Abstract
Standard accounts of mental health are based on a “deficit view” solely focusing on cognitive impairments associated with psychiatric conditions. Based on the principle of neural competition, we suggest an alternative. Rather than focusing on deficits, we should focus on the cognitive potential that selective dysfunctions might bring with them. Our approach is based on two steps: the identification of the potential (i.e., of neural systems that might have benefited from reduced competition) and the development of corresponding training methods, using the testing-the-limits approach. Counterintuitively, we suggest to train not only the impaired function but on the function that might have benefitted or that may benefit from the lesser neural competition of the dysfunctional system.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Psychiatry and clinical psychology, except humanistic and positive psychology based on encouraging human potential1,2,3, are driven by a deficit view: people whose mental performance deviates to a particular (often not well-defined and justified) degree from what is considered average human performance are deemed to be “ill” and in need of correction—with the therapeutic aim of reducing the discrepancy between their performance and the population mean. The deficit view is shared by many other disciplines, including linguistics, sociology, education, disability studies, and anthropology, which all try to characterize atypical individuals in terms of their observed deficiencies, dysfunctions, difficulties, challenges, and limitations4,5,6,7. This deficit view has been criticized because it stigmatizes both atypical behaviors and the people showing them, with potentially severe negative personal and social consequences5,8,9,10,11. In the field of anthropology, Taylor and colleagues12 coined the term complementary cognition to point out that human cognitive evolution is likely to have resulted in individuals specialized in different but complementary neurocognitive search strategies (i.e., exploratory or exploitative activities). In so doing, human evolution has created a balance in individual neurocognitive specialization to enable an efficient adaptation to guarantee the survival of the human species12. Somewhat along these lines, researchers in the field of autism research have called for a shift from deficit-based to abilities-focused approaches to counteract the over-pathologization of human differences5,13. However, a systematic theoretical framework to provide the needed guidance for such a shift is lacking, so that the societal problems of the deficit view remain much better defined than possible alternatives.
Implications of neural competition
Here we would like to suggest a possible avenue towards such a theoretical framework. An avenue that does not deny deficits or at least underperformance as compared to some standards, but that also considers the possible positive potential that such deficits or challenges might point to. Pretty much like large shadows imply a vital source of light, mental weaknesses might point to mental potential, at least in many cases, even though this potential would often need to be identified and systematically developed. Our current approach was stimulated by our recent considerations on the downsides of cognitive enhancement14, and it can be viewed as the flipside of these considerations. One of the two principles that our previous article was based on is the principle of neural competition. It refers to the fact that the human brain is capacity-limited15,16,17 and that one of its essential characteristics is that neurons and neural networks compete for the representation and processing of environmental and internal information18,19,20, as indicated in Fig. 1a. If so, strengthening one particular function or system or representational space through cognitive enhancement training would be expected to impair other processes or systems or representational spaces, as indicated in Fig. 1b. Indeed, studies employing transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), a noninvasive brain stimulation technique, have revealed a trade-off between enhanced and non-enhanced cognitive functions via the modulation of the cortical excitation/inhibition balance in the stimulated brain area21,22,23. Hence, stimulating the brain via tDCS to attain cognitive enhancement can increase one function but at the expense of another one14. In line with this idea, it has been shown that cognitive enhancement and impairment can be obtained within the same stimulation protocol24: Stimulating the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) impairs learning while increasing automaticity for the learned material24 and stimulating the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) facilitates learning while hampering automaticity24. Given that enhancement studies commonly focus on the to-be-enhanced function, these impairments are likely to go unnoticed, and we suggested spending more attention on possible costs of enhancement14.
Interestingly, however, this view has a flipside that is indicated in Fig. 1c. Namely, if a particular function or system or representational space is less well-functioning, less well-developed in a given individual, as is suspected from neurologically and psychiatrically atypical individuals, this function/system/representational space should be a less potent competitor in the human brain. If thus, other functions/system/representational spaces are facing less competition, they either should have taken the opportunity to develop more efficiently than they would have with stronger neural competition, or they should at least have the potential to develop further. Evidence supporting this expectation is available from various lines of neurocognitive research, like obvious from the following three examples.
