Introduction

Stratification of social status has important implications for behavior and emotional health in both animals and humans, with individuals lower in social standing generally experiencing worse outcomes than those with higher standing1,2,3,4. Chronic psychosocial stress and agonistic interactions experienced by those in subordinate roles likely underlies this strong correlation between social rank and health5,6. In laboratory mice, several different paradigms have been used to examine differences in neural systems and substrates that may correlate with the assignment of animals as either dominant (e.g. alpha animals) or subordinate (e.g. beta, gamma, delta animals) in social dominance ranking constructs7,8,9. These include behavioral assays such as the tube test, territory urine marking, and the warm spot test where induced conflict and ensuing dominance among animals determines rank order10,11,12. One of the most commonly used assays to identify social dominance rank is through the observation and scoring of agonistic behavior of group-housed animals in a homecage setting10,12,13,14. This method relies on the natural, self-organizing dominance hierarchies that can develop in many strains of group-housed cages of mice as animals engage in periodic offensive (e.g. aggressive) and defensive (e.g. submissive) behaviors through the course of standard laboratory animal housing14. Agonistic behaviors can be elicited spontaneously, at the onset of the diurnal active period (e.g. lights-off), or after a simple perturbation such as a change into a new unfamiliar primary enclosure (e.g. cage change)15. Once established, hierarches are generally stable with especially high maintenance of rank over time for cagemates identified as either most- or least-dominant among group housed animals7,16,17. Using this methodology, several studies implicate medial prefrontal cortex- and nucleus accumbens-dependent neural circuits that influence social dominance rank12,18,19,20. Further, alterations in neuropeptides, hormones, genes and inflammatory biomarkers that are differentially expressed in dominant versus subordinate cagemates have been identified14,15,21,22,23,24. Of interest to our own work studying stress peptides such as corticotropin releasing factor (CRF)25,26,27, previous studies have shown alterations in CRF mRNA levels in dominant versus subordinate animals14,24, suggesting a relationship between social dominance and this important regulator of stress and social behavior28,29,30.

Along with CRF, another neuropeptide—pituitary adenylate cyclase-activating polypeptide (PACAP)—has been implicated in brain adaptations to stress31,32,33 and regulation of CRF and stress hormones such as corticosterone (CORT) through the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis34,35,36. PACAP belongs to the secretin/glucagon superfamily of peptides and exists in two biologically active forms (as 38- and 27-amino acid peptides) found in peripheral tissues and brain37. PACAP-38 is the predominant form in the brain and shares identical amino acid sequence homology in species including mice, rats, sheep, and humans, indicating strong evolutionary conservation38. The high density of PACAPergic afferents and PACAP-type-I receptors (PAC1Rs) within the extended amygdala (e.g. central nucleus of the amygdala [CeA] and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis [BNST])39,40,41 suggests that this peptide plays a role in modulating neural activity related to psychological stress as well as experience-dependent learning42,43,44. We have demonstrated that PACAP influences AMPA receptor-dependent synaptic transmission in the CeA45, which receives direct PACAPergic innervation from the parabrachial nucleus46 and where PACAP may be endogenously released in response to pain. Further, we found that exogenously administered PACAP can affect expression of fear-related behavior and CORT levels in a fear-conditioning paradigm47,48, cause persistent alterations in sleep architecture49, and impact motivation and social behavior50. The current study was designed to examine differences in PACAP expression within the extended amygdala in cagemates of mice ranked according to social dominance interactions. We also examined the impact of social dominance rank on acoustic startle response, a behavioral test that is sensitive to stress and emotional state and is influenced by PACAP51,52, and tested the mice to ensure that differences in motor capabilities could not explain the formation of the hierarchies. Given that preclinical paradigms have been useful tools to help understand the neurobiology of emotional disorders that may arise from repeated psychological or physical stressors, understanding how PACAP systems influence social dominance behavior may have face and construct validity for studying these illnesses as they appear in humans4,18,53.

