Introduction

The contribution of multiple individuals to group decision-making can bring substantial benefits1. Shared decisions can be more accurate2,3, for example homing pigeons (Columbia livia) have more direct homing routes when flying in dyads than when flying alone4. Shared decisions also allow all group members to acquire vital resources while remaining part of the group5, as they allow individuals in a state of need to influence group decisions6. While many studies have found evidence in support for shared decision-making, for example by observing a range of different individuals initiating movements7,8,9, the extent to which collective decision-making is governed by similar movement rules across species requires further investigation10,11.

Collective decisions can be an emergent outcome of the movement interactions among individuals12. The classic theoretical model that proposes this hypothesis provides two sets of testable predictions: (i) that the geometry of a conflict in preferences among initiators (the angle of their directional vectors) should determine the actions of followers, and (ii) that individuals should follow a majority rule when choosing which direction to follow1,11,12. The first prediction is that when faced with differences in the preferred direction of movement of group members, followers should average between directions if the disagreement among initiators is small (i.e. ‘compromise’) or choose one option over the other (i.e. ‘choose’) if the disagreement is large (above a critical angle12). Greater disagreement (e.g. a larger angle between initiators and/or having more initiators proposing different directions) should also reduce the probability of following13. The second prediction is that when choosing a direction, followers should move where the majority of preferences are directed12. These key predictions allow quantitative comparisons of the processes driving collective decisions across different species. However, testing these predictions is challenging, as they require information about how potential decision-makers—both initiators and followers—move relative to one-another14.

Two studies have provided evidence for the geometric prediction of the aforementioned classic model of collective motion in semi-wild or wild animal groups4,13. GPS-tracking of pairs of homing pigeons showed that if the disagreement between the two birds’ directional preferences when flying back home was small, individuals averaged their routes. Instead, if disagreement was over a critical threshold, either the dyad split or one of the two birds became the leader4. However, as the study was conducted on dyads, there was no test of the classic model’s prediction on which direction individuals would choose when faced with large disagreement and a numerical difference between the clusters of concurrent initiators. That gap was covered by a study13 that fitted GPS trackers to the majority of individuals in a troop of olive baboons, a species in which individuals form groups with very stable membership. By analysing the relative movements of individuals, and extracting initiations and following behaviours, the study showed support for the two sets of predictions for shared decision-making emerging from interactions among individuals. First, individuals were less likely to follow when there was greatest directional conflict among initiators, but when following, individual baboons averaged proposed directions by initiators when the disagreement was small and chose one or the other when the disagreement was large13. Second, when choosing a direction, individual baboons used a majority rule—moving in the direction with the largest number of initiators13. Thus, evidence is beginning to suggest that emergent decision-making processes might be relatively common across animals that move as groups, and could potentially be underpinned by a consistent set of individual decision rules.

One challenge with determining whether species use similar rules when making decisions is that careful replication is required. While the replication crisis in biology15,16,17 largely stems from the incentive structures favouring novelty18, there are also logistical barriers to replication. For example, one recent study19 testing whether the increase of CO2 in the ocean impacts the behaviour of coral reel fish replicated previous experiments by examining a large number of captive fish (900) from multiple species (6) and across several years (3), matching the conditions of older experiments and finding low support for the original results. However, critics—right or wrongly—noted that methodological differences could also contribute differences in the results20, meaning that the true answer remains largely unknown. The challenges that are inherent with working with whole organisms, and with the different ecological conditions that they might experience in different studies, means that replications remain relatively rare. While large-scale collaborative networks21,22 can overcome some of the barriers to making comparative studies, large-scale and long-term studies conducted in the wild often cannot be replicated, despite these being among the most influential23,24,25,26. A consequence of this is not only a lack of certainty in our scientific results, but also a lack of data on the generality of our findings.