First, studies on blind individuals have indicated enhanced potential in auditory, tactile, and other kinds of perception, presumably as a direct consequence of the lack of use of what in sighted individuals are considered “visual” areas through/for visual perception25,26,27. To counterbalance the loss of a sense, the human brain generates or builds up corticocortical or subcorticocortical connections between the deprived and the intact senses28,29, suggesting that cross-modal plasticity is an adaptive process that enhances the remaining senses in blind people30. The visual cortex of blind people often exhibits a functional and structural reorganization, as indicated by activation of “visual areas” during odor detection, categorization, and discrimination31,32 and tactile perception33. Findings of this sort point to extensive cross-modal plasticity25,26,27, with regions of blind people’s visual becoming increasingly responsive to input from other sensory channels. Consistent with this idea, the stimulation of the visual cortex via transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in blind participants did not evoke phosphenes but elicited tactile sensations34. That is, lack of competition from the visual modality for representational space in the visual cortex creates the potential to enhance processing through other sensory modalities. Indeed, compensatory plasticity, in many cases, leads to the development of supranormal skills when using one of the remaining senses, such as improved tactile grating orientation and better pitch discrimination in blind people compared to sighted controls26.
Second, studies on phantom limb syndrome show that amputees still experience sensations from the amputated limb35. Similar to blindness, these cases reflect a cortical reorganization36,37,38, which makes neural representational spaces that had previously been used by the now amputated limb available for the representations of other body parts, commonly those with adjacent representations in somatosensory and motor maps39,40. Consistent with neuroimaging evidence, a TMS study, indexing cortical mapping before and after upper limb amputation, showed that the neighboring areas took over the deafferented zone41. Consequently, the amputee might attribute signals resulting from movements of these body parts as indicating movement of the amputated limb, which violates expectations and is often experienced as pain. Indeed, it has been shown that the degree of the somatotopic shift from the lip map to the deafferented hand map predicts the severity of phantom pain42. That is, when the brain’s primary sensorimotor cortex no longer gets inputs from the amputated hand, signals from the lips begin to take over that area. Such a maladaptive reorganization of the sensorimotor cortex is likely to cause pain in the phantom limb, as absence of inhibitory activity in the sensory-cortical feedback pathways triggers continued efferent motor cortical commands as a result of enhanced cortical excitability43.
Third, ocular dominance has been shown to result from the competition between neurons and neural networks to represent and process environmental and internal information44,45,46. For instance, the dominance of one eye over the other was a direct function of the amount of light stimulating the dominant eye in birds44. Further, an individual increase of right-eye dominance substantially enhanced the grain–grit discrimination success in pigeons by 10%, suggesting that high discrimination accuracy is mainly related to right-eye dominance45. This suggests that neural competition during neurobiological development is driven by the active use of the competing systems, with more use of one system reducing the functioning of others.
A cognitive-potential approach
The principle of neural competition suggests that dysfunctional neural systems are likely to be weaker competitors in the neural competition, which might be beneficial for competitors. Of course, one could imagine scenarios in which no beneficiary exists, perhaps because the cortical development of a natural beneficiary was completed before competition from the less efficient system was sufficiently reduced, probably because the dysfunctional nature of the less efficient system created more disturbing “cortical noise” than the benefit from reduced competition could have helped, or because the extra capacity or representational space offered by the less efficient system was of no practical use for any other system. Hence, we do not claim that the low efficiency of one system must lead to more efficiency in another—all we claim is that this is a possibility. Moreover, it may well be that low efficiency in one system has already boosted the functionality of another. For instance, damage to the left cortical hemisphere and the language-related areas it houses in most adults relatively early in the ontogenetic development leads to a relatively efficient take-over from the developing right hemisphere, which can lead to the entirely unimpaired acquisition of language47. In this case, the brain may already have exploited available resources to an optimal degree, so further optimization attempts might very well lead to overall impairments. For instance, the principle of neural competition is not unlikely to rely on changes in neurotransmitter levels19, which are known to relate to cognitive performance in a nonmonotonic, often curvilinear (inverted-U) relationship48, so that further pushing a system or function towards better functioning might actually impair performance.