Methods and materials

Animals

Male C57BL/6 mice bred and housed in the McLean Hospital vivarium were used. To generate the experimental animals, timed breeding pairs were established to allow pooling of male offspring into group cages of 4 mice/cage at approximately 3 weeks of age. At the time of group housing, the body weight of each mouse was determined and four animals of similar weight were housed together in each polycarbonate cage (28 × 18.5 × 12.5 cm) with laboratory bedding (Alpha Chip; Northeastern Products Co.) and one square (5 × 5 cm) of nesting material (Nestlet; Ancare). All mice were maintained on 12/12 h light dark cycles (lights on at 700 h) and food and water were provided ad libitum. Weekly cage changes to place animals into a clean homecage with new bedding and nesting material occurred on the same day each week always between 1000 and 1300 h. The timeline of all procedures is illustrated in Fig. 1. All animal procedures were approved by McLean Hospital’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (Office of Laboratory Animal Welfare Assurance number A3685-01) in accordance with the National Institute of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (8th Edition). The study was performed and results reported in accordance with ARRIVE guidelines.

Figure 1
figure 1

Timeline of the experimental design used in this study, including timepoints of various procedures used following the Week 12 (W12) observation of home-cage social dominance interactions; see Supplemental Fig. 1 for ethogram of offensive/defensive behaviors that were scored to determine rank. PACAP Pituitary adenylate cyclase-activating polypeptide, IHC immunohistochemistry, CORT corticosterone, DOB date of birth.

Social interaction measurement and determination of social dominance hierarchy.

Starting at 10 weeks of age, each the 4 group-housed mice were weighed and tails were marked with distinctive markings to individually identify each animal prior to cage change. Mice were observed for the presence of agonistic behaviors (see below) at this time point and cages where this behavior was not present were omitted from further study. At 12 weeks of age, immediately following the introduction of each group of mice into a new clean homecage, social interactions among the group-housed mice were video recorded for 15 min. A digital camera was placed above the center of the cage and used to record behavior for scoring of offensive (aggressive) and defensive (submissive) behavior using previously described methods to identify dominant and submissive mice13,14,54. A description of the types of agonistic behaviors used to rank animals according to an overall score in a matrix of offensive versus defensive behaviors is illustrated in Supplemental Fig. S1A. By assigning a “point” to each mouse in the cage for every offensive and defensive behavior it engaged in over the 15 min period, they could be ranked as Dominant or Submissive (e.g. having the highest score for offensive behaviors and lowest score for defensive behaviors or vice versa, respectively; see Supplemental Fig. S1B). As described in Horii et al.14, we identified a third rank of mice as “Intermediate” describing mice with offensive/defensive scores in-between those identified as dominant and submissive. However, unlike the study of Horii et al.14 which excluded this group from analyses, we retained these intermediates for our analyses as a third comparator group comprising the two animals in the cage of four that were ranked neither as Dominant nor Submissive. A separate cohort of 2 cages of mice (n = 4/cage) were solely used to track and demonstrate the stability of identified social dominance rank over time by recording agonistic behavior (15 min sessions) following cage change for an additional 3 weeks (Week 13–15; Supplemental Fig. S1C).

PACAP immunohistochemistry (IHC)