Here, we conduct a within- and between-species replication study to investigate how consensus is achieved when individuals are faced with conflicting directional preferences among group members. We study vulturine guineafowl, a sympatric species to olive baboons that also forms stable and cohesive groups. A previous observational study on collective departures suggested that every member of a vulturine guineafowl group can initiate movement from a scattered food resource but that individuals excluded from clumped food patches are more likely to lead their group after receiving aggression6. These findings indicated that dominance can play a role in modulating leadership—at least in the context of departures from food patches. In the present study, we fit high-resolution solar-powered GPS-trackers to almost all adults from two groups of vulturine guineafowl and implement the same analytical procedure as the previous study on baboons13 to determine how group members reach consensus across a broader range of movements. We first confirm that all group members can successfully initiate movement, and that males (who are on top of the dominance hierarchy27) have greater influence on group movements than females. We then show that vulturine guineafowl express the same geometric properties and majority rule as predicted by the classic theoretical model of leadership and collective decision-making12, and match almost exactly the empirical results observed in wild olive baboons13. Our study provides a powerful replication of previous empirical work, enabling quantitative comparisons between observational and GPS-based methods, and between two taxonomically distant species that live in the same habitat.

Results

Sex but not dominance determine who has influence

Decisions by animal groups can be despotic, where one individual decides28, partially shared (or graded), where some individuals contribute to decisions more than others29, or fully shared, where all individuals have an equal influence13. When contribution to decision-making is unequal, it has generally been predicted that dominant individuals should have greater influence30, but this has received mixed support28,31,32,33,34.

We first explored whether leadership in vulturine guineafowl is fully shared or graded by quantifying the role of dominance on influence. Vulturine guineafowl groups have steep dominance hierarchies (see Supplementary Fig. 1), which remain stable for several months6,27. We applied the approach described by Strandburg-Peshkin et al.13 to infer who initiates movements and who follows, based on dyadic movement patterns from the GPS tracks collected simultaneously across group members (see Methods for details on GPS tracking). Initiation attempts were characterised by an increasing inter-individual distance followed by a decreasing inter-individual distance (see Methods and Fig. 1). Depending on the relative contribution of each individual to the change in distance, initiations were classified as being successful (‘pulls’, where A moves to increase the distance and B moves to subsequently reduce the distance) or unsuccessful (‘anchors’, where A moves to increase the distance but, subsequently, decreases it by moving back towards B).

Fig. 1: The distribution of distances (d, in metres) between initiators and potential followers, during pulls and anchors at three different times.
figure 1

First, when the initiation starts (first column; A, D, G, J); second, when the initiator reaches the maximum distance away from the potential follower (second column; B, E, H, K); and third, when the potential follower starts moving towards the leader (in pulls; C, I) or the leader returns by moving towards the potential follower (in anchors; F, L). Distributions of inter-individual distances are shown separately for groups 1 (AF) and 2 (GL).

Summarising 502,253 leader-follower cases from two social groups, we confirm that all group members can initiate movement and pull others, but that there is a distinct subset of individuals that are more likely to be followed (Fig. 2A, D). To investigate the relationship between dominance and the probability of being followed, we ran permutation tests within and between sexes (Fig. 2B, C, E, F, see the Methods section for details on the permutations). While it appears that more dominant individuals are more likely to be followed (Fig. 2B i, E i), analyses controlling for sex show it was rather that males, who are dominant over females, are more likely to be followed (Fig. 2B ii-iii, C, E ii-iii, F), and that there is no effect of dominance within sex. In a two-puller context comprising one male and one female initiator and where followers choose one direction (see below), the effect of sex translates to a difference in success rate of approximately 10% (Group 1; Pmale success = 0.543, Group 2; Pmale success = 0.553). However, success is not only determined by the probability of being followed, but also by the rate of initiating. When considering the number of successful initiations for each individual, we again find no effect of dominance but a consistent effect of sex (Fig. 3).

Fig. 2: Dyadic influence, and relationships with dominance and sex, across two social groups.
figure 2

A, D In each group, we calculated the dyadic influence index, ranging from 1 (red) when the individual in the column always leads the individual in the row to −1 (blue) when the relationship is reversed. Individuals are plotted in descending dominance rank, e.g. the alpha male of Group 1 is “W1316” and the lowest ranking group member being the female “W1544”. Individuals in rectangles are males. B, E The result of permutation tests on the relationship between dominance and leadership across all individuals (i), within males (ii), and within females (iii). While there appears to be an effect of dominance on individual influence (i), this effect is not present within sexes (ii-iii). The y-axis corresponds to the absolute difference between dominance rank and influence rank (see Methods). C, F A permutation test on the relationship between sex and influence confirms that the relationship is driven by males being more influential than females. The y-axis shows the absolute difference between two binary variables; whether an individual was within the top n-ranked individuals, where n represented the number of males in the group and whether the individual was a male or not (see Methods). Panels B, C, E, F show the observed value (red star) relative to the mean (black dot) and 95% range from 1000 permutations of the datasets. P values are calculated as the proportion of the number of values of the randomised data being larger than the observed values divided by the number of randomised values (i.e. 1000).

