol response to ipsapirone (15.6±8.4 vs 12.5±7.7 μg/dl; t=1.29, df=50, p=0.20).

In stepwise regression analysis, neither dimensional nor diagnostic measures were associated with baseline cortisol levels. Subsequently, in the stepwise regression analysis of dimensional variables, Impulsivity was the single significant predictor of decreased cortisol response to ipsapirone (adjusted R2=0.072; t=−2.22, p=0.031). Dimensional Hostility, Depression and Anxiety symptoms, and Childhood Maltreatment were not associated with significant effects on cortisol response. Stepwise regression analysis of diagnostic variables revealed two significant diagnostic predictors of cortisol response to ipsapirone (total adjusted R2=0.176): BPD (t=2.71, p=0.009) and IED-R (t=−3.41, p=0.001). The mean cortisol response for BPD vs nonBPD subjects was 14.8±7.5 μg/dl and 12.6±8.3 μg/dl, respectively; and for IED-R vs nonIED-R subjects were 10.6±7.2 and 15.4±8.1 μg/dl, respectively. The results of the regression analyses indicate that BPD was associated with significantly increased cortisol response whereas IED-R was associated with significantly decreased cortisol response to ipsapirone. However, cortisol response to ipsapirone was negatively correlated with impulsivity in the BPD group (Pearson's r=−0.57, p=0.021) (Figure 1). No mood or anxiety disorder diagnoses were associated with significant effects on cortisol response, nor were the total number of personality disorder, mood disorder or anxiety disorder diagnoses per subject. We also conducted a post hoc stepwise regression analysis to evaluate the relative association of those diagnostic and dimensional variables that were found to be significant predictors of cortisol response in the regression analyses just described. BPD, IED-R, and Impulsivity scores were entered to the regression model in the same forward manner as with other analyses. This analysis revealed a single significant predictor, Impulsivity (adjusted R2=0.072; t=−2.22, p=0.031). BPD and IED-R effects in this regression model were reduced to nonsignificance (t=1.52, p=0.134, and t=−1.55, p=0.127, respectively). These findings appear to indicate that there was a negative relationship between Impulsivity and cortisol response to ipsapirone in the BPD group, although the BPD group on average showed cortisol responses that were shifted upward relative to the rest of the personality disorder sample (Figure 1). These findings also suggest that the effects of BPD and IED diagnoses on cortisol response to ipsapirone are largely mediated by dimensional impulsivity.

Figure 1
figure 1

Relationship of impulsivity to peak plasma cortisol response to ipsapirone in personality disorder subjects.

Prolactin

The mean baseline prolactin level was 7.7±3.6 ng/ml, and mean prolactin response to ipsapirone was 3.6±7.0 ng/ml. Age was inversely related to baseline prolactin (r=−0.37, p=0.008) but not prolactin response to ipsapirone (r=0.02). Body weight was associated with neither baseline prolactin (r=−0.24, p=0.09) nor prolactin response to ipsapirone (r=0.17 p=0.23). Female subjects had significantly higher baseline prolactin levels than males (10.8±4.0 vs 6.3±2.4 ng/ml) (t=5.00, df=50, p<0.0005) but prolactin levels in response to ipsapirone that were not significantly different from males (1.6±5.4 vs 4.5±7.5 ng/ml) (t=1.41, df=50, p=0.17). In stepwise regression analyses, symptom measures were not associated with baseline prolactin; however, Impulsivity was significantly associated with blunted prolactin response to ipsapirone (adjusted R2=0.10; t=−2.58, p=0.013). Age and sex did not make significant additional contributions to prolactin response to ipsapirone with Impulsivity entered to the regression model. No diagnoses were significantly associated with prolactin either at baseline or in response to ipsapirone.

Temperature

Age was inversely related to baseline temperature (r=−0.33, p=0.023) but was not related to temperature response to ipsapirone (r=−0.09, p=0.5). Female subjects had a significantly higher baseline temperature than male subjects (98.0±0.5°F vs 97.4±0.6°F) (t=3.76, df=49, p<0.0005) but a temperature response to ipsapirone that was not significantly different from male subjects (0.89±0.76°F vs 0.66±0.78°F) (t=−0.98, df=49, p=0.33). In regression analyses, only BPD among the diagnostic and symptom measures was associated with baseline temperature (adjusted R2=0.12; t=3.21, p=0.002). However, with baseline temperature entered to the stepwise regression analysis of temperature response to ipsapirone, neither BPD nor other diagnoses nor symptom measures were associated with temperature response to ipsapirone.

DISCUSSION

In this study, we evaluated the association of trait and diagnostic measures of impulsive aggression with impaired central 5-HT1A receptor function, measured by plasma cortisol, prolactin, and hypothermic responses to ipsapirone, in an outpatient sample of 52 individuals with personality disorder diagnoses. We found that trait impulsivity is associated with decreased postsynaptic 5-HT1A (and possibly D2) receptor responsivity to ipsapirone, and may mediate the association of BPD and IED-R with altered 5-HT1A receptor function. Categorical and dimensional measures of depression and anxiety were not responsible for these associations, nor was the severity of childhood maltreatment history. In addition, no evidence was found for altered responsivity of the cell-body 5-HT1A autoreceptor.