Four days after the week 12 cage change and recording of social interaction to determine social dominance rank, cages of mice were overdosed with sodium pentobarbital (130 mg/kg; IP) and upon loss of toe-pinch reflex they were perfused intracardially with 0.9% saline (25 ml) followed by 2% paraformaldehyde, 0.05% gluteraldehyde, and 0.2% picric acid in 0.1 M PBS (75 ml) pH7.4. The execution of successful perfusion and fixation, which is critical for having confidence in the reproducibility of IHC assays, was evaluated by confirmation of three visual observations: tail curl within 1 min following introduction of the fixative solution, rigor in the head, legs and arms following perfusion, and the presence of yellow coloring (provided by the picric acid in the fixative) in the brain during removal. As a result, two mice in the Intermediate group were judged to have had poor perfusions and were removed from analysis. Brains were removed, stored for 3 d in a 30% sucrose/0.1 M PBS solution, and then cut serially in 30 µm coronal sections with every-other section from the BNST, and every-third section from the CeA placed in a 2-ml borosilicate glass vial (12 sections/vial for both brain areas) for processing of PACAP immunohistochemistry. All incubations were done on a rocker platform at room temperature. Sections were incubated in 0.6% hydrogen peroxide in 0.1 M PBS for 30 min followed by 3 washes (5 min) in 0.1 M PBS. Sections were preincubated in antibody medium (2% normal donkey serum, 1% bovine serum albumin, 00.3% Triton-X-100 in 0.1 M PBS) for 2 h followed by incubation for 24 h with a rabbit polyclonal antibody against PACAP (1:1000; Penninsula Labs; #T-4473) diluted in antibody medium. The sections were washed with PBS and incubated for 1 h with a donkey anti-rabbit biotinylated secondary antibody (1:400; minimum species cross-reactivity; Jackson ImmunoResearch). The sections were washed with PBS and incubated for 30 min with the avidin–biotin complex (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame CA) and then incubated for 5 min in 3,3’-diaminobenzidine (DAB)/H2O2 (Sigma Fast; Sigma) as a chromogen for visualization of PACAP peptide through the BNST and CeA. Processed sections were mounted on microscope slides and coverslipped with Permount (Fisher Scientific, Pittsburgh, PA) and observed with a Zeiss Axioscope 2 (Zeiss, Oberkochen, Germany). Still frame images of three coronal sections from each brain were captured approximating BNST and CeA regions from the mouse brain corresponding to images 52, 53, and 54 (BNSTov) and 69, 70 and 71 (CeAL) from the Allen mouse brain atlas (mouse.brain-map.org)98,99. As the CeAL has a much longer rostro-caudal extent than the BNSTov, we focused on caudal sections of the CeAL, which has denser PACAP expression than rostral sections (see Supplemental Fig. S2). PACAP immunoreactivity in each brain area was quantified by unbiased measurements of the optical density (O.D.) of pixels using ImageJ software for Macintosh (Scion Corp, Fredrick, MD, USA); ImageJ is a public domain, JAVA-based image processing program developed by the National Institutes of Health (NIH). Captured sections were switched to gray scale for threshold adjustments (scale 0–141 units normalized to background values of white matter) and mean values of O.D. were calculated from 0.1 mm2 regions with a fixed template placed within each of the brain areas (see Supplemental Fig. S2 for representative demarcation of areas measured). Some alternate sections through the BNST and CeA were also labelled for PACAP using immunofluorescence according to previously published methods48 for illustration purposes and to further delineate the boundaries of the BNSTov and CeAL (Supplemental Fig. S2) but were not used for quantification of PACAP in these regions.

Corticosterone assay

Serum CORT levels were measured following cage change and the 15 min session for recording of agonistic behavior in a separate cohort of group-housed 12 week-old mice to determine the impact of agonistic behavior on this stress-dependent hormone in animals of different social rank. A separate cohort of animals was used as a control group for the stress of cage change and accompanying agonistic behaviors by collecting blood in undisturbed cages of mice 48 h after the 12 week cage change and recording of social interaction used to determine social dominance rank in this cohort. Given diurnal variations in mouse CORT levels, blood sampling was conducted between 1000 and 1100 h, when CORT levels are stably low55,56. Mice were overdosed with sodium pentobarbital (130 mg/kg; IP) and upon loss of toe-pinch reflex, the chest cavity was opened. A 0.5 cc Insulin syringe (U-100 syringe; Becton-Dickson, Franklin Lakes, NJ) with a 28 gauge needle (0.5 in length) was used to draw blood from the right ventricle of the heart. This procedure is rapid (< 3 min) and previous work indicates that it is unlikely that anesthesia significantly impacts CORT levels on this time scale57 although others have found that longer exposures to pentobarbital anesthesia can affect CORT responses in rats58. Blood was transferred to a sterile 2 ml serum blood collection tube (BD Vacutainer; Becton-Dickson, Franklin Lakes, NJ) and allowed to clot at room temperature for 30 min before centrifugation for 10 min at 3000 rpm. Serum was removed, aliquoted, and stored at − 80 °C until assayed by ELISA following the manufacturer’s directions for quantitative determination of CORT levels in rat/mouse serum (Alpco Diagnostics, Salem NH). All samples were loaded onto a single plate in duplicate wells and assayed using a BioTek Synergy HT microplate reader to compare samples against a standard curve of known mouse CORT concentrations. The sensitivity of the assay was 6.1 ng/ml.

