Introduction

Microsaccades are the involuntary, fast and very small eye movements that occur during visual fixation. Over the past decade, the behavioral properties and functional role of microsaccades have been widely investigated1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13. It has been found that microsaccades play an important functional role in counteracting visual fading. In order to study neural dynamical mechanism of microsaccades for counteracting perceptual fading, neural responses correlated with microsaccades have been extensively studied in experiments at different levels—from neuronal activities7,11,14,15 to electroencephalogram (EEG)9,16 and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)5,12—in a number of cortical areas involved in visual information processing, including V15,7,12, V25,12,14, V312, V414 and MT12,15.

Recently, a report gave an explanation for microsaccades in counteracting visual fading by constructing a feedforward network model with short-term depression (STD) at thalamocortical synapses17, alternative to the assumption of retinal adaptation. The adapted synapses subjected to STD led to response depression in V1, which induces visual fading because of sustained depression during fixation. Therefore, it is possible that the generation of microsaccades serves to counteract the STD-induced depression of neuronal activity in order to counteract visual fading. In particular, the depressed model successfully reproduced several microsaccade-related experimental observations. For example, the neural response after microsaccade is stronger when a rhythmically flashing stimulus bar is on during fixation, as compared to a condition in which the bar is always on (stationary, i.e., non-flashing). The response peak induced by microsaccade increases with the increasing of microsaccadic magnitude or velocity. Moreover, the increasing response reaches saturation for large microsaccadic magnitude or velocity.

It was found by model simulations, that the above results are attributed to the sensitivity of STD to the change of stimuli17. These findings strongly suggest that the depression model is very useful to investigate microsaccade-related neural responses. Indeed, computational studies have explored the effect of STD on network dynamics and found various rich dynamical behaviors18,19,20,21, suggesting many important roles of STD in neural computations. Thus, the depression model can be expected to produce other abundant features in microsaccade-related neural responses. Model predictions of such features would provide theoretical guidance for experimental investigations. In this paper, by using the model, we extensively study the features of microsaccade-related neural responses with respect to several key parameters, which could be experimentally tested in future.

Methods

According to visual pathway22,23, a feedforward network model consisting of two layers corresponding to LGN and V1 with STD at thalamocortical synapses was proposed in Fig. 117. Evoked by fixated dot, the LGN neuron j fires with rate Rj following Gaussian tuning curve G1 (shown in Fig. 1). Then, the firings Rj are straightly projected to V1 neuron i by STD synapses with linking weights Wij following Gaussian tuning curve G2 (shown in Fig. 1). The membrane potential Vi of V1 neuron i in output layer is described by

Figure 1
figure 1

(Adapted from Ref. 17) The feedforward network model including STD during fixation with microsaccade.

Here, neurons in LGN and V1 are labeled and arranged by the center positions xj and of their receptive fields in the ranges from −L to L, respectively. Gaussian filters (receptive fields) in LGN layer transform the afferent stimuli evoked by fixated dot into the inputs with Gaussian firing rate profile: . The A represents the amplitude of a visual input at fixated-dot position . The σ1 is width of the tuning curve. The output layer V1 is connected to input layer LGN by thalamocortical synapses with synaptic strengths Sj, which are subjected to the modification: STD. These connecting weights Wij follow the Gaussian tuning curve: , where denotes denotes the position difference of receptive field centers between the input neuron j and the output neuron i. The microsaccade during fixation can be regarded as relative movement of the fixated dot over LGN with microsaccadic magnitude ΔM. In order to eliminate the effect of boundary owing to the limited scale of network, the corresponding input tuning curve G1 is extended to a period function with period 2L.

Here, we adopt the experimentally fitted parameter values  ms, V0 = −70 mV and VE = 0 mV24,25. Each V1 neuron i integrates inputs coming from LGN neurons j at spike time distributed as Poisson spike trains Rj by chemical couplings of δ function. When Vi reaches the threshold value −55 mV, neuron i emits a spike and then Vi is reset to −58 mV. The parameter g denotes the maximal synaptic conductance. The synaptic strength Sj(t) at thalamocortical system is subjected to the STD mechanism,

The parameter f (0.0 < f < 1.0) determines the amount of depression at synapse j induced by each spike in neuron j. The parameter denotes the depression recovery time. When the afferent neuron j fires a Poisson spike train at rate Rj, the synaptic strength will quickly decrease to the approximate steady state (SS, for a high rate Rj)24,

But, for a small firing rate Rj, the synaptic strength approximately maintains its original value 1. This depression model gives a good fit of experimental data24. In the following simulations, we take f = 0.75 and  ms (except for parameter values in Fig. 8(a,b) for comparison), which lie within the range indicated in the experimental data26,27. The main qualitative results do not depend on the two parameters.