More interesting for our approach are cases in which the potential that the lower efficiency of one system might provide has not yet been already identified and actively used by other systems. Substantial and systematically guided training might be necessary to use the full potential of unused cortical capacities. Accordingly, our cognitive-potential approach calls for two successive steps that fully exploiting the cognitive potential of individuals must entail. Before describing these two steps in turn, we would like to emphasize that developing possible potential may not be without risk. On the one hand, the situation sketched in Fig. 1 might represent the endpoint of some already completed ontogenetic process, which would imply that the degree of dysfunctionality of the dysfunctional system is relatively permanent. In this case, attempts to develop remaining potentials of other systems would not be risky. On the other hand, however, the process leading to the dysfunctionality might be still underway. If so, developing the potential of other systems might increase the competition with the dysfunctional system and further impair its functionality. Obviously, in these cases attempts to maintain the present level of (dys)functionality of the impaired system would have absolute priority over other cognitive-enhancement strategies.
Step 1: Identifying beneficiaries of selective dysfunction
As a first step, it is important to assess the individual potential of individuals with neural systems that are considered dysfunctional to some degree. This requires the identification of those neural systems that might have benefited or that might still benefit from the lesser neural competition from the dysfunctional system. Even though there is no established theoretical framework that could provide a list of possible candidates, the available evidence suggests at least three criteria that are likely to provide a promising search template. First, studies indicating reorganization in amputees suggest that neuroanatomical proximity can be an important cue36,39,40. Hence, representational spaces or functions closest to a dysfunctional system might be suspected to have taken over parts of the neural capacity that a more functional system would have occupied. Second, studies on blind individuals and on the neural development in pigeons26,44,45 suggest that functional alternatives can benefit from lesser competition: impaired functioning of one sensory modality is likely to benefit other modalities, impaired functioning of one symmetrical system, like the eye or a cortical hemisphere, is likely to benefit the other one. Third, evidence on the antagonistic nature of some neural systems suggests that weaknesses of one competitor might benefit its natural competitor. For instance, behavioral and neurocognitive studies have suggested that dopaminergic systems in prefrontal cortex might interact antagonistically with dopaminergic systems in the striatum to bias information processing towards persistence (high selectivity, focus on currently relevant information only) or flexibility (integrative, parallel processing), respectively49,50,51. Among other things, this suggests that dysfunctions related to prefrontal cortex might impair cognitive functions relying on this area but promote functions relying on the striatum—i.e., prefrontal dysfunction might promote flexibility. There is indeed some evidence in favor of this prediction. In individuals diagnosed with OCD and in individuals diagnosed with ADHD, who both suffer from an unbalance of dopaminergic systems in prefrontal cortex and in the striatum52,53, some cognitive functions have been reported to be impaired as compared with healthy matched controls, whereas others are apparently improved. We suggest that this paradox can be resolved if OCD and ADHD are not considered independent categories but as pointers to the two opposite poles of a common dimension, very much along the lines of the antagonistic persistence-flexibility dimension54,55. The potential to disentangle what is signal and what is noise represents the basis for a successful balance between the antagonistic poles of the persistence-flexibility dimension. During information processing, the level of a desired signal to the level of background neuronal noise, also called signal-to-noise ratio (SNR)56,57,58,59,60, is the most conceivable neural candidate underpinning the antagonistic persistence-flexibility dimension, also referred to as the metacontrol hypothesis of cognitive control54,55: less noise (i.e., high SNR) implies a more stable cognitive state61,62,63, while more noise (i.e., low SNR) might generate more behavioral variability64,65, implying a flexible cognitive state. Following these lines of reasoning, the trade-off between a functional and a dysfunctional system depends on the SNR: high SNR might support a more stable cognitive state (i.e., cognitive persistence), but at the costs of variable cognitive state (i.e., cognitive flexibility), and the opposite holds for low SNR.