Rotorod

To explore whether motor capacities differ among groups—a factor that could potentially be involved in how the social hierarchies form—mice were tested on the accelerating rotorod (Ugo Basile; RotaRod model 7750) one day after the 12 week cage change and recording of social interaction among the group-housed mice. The four mice from each cage were placed on the rotorod cylinders at the same time starting at a slow rotational speed (4 rpm) which gradually increases over 2 min to a maximum of 40 rpm. Each lane on the device is equipped with individual timers to record latency-to-fall with a maximum trial length of 3 min. Mice are tested three times with a 5 min interval between tests with an overall latency-to-fall score averaged across the three trials for each mouse. Following this test, mice were perfused as described above for analysis of PACAP IHC.

Acoustic startle

A separate cohort of mice was tested for acoustic startle one day after the 12 week cage change and recording of social interaction among the group-housed mice. Testing was conducted in 4 identical mouse startle cages consisting of 6 × 6 × 5 cm Plexiglas cages with metal rod flooring attached to a load-cell platform. Both the startle cages and platform were located within a 69 × 36 × 42 cm fan-ventilated sound-attenuating chamber (Med Associates, Georgia, VT). Cage movement resulted in a displacement of a transducer in the platform where the resultant voltage was amplified and digitized on a scale of 0 to ± 2000 arbitrary units by an analog-to-digital converter card interfaced to a personal computer (PC). Startle amplitude was proportional to the amount of cage movement and defined as the maximum peak-to-peak voltage that occurred during the first 200 ms after the onset of the startle stimulus. Constant wide-band background noise (60 dB; 10–20 kHz) and 50 ms startle stimuli (1–32 kHz white noise, 5 ms rise/decay) were generated by an audio stimulator (Med Associates) and delivered through speakers located 7 cm behind the startle cage. The calibration, presentation, and sequencing of all stimuli were under the control of the PC using specially designed software (Med Associates). For testing, mice were placed in the startle chambers and given a 5 min acclimation period followed by presentation of two habituating startle stimuli (100 dB, 30 s interstimulus interval; ISI). Mice were then presented with 30 startle stimuli at three different intensities (95, 100, 105 dB); the 10 trials at each intensity were presented in a semirandom order with a 30 s ISI. Startle amplitude data were expressed as the mean averaged across the 10 trials for each of the three startle-eliciting intensities.

Statistical analyses

Data are presented as means ± standard error (SEM) for mice ranked as Dominant, Intermediate, or Submissive based on their agonistic behavior scores following the Week-12 cage change. The impact of social dominance rank on PACAP expression in the BNSTov and CeAL and on blood CORT levels was analyzed using a two-way ANOVA with rank (Dominant, Intermediate, Submissive) as a between-subjects factor. Additional comparisons of social rank on PACAP and CORT were further carried out as appropriate with one-way ANOVAs with subsequent post-hoc comparisons using Sheffe’s test. Body weight and rotorod performance was analyzed using separate independent-measures one-way ANOVAs. Startle data were analyzed using a two-way ANOVA with rank (Dominant, Intermediate, Submissive) as a between-subjects factor and startle intensity (95, 100, 105 dB) as a within-subjects factor.

Results

PACAP IHC, body weight and rotorod performance

A total of seven cages of mice were scored for agonistic behaviors at 12 weeks of age to rank animals into Dominant, Intermediate and Submissive groups and process their brains for PACAP IHC in the extended amygdala. Figure 2 shows representative brain sections from animals of different social rank illustrating PACAP IHC in the BNSTov and CeAL subdivisions which were quantification by optical density measurements of 0.1 mm2 regions in each of these areas. A two-way ANOVA of PACAP expression across groups showed a main effect of brain area (F1,23 = 20.8, P < 0.0001), indicating generally higher levels of PACAP expression in the BNSTov versus the CeAL, and social dominance group (F2,23 = 3.53, P < 0.05). The brain area x social dominance group interaction was not significant. One-way ANOVA of PACAP expression in the BNSTov showed a significant overall effect (F2,23 = 5.27, P < 0.05); individual pairwise comparisons showed significantly higher levels of PACAP in the BNSTov in Submissive versus Dominant and Intermediate mice (P < 0.05 both comparisons).