Fig. 3: Initiation rates, and relationships with dominance and sex, across two social groups.
figure 3

Panels A and D show the number of times each individual pulled another group member (black) or was anchored (grey). Individuals are plotted in descending dominance rank and those in rectangles are males. B, E The result of permutation tests on the relationship between dominance and successful initiation rates per hour (i.e. pulls) across all individuals (i), within males (ii), and within females (iii). While there appears to be a weak effect of dominance (i), this effect is not present within sexes (ii-iii). C, F A permutation test on the relationship between sex and successful initiation rates per hour (i.e. pulls) confirms that the relationship is driven by males initiating more often than females. The y-axis shows the absolute difference between two binary variables; whether an individual was within the top n-ranked individuals, where n represented the number of males in the group and whether the individual was a male or not (see Methods). The y-axes in B, C, E, F correspond to the absolute difference between dominance rank and ranked values of initiation rates. Permutation tests and panels are as per Fig. 2.

Our results show that leadership in vulturine guineafowl is shared, aligning with the previous work on olive baboons13 and with direct observations in this system6. However, unlike in baboons, leadership in vulturine guineafowl is not completely equal. Instead, it is graded, with males being more likely to be followed and initiating at higher rates (on average) relative to females. This difference to baboons may relate to the fact that males, who are dominant, are also the philopatric sex in vulturine guineafowl35. Staying in their natal group potentially allows guineafowl males to maintain life-long, and thus stronger, influence relationships with other members of their group. The natal sex also has more information about the local landscape than the dispersing sex, which may contribute to observed differences (though we note that the baboon study13 did not explicitly tested for a sex difference). This difference may not, however, always play a role in decision-making. Females were often as successful at initiating as male group members, and many females initiated movements more often than some males. The relatively small differences between male and female vulturine guineafowl is likely to reflect the relatively low rates of conflict in most of their collective movements. We found that guineafowl are substantially more likely to follow initiators (Psuccess range: 0.7–0.9) than baboons (Psuccess range: 0.2–0.8)13. This is likely to explain the high degree of cohesion and small intra-group dispersion of vulturine guineafowl.

Individuals are more likely to follow when initiators agree

We aggregated the simultaneous initiation attempts acting on single candidate follower individuals into ‘events’ based on their overlapping start and finish timestamps, following Strandburg-Peshkin et al. 13. While initiation attempts lasted on average for 2.4 min (SD = 2.7), the temporal overlapping nature of initiations that were combined into events meant that events lasted longer than the initiation attempts themselves, with an average of 7.0 min (SD = 6.2; Supplementary Fig. 2). For each event, we calculated the direction of the initiators in relation to the position of the potential follower and their directional agreement. Directional agreement ranged from 0, when the movement vectors of initiators were equally distributed over potential directions, to 1 when the movement vectors were perfectly aligned (see Methods section for more details). We also noted whether the potential follower was subsequently pulled or not, and calculated the direction of the movement of the follower if the follower was pulled. We defined events as successful if at least one initiator pulled the potential follower.

We found that the number of simultaneous initiators, the level of their directional agreement, and the interaction between these two, all predict the probability of following a given initiation. In both groups, increasing the number of initiators has a positive effect on following when the angular agreement was high, but a negative effect on following when the agreement was low (Fig. 4; Supplementary Table 1). Although the results are consistent across both groups, only the interaction is significant in Group 2 (for which we collected substantially fewer data; see Supplementary Table 2). Supplementary analyses that account for changes in the number of tracked individuals and the distance between initiators and potential followers confirm that our results are robust to variation in data collection and to the assumptions of the methods.

Fig. 4: An individual is more likely to follow when there are few simultaneous initiators, and, as the number of concurrent initiators increases, when initiators have high agreement.
figure 4

As the number of initiators increases, then the directional agreement becomes increasingly important in determining the decision of a follower, as revealed by the positive interaction term in the GEE models (Supplementary Table 1).