Indeed, OCD has been considered an impairment in cognitive flexibility (Gruner and Pittenger, 2017), which fits with the evidence of an altered SNR as indexed by sensorimotor gating66,67 and with the well-known phenomenology of repetitive behavior with rigid rituals and diminished behavioral flexibility68. At the same time, individuals diagnosed with OCD were reported to outperform healthy controls in tasks requiring a focused state69 or in tasks requiring the selective reactivation of a previously inhibited mental set70. Hence, OCD seems to be characterized by atypically bad performance in flexibility-heavy tasks and atypically good performance in persistence-heavy tasks. Conversely, individuals diagnosed with ADHD display low SNR as measured by “1/f noise”, index of scale-free neural activity in EEG, which resulted to be increased after methylphenidate use57, and deficits in tasks requiring a focused cognitive state, such as sustained attention and vigilance71, but they outperform healthy controls in divergent thinking, which requires the generation of many different ideas72, in exploratory foraging patterns73, and in the implicit learning of an artificial grammar74. Hence, ADHD seems to be characterized by atypically bad performance in persistence-heavy tasks and atypically good performance in flexibility-heavy tasks.
It is important to consider that the available findings might not generalize to each single individual. For instance, particular genetic predispositions might prevent or hamper the occupation of underused cortical capacities for other purposes, or the cortical organization might be such that antagonistic relationships between systems might not have emerged as in more typical brains. Accordingly, the identification of potential can certainly be guided by available findings, which often rely on sample means, but eventually needs to be tailored to each individual.
Step 2: Training to the limit
Once the potential has been identified, the question will be how to make use of it. One important moderator for this question will be individual differences. Obviously, some individuals might have identified their corresponding potentials already and are already experienced in making optimal use of it. Others may have focused on their deficits only and may not have spent any efforts on developing their potential. Even others may not even have a particular potential, for reasons as mentioned in the previous section. Accordingly, the second step needs to be based on individual assessments of a given person’s potential. Given that the degree to which a given individual has already exploited her potential will often be hard to determine objectively, we recommend to apply the logic of the testing-the-limits research approach proposed by Baltes and colleagues to study performance in the elderly75,76,77. According to this approach, simply comparing cognitive performance across particular groups that are assumed to differ makes little sense, because the causes underlying possible significant differences remain uncertain: it might be a lack of capacity, a lack of using the capacity, or both. To determine true capacity limitations, it is not essential to know what the current spontaneous performance is but, rather, what the optimal performance can be. In other words, the approach is interested in potential. To assess true potential, training methods need to be identified to optimize the use of this potential, so to see which levels of performance can be reached after this training. Hence, in line with this testing-the-limits approach, we call for “selective optimization with compensation”75,76,77,78,79 which assumes that the best way to maintain high levels of cognitive performance is to focus and restrict the training on the potentially enhanced domain of functioning unaffected by the diagnosed condition. With respect to our OCD-ADHD example, individuals diagnosed with OCD should be selectively trained in persistence while individuals diagnosed with ADHD should be selectively trained in flexibility. Note that this approach suggests the exact opposite of what can be considered the more intuitive and widely used therapeutic strategy: while intuition would suggest training people in what they are bad at, our approach suggests training people in what they could become good at. Strengthening potential rather than trying to repair weaknesses is not only likely to be more efficient, but it can also be expected to be more motivating for the diagnosed individuals and to foster their self-respect—the lack of which is indeed a common problem in atypical individuals80,81,82,83,84.
Outlook
Standard accounts of mental health are based on a “deficit view” solely focusing on cognitive impairments associated with psychiatric conditions. This view is known to lead to personal discouragement, lack of self-respect, and societal stigmatization, which often further increase the problems diagnosed individuals are facing. The “deficit view” has been challenged also in the field of anthropology by the idea of the evolution of complementary cognition, suggesting that successful adaptation arises from the cooperation of individual members who are neurocognitively specialized in different but complementary neurocognitive search strategies12. According to complementary cognition, once a certain threshold has been reached, the only efficient way to increase brain capacity is via specialization. As a result of the trade-off between a functional and a dysfunctional complementary system, the specialization in one system comes at the cost of an impairment in the complementary system12. Based on the principle of neural competition, in line with the idea of the evolution of complementary cognition, we suggest an alternative to the “deficit view”: rather than focusing on deficits, we should focus on the possible cognitive potential that selective dysfunctions might bring with them. Our approach is based on two steps: the identification of the potential (i.e., of neural systems that might have benefited from reduced competition already or that might benefit after specific training) and the development of corresponding training methods, using the testing-the-limits approach. Counterintuitively, we thus suggest to target training not on the impaired function but, rather, on the function that might have benefitted or that may still benefit from the lesser neural competition of the dysfunctional system. This approach of identifying potential mental gains and their possible magnification is likely to promote educational achievement and the development of self-esteem in diagnosed individuals.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
References
Bugental, J. F. T. The third force in psychology. J. Humanist. Psychol. 4, 19–26 (1964).