Figure 2
figure 2

Representative coronal sections through the BNST (AD) and CeA (E,F) showing PACAP IHC expression from Dominant (A,E), Intermediate (B,C) and Submissive (D,F) ranked mice. Number in lower right corner of each panel reflects the measured optical density (O.D.) value of PACAP expression in 0.1 mm2 regions in the BNSTov and CeAL from each section (see Supplemental Fig. 2 for anatomical location of within the BNSTov and CeA where O.D. was measured). (G) Average O.D. values of PACAP expression in the BNSTov and CeAL for each group. Mice in the Submissive group showed significantly higher levels PACAP expression in the BNSTov compared to the other two groups; PACAP expression was not significantly different in the CeAL between groups. Bar graph data are shown as mean ± s.e.m. CPu Caudate-putamen, BLA basolateral amygdala. Scale bars = 100 mm. *P < 0.05.

Figure 3A illustrates average body weight data from animals in the different social dominance groups analyzed for PACAP IHC in the BNSTov and CeA shown in Fig. 2. A one-way ANOVA across groups revealed no significant main effect. Although there was a trend for Submissive animals to have lower average weight than Dominant or Intermediate animals, post-hoc comparisons with Sheffe’s test revealed no significant pairwise differences in body weight. We further analyzed these data to look for correlations between PACAP expression in the BNSTov—which was significantly higher in Submissive animals compared to the other two social dominance groups—and body weight; Fig. 3B illustrates this relationship. There was a trend for animals with lower body weight to have higher BNSTov PACAP expression levels (r2 = 0.34), but this correlation was not significant. Figure 3C illustrates rotorod data from these same animals in the different social dominance groups. A one-way ANOVA across groups revealed no significant main effect of latency to fall, the dependent measure of motor coordination in this test.

Figure 3
figure 3

(A) Average body weight data from mice in the different social dominance groups at 12 weeks of age. (B) Relationship between body weight data and BNSTov PACAP expression (O.D.; optical density value from 0.1 mm2 region) for mice in the different social dominance groups; there was an overall trend for mice with lower body weight to have higher BNSTov PACAP expression levels (r2 = 0.34), but this correlation was not significant. (C) Average rotorod data from these same mice in the different social dominance groups. Bar graph data are shown as mean ± s.e.m.

Serum CORT levels after agonistic interactions

A total of six cages of mice were used to examine blood serum CORT levels in Dominant, Intermediate and Submissive groups sacrificed immediately after mice engaged in agonistic interactions and behavior was recorded for identifying Dominant, Intermediate and Submissive mice following cage change at 12 weeks (Cage Change condition). An additional cohort of five cages of mice was used as controls to examine blood serum CORT levels in Dominant, Intermediate and Submissive groups identified at 12 weeks but were then undisturbed (i.e. no cage change) prior to sacrifice (48 h after 12 week cage change; Control condition). CORT data for each of the social dominance groups from both conditions are shown in Fig. 4. A between-subjects two-way ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of condition (F1,38 = 13.43, P < 0.005) and significant main effect of social dominance group (F2,38 = 6.33, P < 0.005). The condition x social dominance group interaction was not significant. Simple effects tests revealed that CORT levels were significantly higher after cage change in Dominant (F1,38 = 7.78, P < 0.05) and Intermediate (F1,38 = 6.1, P < 0.05) groups compared to respective control condition. CORT levels were not significantly different between the conditions for Submissive group mice indicating no effect of cage change/social interaction stress on CORT for this rank of mice. Separate one-way ANOVAs for each condition across social dominance groups revealed a significant main effect of CORT level in the cage change condition (F2,21 = 8.29, P < 0.005). Subsequent pairwise comparisons revealed significantly lower levels of CORT in Submissive mice compared to Dominant and Intermediate mice (P < 0.005 both comparisons).

Figure 4
figure 4

Serum corticosterone (CORT) levels from mice in the different social dominance groups measured under different cage change conditions. Groups of mice were sacrificed either immediately after the home cage change and 15 min recording session to identify Dominant, Intermediate and Submissive mice (Cage Change condition) or 48 h after this timepoint to measure CORT from undisturbed cages of mice (Control condition). Mice in the Dominant and Intermediate groups, but not Submissive group, showed significantly elevated CORT levels relative to controls (#P < 0.05) after cage change and social dominance interactions. After cage change, Submissive group mice showed significantly lower levels of CORT compared to other two groups (**P < 0.005 versus both groups) following this session. Bar graph data are shown as mean ± s.e.m.