Figure 8
figure 8

Robust power-law behavior with exponent −2 as the increase of σ1 against some parameters, f (a), (b), g (c), N (d), T (e), A (f) and σ2 (g). The parameters are (ag), (bg),  ms (a,cg), (a,b,dg), (ac,eg),  ms (ad,f,g), (ae,g) and (af). Data are averaged over 20 independent runs.

Experimentally, microsaccades are very fast movements3. In Ref. 17, we have studied the effect of finite microsaccadic speed and compared to experimental observations9. For simplicity, we here ignore the time course of microsaccades. In our simulations, microsaccade is regarded as a relative displacement ΔΜ of the tuning curve G1, which happens immediately. N neurons in LGN and V1 are spread uniformly in the ranges from −L to L (shown in Fig. 1), respectively. We count the total number of spikes of the V1 neurons in a moving time bin T as a measure of the neural response. N = 1000 (except for parameter values in Fig. 8(d) for comparison), L = 10 and T = 50 ms (except for parameter values in Fig. 8(e) for comparison) are given. Choosing different parameter values N, L and T, however, does not alter the qualitative results.

Results

In this section, our model predicts several new features of microsaccade-related neural responses which are likely testable in experiments. We first describe the dependence of microsaccade-induced neural responses on stimulus brightness, represented by the input amplitude A. Then, we study the effect of widths σ1 and σ2 of the two tuning curves G1 and G2. Finally, we provide a significant prediction that microsaccade-related neural response complies with a property “sharper is better”, which has been observed in many contexts in neuroscience, including orientation selectivity28, perceptual learning29,30 and auditory processing31. Interestingly, the property exhibits a robust power-law relationship between effectiveness of microsaccades and the width of Gaussian tuning curve G1.

Dependence on stimulus brightness

As shown in Eq. (3), the depression of synapse depends on the firing rate of the presynaptic LGN neuron. The higher the firing rate, the smaller the steady strength of the synapses. The stimulus brightness of fixation dot is denoted by amplitude A of the LGN neuron firing rate Rj in our model (shown in Fig. 1). Therefore, the stimulus brightness is expected to impact strongly on the microsaccade-related responses. Here, we simulated the effect of the amplitude A on neural responses induced by microsaccades. As shown in Fig. 2(a), a response peak appears soon after microsaccade and the value depends strongly on A. In detail, we measure the dependance of baseline of neural activity before microsaccade, peak of neural activity after microsaccade, change as the difference between peak and baseline and effectiveness as the ratio of change to baseline. Clearly, both the response baseline and peak increase linearly with the increasing of A when clear response can be induced for A > 50 (Fig. 2(b)) because the input from LGN neuron is proportional to A. We can write the linear relations of and , respectively. Here k1 and k2 denote the slopes of the two linear relations and c is the value of peak when A is 50. Since the peak is always larger than the baseline, there is k2 > k1 > 0. So, the change increases roughly linearly as the increasing of A (Fig. 2(c)). But, the effectiveness decreases as A becomes larger (Fig. 2(d)).

Figure 2
figure 2

(a) Microsaccade-related neural activity for different brightness A. Microsaccade-related response baseline and peak (b), change (c) and effectiveness (d) for different brightness A. The parameters are g = 0.15, ΔM = 0.8 and σ1 = σ2 = 1.5. Data are averaged over 20 independent runs.

Next, we studied effect of parameter A on the saturation of microsaccade-induced neural activity. In EEG experiment data, it has been recently found that9, neural response related with microsaccade increases with the increasing of microsaccade magnitude within the small region. The increasing response reaches a saturation value for larger microsaccade magnitudes. In our simulations, the response peak increases as the microsaccade magnitude ΔΜ with small size in Fig. 3. When the microsaccade magnitude increases to a threshold, the increasing response peak reaches to saturation, consistent with the experimental results in ref. 9. This saturation can be well explained as follows in our model17. When the moving distance due to large microsaccade exceeds the region with strong synapse-depression, the synaptic input will increase to the largest value (Sj = 1) and become independent of the microsaccade magnitude, leading to saturated response. Here, we focus on effect of parameter A on the saturation. It is found that in Fig. 3, the saturation value becomes larger and larger with the increasing of A. It is because A denotes the stimulus strength of fixated dot. The large A corresponds to the large neural response in LGN and so the large neural response and the responsive saturation value are produced in V1. We note that the threshold of microsaccade magnitude producing saturation is independent of A, since size of the synaptic depression region due to fixation is determined by the width of tuning curve G1 of firing Rj, which is fixed in these simulations.

Figure 3
figure 3

Saturation of the response peak for large microsaccades for different A.