Maslow, A. H. A theory of human motivation. Psychol. Rev. 50, 370–396 (1943).
Resnick, S., Warmoth, A. & Serlin, I. A. The humanistic psychology and positive psychology connection: implications for psychotherapy. J. Humanist. Psychol. 41, 73–101 (2001).
Bernstein, B. A public language: some sociological implications of a linguistic form. 1959. Br. J. Sociol. 61, 53–69 (2010).
Dinishak, J. The deficit view and its critics. Disabil. Stud. Q. 36, No 4 (2016). This paper, using the case of autism, discusses the criticisms raised to the deficit view identifying its theoretical flaws.
Mason, M. The deficit hypothesis revisited. Educ. Stud. 12, 279–289 (1986).
Saco-Pollitt, C., Pollitt, E. & Greenfield, D. The cumulative deficit hypothesis in the light of cross-cultural evidence. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 8, 75–97 (1985).
Akhtar, N. & Jaswal, V. K. Deficit or difference? Interpreting diverse developmental paths: an introduction to the special section. Dev. Psychol. 49, 1–3 (2013).
Astle, D. E. & Fletcher-Watson, S. Beyond the core-deficit hypothesis in developmental disorders. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 29, 431–437 (2020). This paper proposes that, in order to move forward from the deficit view, among others, well-powered studies are necessary with inclusive samples using methods that capture complexity such as structural equation modeling.
Lervåg, A. Editorial: is there a core deficit in specific learning disabilities? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 62, 677–679 (2021).
Runswick-Cole, K. ‘Us’ and ‘them’: the limits and possibilities of a ‘politics of neurodiversity’ in neoliberal times. Disabil. Soc. 29, 1117–1129 (2014). This paper suggests the importance of a “politics of neurodiversity” based on the idea that “neurodiverse” population should be considered as a political grouping the same way as class, gender, sexuality or race.
Taylor, H., Fernandes, B. & Wraight, S. The evolution of complementary cognition: humans cooperatively adapt and evolve through a system of collective cognitive search. Camb. Archaeol. J. 1–17 (2021) https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774321000329.
Doyle, N. Neurodiversity at work: a biopsychosocial model and the impact on working adults. Br. Med. Bull. 135, 108–125 (2020).
Colzato, L. S., Hommel, B. & Beste, C. The downsides of cognitive enhancement. Neuroscientist 27, 322–330 (2021). This article raises the idea that only a theory-driven approach will be able to account not only for cognitive benefits but also for potential metal costs caused by cognitive enhancement techniques.
Buzsáki, G., Logothetis, N. & Singer, W. Scaling brain size, keeping timing: evolutionary preservation of brain rhythms. Neuron 80, 751–764 (2013).
Laughlin, S. B. & Sejnowski, T. J. Communication in neuronal networks. Science 301, 1870–1874 (2003).
Marois, R. & Ivanoff, J. Capacity limits of information processing in the brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 296–305 (2005).
Cisek, P. A sensorimotor alternative to coding is possible. Behav. Brain Sci. 42, e222 (2019).
Mysore, S. P. & Kothari, N. B. Mechanisms of competitive selection: a canonical neural circuit framework. eLife 9, e51473 (2020).
Wang, X.-J. Decision making in recurrent. Neuronal Circuits Neuron 60, 215–234 (2008).
Krause, B., Márquez-Ruiz, J. & Kadosh, R. C. The effect of transcranial direct current stimulation: a role for cortical excitation/inhibition balance? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 602 (2013).