Social dominance and acoustic startle

Figure 5 shows acoustic startle data from 7 cages of mice ranked as Dominant, Intermediate and Submissive. A two-way ANOVA of startle amplitude for each of the startle eliciting intensities (95, 100 105 dB) across groups showed only a main effect of startle intensity (F2,50 = 309.2, P < 0.0001) as startle amplitude is increased with more intense startle stimuli.

Figure 5
figure 5

Acoustic startle response from mice in the different social dominance groups across startle intensity showing no differences between groups in this measure of sensorimotor reactivity. Bar graph data are shown as mean ± s.e.m.

Discussion

The current study was designed to determine if there are relationships between expression of the stress-related neuropeptide PACAP in the extended amygdala and the social strata of group-housed C57BL/6 mice that occurs in the natural establishment of social dominance hierarchies among conspecific cagemates. Our hypothesis was that due to chronic, intermittent aggressive social interactions and naturalistic stress that animals experience over the nine weeks of group-housing, as mice self-organize into the social dominance hierarchies, there will be changes in stress-related factors that correspond with dominance rank. Social dominance rankings in the current study were based on the frequency of agonistic behaviors (e.g. aggressive and submissive behaviors) among group-housed mice following introduction into a new, unfamiliar homecage. Through these weekly perturbations in housing, mice re-establish and reveal stable dominance hierarchies that we presumed would have an underlying neurobiological signature in brain areas that subserve stress, threat detection, and adaptive responding, namely, the extended amygdala comprising the PACAP-dense regions of the BNST and CeA48,59,60. Accordingly, we found that expression of PACAP was highest in the BNSTov of the least-dominant (i.e., Submissive) mice compared to the most-dominant (i.e., Dominant) mice or mice ranked as neither most- or least-dominant (i.e., Intermediates). Dominant and Intermediate group mice showed similar levels of PACAP in the BNSTov. Interestingly, there were no differences in PACAP expression in the CeAL between any of the social dominance groups. Further, we examined if there was any impact of social dominance hierarchy on other dependent measures that are known to be influenced by stress and PACAP including body weight, CORT levels, and the acoustic startle response. Although Submissive mice tended to have lower body weight, there were no significant differences between groups. We found that cage change and accompanying bouts of agonistic behaviors stimulated CORT release in both Dominant- and Intermediate-ranked mice, but not Submissive mice, compared to an undisturbed control group where social dominance interactions were not initiated. Despite these changes in PACAP and CORT, we did not observe any differences between groups in sensorimotor reactivity of the acoustic startle response.

The finding of elevated PACAP expression in the BNST of Submissive group mice—cagemates that are the recipients of the greatest amount of aggressive behavior and respond with the greatest amount of defensive behavior—is consistent with previous reports showing that repeated bouts of stress increase both mRNA transcript and protein levels of PACAP in the BNST of rodents36,61,62. Unlike chronic stress, however, the effects of a single exposure to stress on PACAP levels in the BNST are mixed, as acute restraint stress has been shown to have no effect on PACAP mRNA levels63, whereas exposure to a series of footshocks significantly increases the density of PACAP peptide expression52. Differences in the quantitative and qualitative experience of the stress is likely to account for differential effects on adaptive regulation of PACAP in the BNST. In the current study, noxious visceral stimuli (e.g., bites, anogenital sniffing, pins) received by Submissive group mice would presumably be transmitted to the dorsal vagal complex (DVC) and lateral parabrachial nucleus (LPBN), where PACAP neurons reside and send projections to the BNST and CeA46,64,65,66,67. Hence, adaptations within this ascending circuit as a consequence of the repeated physical stress of aggressive engagements could account for the upregulation of PACAP peptide in the BNST seen in the Submissive group mice. While the BNST is traditionally known as a node for the integration of exteroceptive and interoceptive input related to stress that can regulate autonomic, neuroendocrine and behavioral phenotypes resembling anxiety states68,69,70, recent reports indicate that it also plays a role in coordinating social behavior71. We cannot determine from the current study as to the functional significance of increased PACAP expression in the BNST of Submissive group mice, but speculate that it may represent a neuroadaptation that provides some survival advantage to a low-ranking animal in the social dominance hierarchy. Along these lines, previous studies have shown that intra-BNST infusions PACAP elevates CORT levels in the blood36 and enhances acoustic startle reactivity51,61, which could be advantageous in helping animals increase arousal, attention and escape responding72,73. In contrast, other reports have shown that intra-BNST infusions of PACAP produce anorexia and loss of body weight which, at face value, would appear as maladaptive with potentially deleterious effects for the animal74. To better understand how our own findings of increased PACAP expression in the BNST in Submissive group mice fit in the context of these previous reports, we further examined the impact of homecage social dominance hierarchy on body weight, blood CORT levels, and acoustic startle (see below).