Dashed line denotes the similar threshold of microsaccade magnitude producing saturation for different A. Here g = 0.2, σ2 = 1.5 and σ1 = 1.5. Data are averaged over 20 independent runs.

Effect of width of tuning curves

We simulate the effect of the width σ1 of tuning curve Rj on microsaccade-induced neural responses in Fig. 4. It is found that, both of the response baseline and peak increase with the increasing of σ1 within the small region (Fig. 4(b)), because the larger σ1 denotes the larger inputs from LGN neurons due to the broader firing region of Rj. When the σ1 increases to a certain value, the microsaccade with the fixed magnitude ΔΜ cannot move the fixated dot out of the depressed region of the synapses. The high response peak compared to baseline after microsaccade does not appear for the large σ1, i.e. the response peak tends to the response baseline (Fig. 4(b)). So, the change decreases approximately to zero with the increasing of σ1 (Fig. 4(c)). The decreasing Change and the increasing Baseline lead to the decreasing (Fig. 4(d)).

Figure 4
figure 4

The same as in Fig. 2, but for different σ1.

The parameters are g = 0.2, A = 100, ΔM = 2.0 and σ2 = 1.5.

Meanwhile, we simulate the effect of the width σ2 of tuning curve Wij on microsaccade-induced neural responses in Fig. 5. Since the larger σ2 reflects the larger inputs from LGN neurons due to the broader coupling region of Wij, the response baseline and peak in V1 increase linearly as the increasing of σ2 (Fig. 5(b)). Similar to the effect of A in Fig. 2, the linear increasings of Baseline and Peak lead to the increasing of Change (Fig. 5(c)) and the decreasing of Effectiveness (Fig. 5(d)).

Figure 5
figure 5

The same as in Fig. 2, but for different σ2.

The parameters are g = 0.2, A = 100, ΔM = 2.0 and σ1 = 1.5.

Next, we study effects of parameters σ1 and σ2 on the saturation of microsaccade-induced neural activity with respect to size ΔΜ. As shown in Fig. 6, the saturation value becomes larger and larger with increasing σ1 or σ2. Since the σ1 denotes the response region in LGN and the σ2 reflects the coupling region from LGN to V1, larger neural response and the responsive saturation value are produced in V1 for larger σ1 or σ2. Particularly, a threshold of microsaccade magnitude producing the saturation depends strongly on σ1 (Fig. 6(a)), but not much on σ2 (Fig. 6(b)). With the smaller width σ1, the smaller microsaccade magnitude is required for producing a saturated response. Here, we can give the following understanding. The threshold of microsaccade magnitude is determined by the depressed region of synapse strength Sj in space (in Eq. (3)), which strongly depends on the width σ1 of tuning curve Rj. The input to V1 neurons depends more strongly on the depressed region, but weakly on the width σ2 of tuning curve Wij. The smaller the width σ1, the smaller the synapse depressed region, leading to the smaller the threshold of microsaccade magnitude.

Figure 6
figure 6

The same as in Fig. 3, but for different σ1 (a) and σ2 (b). Dashed lines denote the different thresholds of microsaccade magnitude producing saturation for different σ1 and σ2. Here g = 0.2, A = 100, σ2 = 1.5 (a) and σ1 = 1.5 (b).

“Sharper is better” for microsaccades

Bell-shaped tuning curves are widely used to encode variables in the external world by many sensory and cortical neurons. Several studies have indicated that information conveyed by bell-shaped tuning curves increases as they decrease in width28,29,30,31. This is the so-called “sharper is better” effect. Especially, for the visual system, this has been experimentally found in many aspects of perceptual learning29,30. For example, the ability of trained monkeys to discriminate small changes is improved by sharpening of tuning curves in V1 neurons29. In our model, the microsaccade-related effectiveness reflects the relative change of neural responses after the onset of microsaccade, which supports the view: our neural system has evolved to optimally detect changes in our environment by moving eyes6. So, the effectiveness could be used to denote perceptual function. We focus on the effects of tuning curves G1 and G2 on the effectiveness. Clearly, Fig. 7 shows that microsaccade-related neural response displays the property “sharper is better”: the smaller the width (σ1 or σ2) of the Gaussian tuning curve (i.e., the sharper the curve), the larger the effectiveness. Particularly, the microsaccade-related effectiveness exhibits a power-law property as a function of σ1 with an exponent −2 (inset of Fig. 7(a)). A similar power law does not hold for σ2 (inset of Fig. 7(b)), while the effectiveness also decreases as the increasing of σ2. This power-law falloff is unusual, as it implies high effectiveness of response to microsaccades for broad range of σ1, indicating that this decrease is slower than exponential decay. In addition, we find that the exponent −2 is quite robust against some parameters, such as f, , g, N, T, A, σ2 and so on (Fig. 8).