Krause, B., Dresler, M., Looi, C. Y., Sarkar, A. & Cohen Kadosh, R. Neuroenhancement of high-level cognition: evidence for homeostatic constraints of non-invasive brain stimulation. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 3, 388–395 (2019).
Krause, B. & Cohen Kadosh, R. Not all brains are created equal: the relevance of individual differences in responsiveness to transcranial electrical stimulation. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8, 25 (2014).
Iuculano, T. & Kadosh, R. C. The mental cost of cognitive enhancement. J. Neurosci. 33, 4482–4486 (2013).
de Borst, A. W. & de Gelder, B. Mental imagery follows similar cortical reorganization as perception: intra-modal and cross-modal plasticity in congenitally blind. Cereb. Cortex 29, 2859–2875 (2019).
Kupers, R. & Ptito, M. Compensatory plasticity and cross-modal reorganization following early visual deprivation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 41, 36–52 (2014).
van der Heijden, K. et al. Reorganization of sound location processing in the auditory cortex of blind humans. Cereb. Cortex 30, 1103–1116 (2020).
Collignon, O., Champoux, F., Voss, P. & Lepore, F. Sensory rehabilitation in the plastic brain. Prog. Brain Res. 191, 211–231 (2011).
Klinge, C., Eippert, F., Röder, B. & Büchel, C. Corticocortical connections mediate primary visual cortex responses to auditory stimulation in the blind. J. Neurosci. 30, 12798–12805 (2010).
Mowad, T. G. et al. Compensatory cross-modal plasticity persists after sight restoration. Front. Neurosci. 14, 291 (2020).
Kupers, R. et al. Neural correlates of olfactory processing in congenital blindness. Neuropsychologia 49, 2037–2044 (2011).
Renier, L. et al. Right occipital cortex activation correlates with superior odor processing performance in the early blind. PLoS ONE 8, e71907 (2013).
Sathian, K. & Stilla, R. Cross-modal plasticity of tactile perception in blindness. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 28, 271–281 (2010).
Kupers, R. et al. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the visual cortex induces somatotopically organized qualia in blind subjects. PNAS 103, 13256–13260 (2006).
Flor, H. Phantom-limb pain: characteristics, causes, and treatment. Lancet Neurol. 1, 182–189 (2002).
Flor, H. et al. Phantom-limb pain as a perceptual correlate of cortical reorganization following arm amputation. Nature 375, 482–484 (1995).
Petrus, E., Dembling, S., Usdin, T., Isaac, J. T. R. & Koretsky, A. P. Circuit-specific plasticity of callosal inputs underlies cortical takeover. J. Neurosci. 40, 7714–7723 (2020).
Yanagisawa, T. et al. Induced sensorimotor brain plasticity controls pain in phantom limb patients. Nat. Commun. 7, 13209 (2016).
Chen, R., Cohen, L. G. & Hallett, M. Nervous system reorganization following injury. Neuroscience 111, 761–773 (2002).
Mackert, B.-M., Sappok, T., Grüsser, S., Flor, H. & Curio, G. The eloquence of silent cortex: analysis of afferent input to deafferented cortex in arm amputees. Neuroreport 14, 409–412 (2003).
Pascual-Leone, A., Peris, M., Tormos, J. M., Pascual, A. P. & Catalá, M. D. Reorganization of human cortical motor output maps following traumatic forearm amputation. Neuroreport 7, 2068–2070 (1996).
Lotze, M., Flor, H., Grodd, W., Larbig, W. & Birbaumer, N. Phantom movements and pain An fMRI study in upper limb amputees. Brain 124, 2268–2277 (2001).
Ziemann, U., Hallett, M. & Cohen, L. G. Mechanisms of deafferentation-induced plasticity in human motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 18, 7000–7007 (1998).
Güntürkün, O. Lateralization of visually controlled behavior in pigeons. Physiol. Behav. 34, 575–577 (1985).
Güntürkün, O. et al. Asymmetry pays: visual lateralization improves discrimination success in pigeons. Curr. Biol. 10, 1079–1081 (2000).
Salinas, K. J., Huh, C. Y. L., Zeitoun, J. H. & Gandhi, S. P. Functional differentiation of mouse visual cortical areas depends upon early binocular experience. J. Neurosci. 41, 1470–1488 (2021).