These findings are consistent with the evidence from animal models and clinical serotonergic challenge studies in humans, which indicate that impulsive and aggressive behavior is associated with impaired central 5-HT1A receptor function. The present experimental paradigm assesses postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors in the hypothalamus, a brain region that is associated with social and emotional behavior, including aggressive behavior. However, these findings may additionally be consistent with a more general postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptor mediation of the blunted serotonergic responses observed in brain imaging experiments in frontal cortical regions that are also responsible for emotional–behavioral integration, such as the anterior cingulate and ventral prefrontal cortex (Siever et al, 1999; Soloff et al, 2000; New et al, 2002). We also found that hostility per se is not significantly associated with altered 5-HT1A receptor function, when the effects of impulsivity are accounted for in the regression model. Antagonistic thoughts and emotions are confounded with behavioral dyscontrol to some extent in measures such as the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory, and therefore we used a separate measure of impulsivity (Barratt Impulsivity Scale) to contrast the negative moods from the behavioral dyscontrol, both of which are exhibited in the syndrome of impulsive aggression. The results suggest that impulsivity is a specific consequence of impaired 5-HT1A receptor function, and that hostility may be related to disturbances at other sites within the central serotonin system, or in other monoamine systems that interact with serotonin such as the catecholamines.

Interestingly, the BPD diagnosis was associated with significantly greater cortisol response to ipsapirone, relative to subjects with other personality disorder diagnoses. A similar finding among BPD patients was reported in a study of cortisol response to m-CPP (Hollander et al, 1994). It is certainly possible that BPD patients exhibit relatively increased activity in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis unrelated to the hypothalamic 5-HT1A receptor, which would be consistent with the findings of a recent study reporting higher diurnal salivary cortisol in female BPD patients without major depression, relative to healthy controls (Lieb et al, 2004). This could result in enhanced cortisol levels in response to similar levels of hypothalamic 5-HT1A receptor activation. Nevertheless, the negative correlation between impulsivity and cortisol response to ipsapirone in the BPD group indicates that this relationship is consistent for the BPD subjects as it is for the sample as a whole. The upward shift of cortisol response observed in the BPD group overall, relative to the other personality disorder subjects, may additionally suggest that BPD patients exhibit a relatively greater capacity for compensatory upregulation of postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptor function. Nevertheless, we found that trait impulsivity was more closely related to cortisol response than diagnostic status. It was also the only measure associated with blunted prolactin response to ipsapirone, which may reflect altered function at the 5-HT1A receptor and/or the D2 receptor in the hypothalamus. Taken together, these results suggest that, in a sample of individuals characterized by a categorical psychiatric diagnosis such as BPD or IED-R, exaggerated degrees of traits such as impulsivity may be the more important (or more direct) correlate of altered central serotonergic dysfunction (Minzenberg and Siever, 2005). The lack of diagnostic or symptom associations with temperature response to ipsapirone suggests that these disorders may exhibit 5-HT1A receptor dysfunction extrinsic to serotonergic neurons themselves, as we have not found evidence for cell-body 5-HT1A autoreceptor dysfunction in these individuals.

The functional status of central 5-HT1A receptors has not been well studied in other impulse control disorders to date. Preliminary evidence suggests that 5-HT1A receptor gene variation contributes a small but significant amount of variance to symptom scores for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (Comings et al, 2000); a variant in the 5-HT1A receptor (due to a missense nucleotide change) has been isolated in a patient with Tourette's syndrome (Lam et al, 1996); and selective 5-HT1A receptor agonists can reduce alcohol intake in dependent animals (De Vry, 1995). However, other investigators have found no association of the 5-HT1A receptor gene with Tourette's syndrome (Brett et al, 1995), and increased cortisol response to ipsapirone in active mania (Yatham et al, 1999). The general role of central 5-HT1A-receptor dysfunction across impulse control disorders remains to be fully investigated. On the basis of the present evidence, we would suggest that 5-HT1A-receptor dysfunction may be a contributing factor to impulsivity across psychiatric diagnoses.

Study Limitations

We did not evaluate the basal circadian rhythms of cortisol, prolactin, or body temperature in this study. There is evidence that depression is associated with phase advances in circadian and ultradian rhythms of circulating cortisol and prolactin levels (Koenigsberg et al, 2004); this phenomenon, to our knowledge, has not been studied in the disorders presently under study. We therefore cannot rule out that observed differences in circulating hormone levels measured after ipsapirone administration are due to differences in underlying rhythmicity of basal hormone levels rather than altered responses to ipsapirone. In addition, in this study (as with many in this literature), we have measured cortisol but not adrenocorticotropin (ACTH), which is a more direct measure of hypothalamic 5-HT1A receptor regulation of the HPA axis. Therefore, we cannot be certain that the present findings do not reflect hyporesponsivity in the adrenal cortex rather than at the hypothalamic 5-HT1A receptor. Future investigations should ideally assess the underlying rhythmicity of circulating hormone levels, and the relative responses of ACTH and cortisol to 5-HT1A receptor stimulation.

Conclusion

Dysfunction of the central 5-HT1A receptor is associated with impulsive aggression in animal models and clinical studies. The present findings suggest that impulsivity may be particularly related to impaired postsynaptic 5-HT1A (and possibly D2) receptor function. The trait impulsivity that is characteristic of BPD and IED may be a more significant correlate of altered 5-HT1A receptor dysfunction than are the categorical diagnoses themselves. Future studies should take advantage of serotonergic agents with enhanced pharmacological selectivity in order to more precisely specify the dysfunction of central serotonin and the relationship of these disturbances to various syndromes of mood disturbance and behavioral dyscontrol.