Our finding that PACAP expression levels in the CeA were not different between any of the social dominance groups, including Submissive group mice, aligns with a previous report showing that chronic stress (intermittent exposure to variable stressor) upregulates PACAP mRNA signal in the BNST, but not the CeA61. Further, others have shown that withdrawal following chronic intermittent ethanol dependence increases PACAP levels in the BNST but not the CeA75. Hence, there is an apparent dissociation between PACAP upregulation in the BNST versus the CeA which may be related to the qualitative nature of the type of stress experienced by animals. Despite that fact that both the BNST and CeA have been conceptualized as an interconnected neural continuum with similar afferent and efferent connections, including PACAPergic innervation, a dissociation between functional roles for these brain areas has been proposed76. According to this construct, the BNST plays a predominant role in sustained threat monitoring and may subserve behavioral phenotypes akin to anxiety (sustained fear) whereas the CeA is preferentially sensitive to phasic threats, mediating behaviors akin to acute fear70,77. In this context, and in relation to homecage social dominance hierarchies where subordinate animals may experience chronic intermittent stress as social rank is continually challenged and reinforced (i.e. sustained threat), we speculate that experience-dependent activation of BNST circuits, including PACAPergic pathways, may predominate over CeA circuits to transform the brains of animals of the lowest social dominance rank.

We also examined the impact of homecage social dominance rank on body weight given reports that, in general, group-housed subordinate animals have lower body weight than dominant animals5,10,14,78. While we did see a trend for Submissive group mice to have lower body weight than Dominant or Intermediate group mice, these differences were not significant. Further, we examined the relationship between BNSTov PACAP expression and body weight across all social dominance groups; there was a trend for animals with lower body weight to have higher PACAP expression in the BNSTov, but this correlation was not significant. Given that intra-BNST infusion of PACAP has been shown to induce anorexia and body weight loss74, we might have expected to see significantly lower body weight in Submissive group mice given our finding that these animals have increased BNSTov PACAP expression. However, a relatively high concentration of intra-BNST PACAP (1.0 μg) was required to induce significant weight loss in the prior report74 and it is unclear how comparable that is to the increase in endogenous PACAP in the BNSTov we observed in Submissive group mice such that it could have significantly impacted food intake and corresponding body weight in this group. Rotorod performance across all three dominance group was not significantly different indicating no impact of social dominance rank on motor coordination, as expected, based on previous reports assessing general motor performance in dominant versus subordinate animals11,14,15. We also examined the impact of social dominance hierarchy on the acoustic startle response, a sensorimotor reflex that is sensitive to repeated stress and anxiety-like states31,51 and is significantly increased by intra-BNST infusion of either CRF or PACAP51,52,61,79. However, we did not observe any differences in acoustic startle response between any of the social dominance groups.