Krägeloh-Mann, I., Lidzba, K., Pavlova, M. A., Wilke, M. & Staudt, M. Plasticity during early brain development is determined by ontogenetic potential. Neuropediatrics 48, 66–71 (2017).
Cools, R. & D’Esposito, M. Inverted-U-shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol. Psychiatry 69, e113–e125 (2011).
Beste, C., Moll, C. K. E., Pötter-Nerger, M. & Münchau, A. Striatal microstructure and its relevance for cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 747–751 (2018).
Fallon, S. J., van der Schaaf, M. E., Ter Huurne, N. & Cools, R. The neurocognitive cost of enhancing cognition with methylphenidate: improved distractor resistance but impaired updating. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 29, 652–663 (2017).
van Schouwenburg, M., Aarts, E. & Cools, R. Dopaminergic modulation of cognitive control: distinct roles for the prefrontal cortex and the basal ganglia. Curr. Pharm. Des. 16, 2026–2032 (2010).
Brem, S., Grünblatt, E., Drechsler, R., Riederer, P. & Walitza, S. The neurobiological link between OCD and ADHD. Atten. Defic. Hyperact. Disord. 6, 175–202 (2014).
van den Heuvel, O. A. et al. Brain circuitry of compulsivity. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 26, 810–827 (2016).
Hommel, B. Chapter two—Between persistence and flexibility: the Yin and Yang of action control. in Advances in Motivation Science, Vol. 2 (ed. Elliot, A. J.) 33–67 (Elsevier, 2015).
Hommel, B. & Colzato, L. S. The social transmission of metacontrol policies: mechanisms underlying the interpersonal transfer of persistence and flexibility. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 81, 43–58 (2017). This paper introduces the metacontrol hypothesis which suggests that cognitive control arises from the balance between cognitive persistence and cognitive flexibility.
Adelhöfer, N., Mückschel, M., Teufert, B., Ziemssen, T. & Beste, C. Anodal tDCS affects neuromodulatory effects of the norepinephrine system on superior frontal theta activity during response inhibition. Brain Struct. Funct. 224, 1291–1300 (2019).
Pertermann, M., Bluschke, A., Roessner, V. & Beste, C. The modulation of neural noise underlies the effectiveness of methylphenidate treatment in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 4, 743–750 (2019).
Pertermann, M., Mückschel, M., Adelhöfer, N., Ziemssen, T. & Beste, C. On the interrelation of 1/f neural noise and norepinephrine system activity during motor response inhibition. J. Neurophysiol. 121, 1633–1643 (2019).
Salinas, E. & Thier, P. Gain modulation: a major computational principle of the central nervous system. Neuron 27, 15–21 (2000).
Servan-Schreiber, D., Printz, H. & Cohen, J. D. A network model of catecholamine effects: gain, signal-to-noise ratio, and behavior. Science 249, 892–895 (1990).
Aston-Jones, G. & Cohen, J. D. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28, 403–450 (2005).
Bensmann, W., Roessner, V., Stock, A.-K. & Beste, C. Catecholaminergic modulation of conflict control depends on the source of conflicts. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 21, 901–909 (2018).
Bensmann, W., Zink, N., Roessner, V., Stock, A.-K. & Beste, C. Catecholaminergic effects on inhibitory control depend on the interplay of prior task experience and working memory demands. J. Psychopharmacol. 33, 678–687 (2019).
Gureckis, T. M. & Love, B. C. Learning in noise: dynamic decision-making in a variable environment. J. Math. Psychol. 53, 180–193 (2009).
Gruner, P. & Pittenger, C. Cognitive inflexibility in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuroscience 345, 243–255 (2017).
Ahmari, S. E., Risbrough, V. B., Geyer, M. A. & Simpson, H. B. Impaired sensorimotor gating in unmedicated adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 1216–1223 (2012).
Russo, M. et al. Obsessive-compulsive disorder: a ‘sensory-motor’ problem? Int. J. Psychophysiol. 92, 74–78 (2014).