The interaction between agreement and the number of initiators on the tendency for vulturine guineafowl to follow, matches closely with the behaviour of baboons. Specifically, baboons also require greater agreement when there are more initiators in order to follow13. In vulturine guineafowl, the patterns are also very similar across both groups: having more simultaneous initiators requires a higher agreement for individuals to follow, and high levels of agreement (>0.6) generally result in a better-than-chance (>0.5) probability of an event being successful. Baboons appear to be more tolerant of disagreement, with any agreement over 0.3 producing a better-than-chance probability of an initiation being successful13.

Followers compromise the initiation directions when initiators agree but choose a direction when initiators disagree

For each successful event, we tested the theoretical prediction12 that the angular agreement of the initiators should determine where a follower moves next. For simplicity, in this particular test we focused on events comprising two initiators (17.160% of all events for Group 1 and 18.157% of all events for Group 2, see Supplementary Table 2), allowing us to calculate the angle between the initiators relative to the potential follower. If a follower moves in a direction that averaged the angle between initiators (i.e. ‘compromise’), then we expected a unimodal distribution in the directions taken by followers across repeated observations at a given angular disagreement. By contrast, if a follower ‘chooses’ one or the other direction, then we expected a bimodal distribution in the directions taken by followers across repeated observations with the same angle of disagreement.

We found that the direction taken by vulturine guineafowl followers has identical properties to those predicted by theory and those found in baboons. Specifically, in both guineafowl groups, followers compromise the initiated directions when the disagreement between initiators is below a critical threshold that separates the two regimes, and choose one direction versus the other when the disagreement is above the threshold (Fig. 5, Supplementary Fig. 3).

Fig. 5: When following, vulturine guineafowl transition from compromise to choose depending on the angular disagreement between initiators.
figure 5

Plots show the angle between two initiators relative to a potential follower (x-axis) and the resulting direction taken by the follower (y-axis). When the angular difference between initiators is above a critical threshold (see grey rectangle for transitional zone), follower directions are significantly bimodal (see Supplementary Fig. 3), suggesting that followers choose one direction or the other. Colours blue, white and red show the probability of a direction to be chosen by a follower. Solid white lines represent the median of the chosen direction(s) under the compromise and choose regimes (or both in the transitional zone).

As with previous results, we found strong concurrence between vulturine guineafowl and baboons. Baboons also express a transitional phase from compromise to choose, which is estimated to range between 72 and 96 degrees13. In Group 1, we found that the lower end of the transitional phase from compromise to choose is almost identical to that of baboons (78 degrees), but that the upper end is much higher (130 degrees). In Group 2, we could only find a transition threshold, which is estimated to be 117 degrees. But, as estimates for Group 2 are based on substantially fewer data, we expect that adding more data will reveal a larger range of uncertainty as in Group 1. The data from Group 2 do, however, also suggest that the upper end of the transition phase to the choose regime takes place at a larger angle in vulturine guineafowl than in baboons.

Followers move in the direction of the majority when choosing

To find where a follower moves when in the choose regime, we focused on cases when two or more individuals initiated toward different directions. We used a spatial clustering algorithm to identify sets of individuals co-initiating in similar directions, extracted cases in which there were exactly two clusters, and counted the number of individuals initiating in each of the two directions.

As predicted, we found support that vulturine guineafowl employ a majority rule when choosing one versus the other direction to move in (i.e. at high levels of disagreement, on the right side of each panel of Fig. 5). Specifically, in both Group 1 and Group 2, followers are disproportionately more likely to move in the direction containing the largest cluster of initiators (Fig. 6, Supplementary Table 3).

Fig. 6: When two clusters of initiators propose different directions with a large angle of disagreement, followers disproportionately choose the direction of the largest cluster of initiators, thus following a majority rule.
figure 6

Black dots represent the empirical data and error bars are 95% confidence intervals estimated by 1000 bootstrapped replications of the data. The red line shows a significant sigmoidal fit to the data. Model summaries are presented in Supplementary Table 3.

Our results confirm that vulturine guineafowl use a similar majority rule to baboons when choosing between directions. In baboons, individuals have an 80% chance of choosing the majority when the difference between the number of initiators in each cluster is three or more. By contrast, the model fits predict that vulturine guineafowl require a larger numerical difference (a difference of 8–9 for Group 1 and 4–5 for Group 2) to reach the same level of discrimination.