Figure 7
figure 7

Microsaccade-related effectiveness as a function of the widths σ1 (a) and σ2 (b) of Gaussian tuning curves G1 and G2, respectively. The insets in (a) show a power-law behavior with an exponent −2 as the increase of σ1. The inset in (b): similar power law does not hold for σ2. The parameters are , , , (a) and (b). Data are averaged over 20 independent runs.

To explore the robust power-law property of effectiveness as a function of σ1, we give the following approximate calculation. With STD of synapses, it is found that24, the total steady-state synaptic conductance resulting from a set of afferents firing at rate σ2 is proportional to RjSj. So, in the network described by Eq. (1), the V1 response baseline is proportional to the sum of synaptical inputs from LGN before microsaccade and the V1 response peak is proportional to the sum after microsaccade (if we make the approximation that synapses add linearly), where denotes the firing rate in LGN after a microsaccade with small magnitude ΔM. We let the small ΔM not to exceed the synaptic depressed region. By using mean field theory and using the steady synapses in Eq. (3), we can get,

where denotes higher order infinitesimal of for large σ1. In Fig. 7(a) and Fig. 8, the power-law relations are given for large σ1 > 1. In the regime of large σ1, we here focus on the relation. We can approximately get . So, the effectiveness can be described by , where k and c0 are independent of σ1 and ΔM and are determined by other parameters of V1 neural dynamics in Eqs. (1) and (2). Moreover, the effectiveness is zero for the larger σ1 because response peak is equal to response baseline in Fig. 4. So, the c0 is identical to 0. We finally get,

Obviously, the effectiveness exhibits an approximate power-law form as a function of σ1 with a robust exponent −2 irrespective of any parameters, such as f, , g, N, T, A and σ2, which is consistent with our simulations in Fig. 8. It is noted that, in the derivation of Eq. (4), the microsaccadic size ΔM is assumed to be small enough that the microsaccade does not exceed the synaptic depressed region. The effectiveness exhibits another power-law property as a function of small ΔM with a positive exponent 2 in Eq. (4). In our simulations, the property is verified for the microsaccade magnitudes smaller than the threshold value producing responsive saturation, shown in Fig. 9. The effectiveness maintains constant for further increasing of the large microsaccade magnitude ΔM (shown in the right side of Fig. 9) because of the responsive saturation (see Fig. 6).

Figure 9
figure 9

Power-law property with exponent 2 as the increase of ΔΜ.

The parameters are given by , , and . Data are averaged over 20 independent runs.

Discussion

By using our model, we extensively study and predict the dependance of microsaccade-related neural responses and responsive saturation value on several key parameters A, σ1 and σ2, which could be tuned in experiments. For possible verification of our theoretical predictions based on the model, we propose the following feasible experimental designs. The amplitude A and width σ1 of Gaussian tuning curve G1 evoked by fixated dot can be modified by changing brightness and size of the dot, which can be easily implemented in experiments. Specifically, the more brightness corresponds to the larger amplitude A and larger dot corresponds to larger width σ1. In addition, the width σ2 of Gaussian orientation tuning curve G2 of thalamocortical connecting weights may be modified by training certain microsaccade-related ability, motivated by ref. 29 where the orientation tuning curve at trained orientation became sharper for trained monkey, while modifications of tuning curve were not observed for the monkey which had not been trained. Then results similar to our simulations could be expected.

Particularly, the change of effectiveness due to change of σ1 displays strong robustness against other parameters with power-law relation. It is plausible that this property reinforces the idea that microsaccades contribute to the vision-detection function: the same microsaccade exhibits different effectiveness for different distribution width σ1 of light evoked by fixation dot, which seems to support the view: our neural system has evolved to optimally detect changes in our environment by moving eyes6.

In conclusion, by using a feedforward network model with STD, several new features of microsaccade-related neural responses are theoretically predicted, which could be tested in experiments. Particularly, we provide a significant prediction “sharper is better”, which has been extensively found in many aspects of perceptual learning. By using mean field theory, we give analytical study on the robust power-law property for “sharper is better”. This prediction, if experimentally verified, would strongly suggest STD in thalamocortical synapses as an important contributor to “sharper is better”. Generally, our study is the first to theoretically predict microsaccade-related neural responses by using biologically plausible model, which could be further tested in experiments. These predictions can give guidance for further experimental studies of the role of microsaccades in visual information processing. These modelling results suggest that, the depression model may be very useful for further investigating behavioral properties and functional roles of microsaccades.

Additional Information

How to cite this article: Zhou, J.-F. et al. Model predictions of features in microsaccade-related neural responses in a feedforward network with short-term synaptic depression. Sci. Rep. 6, 20888; doi: 10.1038/srep20888 (2016).