Benzina, N., Mondragon, S. L., Ouarti, N., Mallet, L. & Burguiere, E. Translational approach to study flexibility as an endophenotype of obsessive compulsive disorders. Eur. Psychiatry 29, 546–546 (2014).
Lucke, I. M. et al. Continuous performance test in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder and tic disorders: the role of sustained attention. CNS Spectr. 20, 479–489 (2015).
Wolff, N., Giller, F., Buse, J., Roessner, V. & Beste, C. When repetitive mental sets increase cognitive flexibility in adolescent obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 59, 1024–1032 (2018).
Barkley, R. A. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol. Bull. 121, 65–94 (1997).
Hoogman, M., Stolte, M., Baas, M. & Kroesbergen, E. Creativity and ADHD: a review of behavioral studies, the effect of psychostimulants and neural underpinnings. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 119, 66–85 (2020). This review article points out that ADHD is associated with enhanced divergent thinking, but not convergent thinking.
Van den Driessche, C., Chevrier, F., Cleeremans, A. & Sackur, J. Lower attentional skills predict increased exploratory foraging patterns. Sci. Rep. 9, 10948 (2019).
Rosas, R. et al. ADHD children outperform normal children in an artificial grammar implicit learning task: ERP and RT evidence. Conscious Cogn. 19, 341–351 (2010).
Baltes, P. B. Testing the limits of the ontogenetic sources of talent and excellence. Behav. Brain Sci. 21, 407–408 (1998).
Baltes, P. B. & Kliegl, R. Further testing of limits of cognitive plasticity: negative age differences in a mnemonic skill are robust. Dev. Psychol. 28, 121–125 (1992).
Kliegl, R., Smith, J. & Baltes, P. B. Testing-the-limits and the study of adult age differences in cognitive plasticity of a mnemonic skill. Dev. Psychol. 25, 247–256 (1989). This study shows that the use of testing-the-limits research approach has the goal to reach the maximum levels of performance potential (developmental reserve capacity).
Küster, O. C. et al. Cognitive reserve in Alzheimer’s dementia: diagnostic accuracy of a testing-the-limits paradigm. J. Alzheimers Dis. 52, 519–528 (2016).
Navarro, E. & Calero, M. D. Cognitive plasticity in young-old adults and old-old adults and its relationship with successful aging. Geriatrics 3, 76 (2018).
Ehntholt, K. A., Salkovskis, P. M. & Rimes, K. A. Obsessive-compulsive disorder, anxiety disorders, and self-esteem: an exploratory study. Behav. Res. Ther. 37, 771–781 (1999).
Lau, Y. W. et al. Stigma resistance and its association with internalised stigma and psychosocial outcomes among psychiatric outpatients. Psychiatry Res. 257, 72–78 (2017).
Packer, L. E. & Pruitt, S. K. Challenging Kids, Challenged Teachers: Teaching Students with Tourette’s, Bipolar Disorder, Executive Dysfunction, OCD, ADHD, and More. xiii, 329 (Woodbine House, 2010).
Silvestri, P. R., Baglioni, V., Cardona, F. & Cavanna, A. E. Self-concept and self-esteem in patients with chronic tic disorders: a systematic literature review. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 22, 749–756 (2018).
Slomkowski, C., Klein, R. G. & Mannuzza, S. Is self-esteem an important outcome in hyperactive children? J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 23, 303–315 (1995).
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by Grants from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, SFB 940, SFB TRR 265 and FOR 2698.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
L.S.C., C.B. and B.H. contributed equally to writing and finalizing the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests. C.B. is an Editorial Board Member for Communications Biology, but was not involved in the editorial review of, nor the decision to publish this article.
Peer review
Peer review information
Communications Biology thanks Nancy Doyle and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary handling editors: Karli Montague-Cardoso and Christina Karlsson Rosenthal.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Colzato, L.S., Beste, C. & Hommel, B. Focusing on cognitive potential as the bright side of mental atypicality. Commun Biol 5, 188 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-022-03126-0
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-022-03126-0
This article is cited by
-
Neurophysiological avenues to better conceptualizing adaptive cognition
Communications Biology (2024)
-
No convincing evidence for the independence of persistence and flexibility
Nature Reviews Psychology (2024)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.