Discussion

Our study shows that the movements of vulturine guineafowl are consistent with the predictions from a classic theoretical model of leadership and collective decision-making, and have striking similarities to the movements described in taxonomically distant but sympatric olive baboons13. In both our guineafowl study groups, we found that any individual could initiate movement, with no direct link between dominance and influence. Male guineafowl are more likely to be followed than females, and also have slightly higher rates of initiations. However, females still initiated often, and many had a high number of successful attempts. Like in baboons, conflicts in vulturine guineafowl group decisions affect the probability that initiators are followed and, when they do, follower movements fall into one of two regimes: when the disagreement between concurrent initiators is small followers average the directions of the initiators and when the disagreement is large they choose the direction with the most initiators. Our study also demonstrates the importance of replication in ecology and animal behaviour15,16,36, showing that by following the same methods and conducting the same statistical tests we could reveal that the emergence of collective decisions from simple rules governing group cohesion are likely to be consistent across very distinct taxonomic groups.

While influence can be distributed within the group37, whereby all individuals can initiate movement and be followed, it is not necessarily equal among group members29,38,39. For example, homing pigeons form influence hierarchies during flight and these hierarchies determine whom an individual is likely to lead and are most likely to be led by Nagy et al.32. However, these influence hierarchies are independent from dominance hierarchies33. In vulturine guineafowl, we found that leadership is generally shared, but that males are more likely to be followed and initiate more often than females. This difference reflects the social structure of vulturine guineafowl societies, where males are dominant over all females6,27, who are also the dispersing sex35. However, males have been found to be more influential than females in collective departures also in species in which female matrilines dominate aggression hierarchies7,40, suggesting that neither the dominance hierarchy alone, nor the dispersal tendency of the sexes, always determine which individuals influence group coordination. Further, while the differences we found between males and females are significant, females still had substantial influence over where groups moved. One key outstanding question is therefore to identify whether there are specific contexts in which the ability to exert influence (i.e. have a higher probability of being followed) may be important.

Within each sex group of vulturine guineafowl, we found no evidence for dominance playing a role on influence (although in both groups, the lowest ranking female initiated less often than almost any other group member). While it appears that female vulturine guineafowl have overall less influence than males on where their group goes, unlike in other species41,42,43,44,45, it is also possible that females exhibit specific strategies to influence decisions46,47. For example, they could influence when groups leave6, as has been suggested in baboons48, with timing decisions potentially reflecting a distinct axis of decision-making13,49. Such routes of influence would not be obvious from the analytical approach that was employed in the current and baboon studies13. Rather, our approach likely captured a number of very general collective movements, including many moment-by-moment decisions (e.g. which way to move around a tree). Identifying the functional importance of each decision (e.g. those that dictate where groups move next at larger spatial scales) remains a challenge in the field.

Despite the properties of follower movements in response to initiators being very similar across vulturine guineafowl and baboons, details may vary from group to group50 and from one context to the next51. In theory, larger group sizes should be associated with a decrease in the angle at which follower movements transition from compromise to choose12. Group 1 was similar in size to the previously studied baboon group, and their transitional phase overlapped13. Further, the transitional phase of Group 1 started at values that were almost 40 degrees smaller than the smaller Group 2, albeit it also went beyond that of Group 2 (Fig. 6). Given this overlap, we can’t safely draw conclusions on whether our findings support the theoretical predictions that indicate that larger groups show a smaller transitional angle, and therefore data on more groups of different sizes are required to address this question. The potential influence of the social, as well as the physical environment is worth exploring, given that environmental effects have already been documented across various facets of collective behaviour51.

One behaviour where we could find some clearer differences between groups is when looking at the majority rule employed by each. In vulturine guineafowl, the smaller group (Group 2) appeared to require a smaller threshold in order to identify a clear majority. A key question is whether the shift in the threshold scales with group size. Our data suggest that the larger group (Group 1) reliably chose the majority (80% of the time) when there was a higher proportional difference (approximately one third of the group 1) in initiators compared to the smaller group (approximately one quarter of the group 2). Baboons required a much smaller majority to reach the same 80% threshold13. Given that the baboon troop that was studied was larger than Group 1 (and had a very similar proportion of GPS-tracked group members), these results suggest that discrimination may be harder in larger groups, and that baboons could have a better capacity to discriminate smaller relative differences than vulturine guineafowl.