Assessment of blood CORT is frequently used as an assay to explore the physiological effects of social dominance as it reflects activity of the descending hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and has traditionally been thought of as a biomarker for stress and healthy functioning of this important feedback system80. Published data, however, are mixed as to the relationship between CORT levels and social dominance rank as some reports indicate subordinate animals have higher levels of CORT than dominant animals, whereas others report the opposite, or no change at all54,78. Differences between findings are likely related to the use of different strains, methods for dominance assessment, and stress-history of the animals11. In the current study we found that in undisturbed cages of mice, there were no differences in the resting level of CORT between the social dominance groups. However, following a cage change and the ensuing social dominance engagements, both the Dominant and Intermediate group show a significant elevation in CORT compared to the control condition, whereas Submissive group mice show similar CORT levels between the conditions. This suggests that Submissive group mice exhibit a blunted CORT response compared to the other two dominance groups in response to the stress of cage change and engagement in homecage social dominance behaviors. A related finding has been reported in rats showing that a subgroup of subordinate-ranked animals failed to mount the appropriate surge in CORT levels following exposure to a stressor5. This subgroup of “non-responders” made up approximately 40% of rats ranked as subordinate, suggesting that subordination can have profound effects on the adaptive function of the neuroendocrine system responsible for an organism’s ability to respond to stressful conditions5. In a more recent report using an assessment to rank group-housed male mice (4/cage) as dominant or subordinate similar to ours (e.g. scoring of homecage agonistic behaviors), the mice ranked lowest in the dominance hierarchy had lower serum CORT levels than dominant mice, although the effects of acute stressor on CORT was not assessed to determine if there is a blunted response in subordinate mice14.

Given our observation of increased PACAP expression in the BNSTov of Submissive group mice, it is tempting to propose that there may be a causal relationship between this neuroanatomical finding and the finding of reduced CORT levels in this same subgroup. While it is known that the BNST plays a role in descending control of the HPA axis response to stress81, the heaviest projections from the BNST to the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN; the first relay in the descending HPA axis) appear to originate from other subdivisions of the BNST, including the anterior ventral (av) and dorsomedial (dm) BNST, with only a weak projection from the BNSTov81,82,83,84,85,86,87. However, there is evidence that CRF neurons in the BNSTov do project directly to the PVN in mice and rats88. This projection is likely relevant because PACAPergic afferents in the BNSTov make direct contact with CRF neurons in this area89 and presumably influence their activity. Further, because CRF neurons in the BNST are known to also co-express GABA90, it is possible that PACAP in the BNSTov modulates inhibitory tone in the PVN through this same projection91. Hence, enhanced PACAPergic innervation of the BNSTov, as seen in Submissive group mice, might affect PVN activity via enhanced CRF/GABA release and potentially serve to inhibit activity of the HPA axis to blunt CORT release in response to stress. Such a mechanism, however, would need to be reconciled with seemingly contradictory findings that (1) direct intra-BNST infusion of PACAP can significantly increase CORT levels36 and (2) elevated CORT in response to social defeat stress is attenuated in PACAP knockout mice92. In addition, it has been reported that another source of PACAP innervation of the BNST is from the PVN itself, suggesting the potential for reciprocal control of BNST-PVN feedback circuits under the control of PACAP67. Our current studies provide the basis for future mechanistic studies to characterize these circuits and their roles in social dominance.

Clearly, the multiple parallel and intersecting systems that underlie adaptive control of stress and may be recruited in the development of social dominance is more complex than can be addressed here. However, we believe our current findings contribute new insight into the study of social dominance and the influence of stress peptides, such as PACAP, on these networks9,53,93. Of interest to us is how these systems may become pathologically dysregulated as a consequence of sustained threat and chronic intermittent stress like that experienced by subordinates in the context of social dominance hierarchies. Bullying in children and teenagers is one such manifestation of social dominance that has demonstrable adverse effects on mental health in subordinated youth, with greater incidence of depression, anxiety, self-harm and suicidality in victims94,95. Hence, developing preclinical models with improved face and construct validity to study the impact of social dominance may lead to transformative advances in our understanding of human psychiatric conditions associated with social dominance and subordination, and enable studies in research animals to better predict outcomes in humans. While we did not uncover a behavioral phenotype (e.g. changes in acoustic startle) in the current study, we did find two significant effects that were idiosyncratic to submissive animals: (1) increased expression of PACAP peptide in the BNSTov and (2) normal resting levels of CORT but a blunted response following mild stress instigated by agonistic interactions. The later finding is interesting as blunted cortisol reactivity in response to stress has been associated with major depression, including in children96,97. Taken together, these observations made in low dominance-ranking animals may have translational value in an effort to elucidate pathologies associated with the adverse consequences of social dominance in humans, and potentially identify better treatments for disorders that arise from these experiences.