Our results show that the processes driving the movement patterns of wild group-living vulturine guineafowl are largely consistent with those previously described in a group of wild baboons13 and in dyads of homing pigeons4, with the specific directions of movements by individuals and responses to conflict when following in particular matching those of olive baboons. Our work adds to the weight of support for predictions arising from a classic theoretical model of leadership and collective decision-making12. Further, by carefully conducting a large-scale within- and between-species replication, we propose that a multitude of group-living species could exhibit highly convergent processes governing how they reach consensus on where to move.

Methods

Data collection

Our study population of vulturine guineafowl resides in a savannah-woodland ecosystem of approximately 12 km2 in the southern part of the Mpala Research Centre (MRC) in Laikipia, Kenya. Vulturine guineafowls are large (~1.5 kg), predominantly terrestrial, and live in relatively large groups (13–65 adults) with largely stable membership52. Groups are not territorial and associate preferentially with specific other groups52.

GPS trackers

We fitted with GPS solar-powered tags almost all adult members from two groups of vulturine guineafowl. We programmed the GPS tags to simultaneously collect 1 Hz data every fourth day from 06:00 to 19:00 by allowing them to fully recharge over three days before starting a full day of operating. For the purposes of other research projects running at the same time53, we set one to two tags in each group to work on a daily schedule and during some months the tags of all individuals in focal Group 2 where programmed to work on a daily basis (see Supplementary Table 4 for the group size per month, number of tagged individuals, how long they were tracked and GPS tag programming setting, see also Supplementary Movies 16 for a demonstration of the whole-group tracking datasets of Groups 1 and 2). This ‘daily’ setting recorded one data point (date, time, coordinates) every second when the battery had a high charge (approximately every second to third day, for up to 8 h continuously). When the battery was at the next highest threshold, tags recorded 10 points spanning the first 10 s of every fifth minute. At the lowest battery threshold, tags recorded one point every 15 min (this setting was used less than 1% of the time). We downloaded data remotely every two to three days using a BaseStation II (e-obs Digital Telemetry, Grünwald, Germany).

We conducted census observations every two days (on average) to record changes on group size and the number of tagged individuals per group across the study period, as some individuals got predated or lost their tags. We summarise this information in Supplementary Table 4.

Dominance hierarchies

First, to estimate the dominance hierarchy, we conducted all-occurrence sampling in each group, recording different types of agonistic interactions, as described by Papageorgiou & Farine6 and Dehnen et al. 27. For each observed interaction, we recorded the time, the winner, and the loser. We recorded data over at least 3 sessions, lasting 2-3 h each, per group, per week across the study period (restricted to days when simultaneous GPS tracking was not taking place). From the agonistic interactions data, we calculated a dominance hierarchy for each group using the randomised Elo scores method54.

To test if the dominance hierarchy remained stable during the study period, we calculated the repeatability score of ranks by randomising the order of the data, splitting the dataset in two halves, and calculating the Spearman rank correlation coefficients across the estimates of ranks from each half. We repeated this process 1000 times, using the function ‘estimate_uncertainty_by_splitting’ from the ‘aniDom’ R package54, to estimate a mean and 95% confidence intervals of the correlation values.

Data processing

We used (and adapted where necessary) the methods and published code developed by Strandburg-Peshkin et al.13. We repeated each of the following steps on the data from the two study groups separately.

Pre-processing GPS data

We used the built-in features from the Movebank data repository to remove the outliers from our dataset that were falling outside of our study area (<0.001% of the data, corresponding to points that were often outside of Kenya). In the rare cases when a tag failed to log one point (e.g. skipping one second, 0.16–0.21% of the data in both groups), we linearly interpolated missing points based on the existing data around that point from the same tag. More specifically, if there was a missing value at time t, between t − 1 s and t + 1 s, we added one point in time t, in the middle of the straight line connecting the two known points of t − 1 s and t + 1 s.

Extracting successful and failed initiation attempts at the dyadic level

We extracted movement initiations, and their outcomes by identifying maxima and minima in the dyadic distance between a given pair of individuals. The data between a minima and a maxima identified cases when an individual \(i\) moved away from another \(j\) (i.e. an initiation). The subsequent behaviour of individual \(j\) between the maxima and the following minima determined the interpretation of the event. If \(j\) moved towards the direction of \(i\), the outcome was defined as a “pull”, whereas if \(i\) moved back towards \(j\), then the outcome was defined as an “anchor”.

We used a set of thresholds to remove pulls and anchors potentially arising from GPS noise or small movements. Specifically, we defined initiation events as only those in which the minimum change in distance between \(i\) and \(j\) was more than 3.5 m. We believe this threshold to be biologically relevant considering the scale that the movements of vulturine guineafowl take place, especially given their high degree of spatial cohesion. It is also above the error of the GPS tags, as our field testing suggested that the estimated relative position of two GPS tags is accurate to within 1 m more than 95% of the time52. Further, we determined that pull or anchor events required one individual doing a disproportionate amount of movement, setting a “disparity” threshold of 0.1, whereby 0 represents both individuals having moved equally during an event and 1 represents a single individual having done all of the moving during the event. Finally, we set a “strength” threshold to 0.1, which could range from 0 when the change in dyadic distance was very small relative to the total dyadic distance (i.e. small movements by individuals far away) to 1 when the change in dyadic distance was very large compared to the total dyadic distance (large movements by individuals that are in the same spot). The latter two are the same settings as the original study by Strandburg-Peshkin et al., whereas we set the minimum change in distance to a smaller value (3.5 m instead of 5 m) as vulturine guineafowl are substantially smaller and more cohesive in their movements than baboons. The dyadic distances throughout the process of initiation are shown in Fig. 1, confirming the small distances over which leadership interactions take place in vulturine guineafowl.

We only kept in subsequent analysis events that took place when at least half of group members’ tags were collecting data, which largely matched the distribution of the data in the original baboon study. In that study, 80% of adults and subadults were tagged, however some tags stopped working for periods of data collection, meaning that as few as 16 of the 26 collared baboons (55%) collected data on some days13. We also applied, and present, the results using a threshold keeping only events when at least 80% of group members’ tags collected data at the same time. The results are presented in the Supplementary Note 1 of the Supplementary Materials (Supplementary Tables 58 and Supplementary Figs. 48) and show that the patterns in our results are not sensitive to the choice of threshold.

Identifying simultaneous initiation events

To investigate pulls and anchors beyond the dyadic level, we grouped together interactions (potential pulls and anchors) that operated simultaneously (i.e. involving one or more initiation attempts that overlapped in time) on one potential follower, and we defined this as an event. We considered interactions as overlapping in time using a chain rule, meaning that if interaction A overlapped with B, and interaction B overlapped with C, then all three would be combined into one event regardless of whether interaction A overlapped with interaction C. For each event, we calculated the direction of the initiators in relation to the position of the potential follower, whether the potential follower was pulled or not, and the direction of the subsequent movement of the follower if the follower was pulled. We defined events as successful if, and only if, at least one initiator was recorded as having pulled the potential follower. To test for a majority rule—whether followers moved in the direction with most initiators, we also clustered of initiators according to their direction using Gaussian Mixture Models55.

Statistics and reproducibility

Does dominance predict influence?

To investigate if dominance predicts influence within each group, we created a matrix representing the relative influence among dyads, with the influence index in dyad, \(i\) and \(j\) defined as \({I}_{i,j}=\frac{{P}_{i,j}-{P}_{j,i}}{{P}_{i,j}+{P}_{j,i}}\), where \({P}_{i,j}\) represents the number of events individual \(i\) pulled individual \(j\). The index ranges from −1 (\(j\) pulled in all events) to 1 (\(i\) pulled in all events), with 0 representing no difference in influence among the two individuals. From these data, we calculated influence ranks by summing each individual’s indices and ranking these sums such that individuals with a larger sum were considered to be more influential.

We examined the effects of dominance and sex on influence rank by running four permutation tests:

(i) We calculated the mean absolute difference between dominance rank and influence rank for each individual. If dominant individuals were more highly ranked in the influence matrix, then we expected this value to approach 0. We evaluated the significance of our measure by recalculating the same value 1000 times after randomising the order of individuals’ dominance ranks relative to their influence ranks.

(ii–iii) We tested whether there was a within-sex effect of dominance by conducting the same test as (i) in males and females independently.

(iv) We tested whether males were more likely to be on the top of the influence hierarchy by calculating the mean of the absolute difference between two binary variables. The first variable represented whether an individual was within the top n-ranked more influential individuals, where n represented the number of males in the group. The second binary variable represented whether the individual was a male or not. We then re-calculated this value in 1000 permutations randomising the link between the two binary variables.

Then, we also examined the effects of dominance and sex on the successful initiation rates (pulls) per hour. To do this, we ran permutation tests similar to (i-iv), but we replaced influence rank with the individuals ranked according to the rates of successful initiations per hour.

In all the permutation tests, we considered an effect to be significant at α = 0.05 if the observed value was closer to 0 than 95% of the values generated by the permuted datasets.

Finally, to examine the effect of sex in the success rate of initiating, we extracted cases in which there were two simultaneous pullers comprising one male and one female initiator. We then calculated the proportion of time the male was the successful puller. We tested the significance of this measure by randomising the sexes across all of these events 1000 times. This allowed us to test if males were more successful pullers than expected by chance. As above, we considered an effect to be significant at α = 0.05 if the observed value was smaller than the probabilities of 95% of the permuted datasets.

How do the agreement and number of initiators affect whether initiators are successful?

We first tested the factors that contributed to individuals’ decisions about whether to follow or not. From the full set of events, we constructed a generalised estimating equations (GEE) model testing whether a focal individual would follow or not (binary response variable where pull = 1 and anchor = 0) was predicted by the level of agreement, and the number of initiators. We quantified directional agreement among simultaneous initiators using the circular variance (cv) of the unit vectors pointing from the potential follower to each initiator in the event, and defined agreement as 1 − cv. Values of agreement are close to 0 when individuals initiate in opposing directions and approach 1 when all individuals initiate in the same direction. Given that events, include all simultaneous initiations by default, our GEE did not include an autocorrelation structure. We used the R package ‘geepack’56 to fit the GEE model.

How does the angle between initiators affect where followers move?

We tested whether the angle between initiators predicted where individuals moved in cases where a guineafowl did follow an initiation, focusing on events comprising two initiators. To identify which regime followers used (compromise or choose) for a given angle of disagreement, we ran a dip test of bimodality and a converging modes test (using the method developed by Hartigan & Hartigan57, and the code from Strandburg-Peshkin et al.13). If vulturine guineafowl were in the compromise regime, then the distribution of angles taken by the follower would not be significantly bimodal (according to the dip test) and would be more unimodal than expected by chance (according to the converging modes test). If neither of these conditions held, then vulturine guineafowl were in the choose regime. We interpreted situations in which one condition held but not the other as demarking a transition between the compromise and choose regimes. We ran these analyses by combining events into 12 degree bins of angular disagreement. We conducted this analysis independently on both groups, using code developed by Strandburg-Peshkin et al.13.

Where do guineafowl move when they choose one direction versus the other?

Finally, we investigated which direction followers chose when in the choose regime by examining the numerical difference among clusters of simultaneous pullers. Specifically, we expand the analysis in the previous section by looking at all events with more than one puller. In each of these events, we used a circular clustering algorithm13 to identify clusters of individuals pulling in similar directions. We then extracted all of the events containing two clusters, and counted the number of individuals in each of the clusters. We then identified which of these clusters was successful, and related this to the numerical difference in the size of each cluster.

If guineafowl follow a majority rule, then they should be much more likely to follow numerically larger clusters. Following Strandburg-Peshkin et al. 201513, we first fit a non-linear least square model where the response variable was the probability of choosing a randomly allocated cluster 1, while the predictor was the numerical difference between the number of individuals in cluster 1 minus the number of individuals in cluster 2. We then estimated the uncertainty for each bin (i.e. for each numerical difference between the size of initiating cluster 1 minus the size of initiating cluster 2) by drawing n samples from a uniform distribution, where n is the number of events in that bin, and calculating the probability that the random values are less than or equal to the observed probability. We repeated this process 1000 times, and extracted the lower 2.5th and upper 97.5th quantile of these probabilities as a measure of the 95% confidence intervals.

Reporting summary

Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.