Introduction

Social memory is critical for individuals to adaptively navigate a social world. Social memory in humans has been a topic of study since the 1950s1,2, and in rodents, experimental exploration spans back to the 1970s3,4. Rodents naturally demonstrate the ability to use social memory to navigate their surroundings. Wild rodents outside of the laboratory form complex social groups that display the ability to discriminate between dozens of individuals. For example, wild house mice interact with dozens of individuals during a month’s time and form social structures to control territory and selectively mate with other high-ranking individuals5. Similarly, rats outside the laboratory form complex and stable social structures that require distinguishing between large number of individuals for long periods of time6,7. Laboratory and wild mice both demonstrate social recognition of kin8. Furthermore, in captivity, both wild mice9,10 and laboratory mice11,12 form social hierarchies. In a large vivarium of 30 individuals, mice formed two distinct dominance subnetworks that were stable across weeks, showcasing their ability to show social preferences and distinguish among many familiar individuals13. Laboratory mice and rats make up the bulk of all neuroscience research and thus the bulk of social memory research14. Individuals of both species live in groups and show long-term relationships that matter for their behavior in their natural environment, thus making these long-term relationships ethologically relevant (Table 1). These rich long-term relationships suggest that the identity of individuals and types of relationships is encoded in the brain. Understanding how the brain encodes identity and distinct types of relationships is important for our fundamental understanding of social cognition. In addition, studying diverse and long-term social memories has strong clinical implications given that rodents are used as models to generate treatments for social and memory deficits associated with neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders15,16.

Table 1 Definitions relevant to this study.

Within the laboratory, a few paradigms are predominantly used to test social memory. In each paradigm there is a subject, the animal whose behavior is being measured, and a social stimulus, the animal being presented to the subject. Rats and mice have been shown to have both sociability preferences (preferring social interaction to no social interaction) and social novelty preferences (preferring interaction with a novel vs. familiar animal)17,18. Behavior such as less investigation time across repeated exposure to the same social stimulus or more investigation of a novel versus a familiar social stimulus suggests social recognition of the subject to the stimulus. The paradigms that have mainly been used, with various modifications, include: the habituation-dishabituation paradigm, the five-trial social memory test (Fig. 1a), and the three-chamber social assay (Fig. 1b), all of which have been described in detail elsewhere18,19,20. Time exploring the social stimuli decreases as familiarization increases in the habituation-dishabituation paradigm and time in the chamber with the novel stimulus is higher in the three-chamber assay. More recently, given the limitations of the three-chamber assay21, there have been smaller chambers used to present a familiar and novel stimulus reducing the need for spatial navigation22,22,23,24 (Fig. 1c). In addition, a handful of studies have created operant-based tasks (Fig. 1d) to study social memories using two familiar individuals17,25,26, as well as variations in the social recognition paradigm in order to present more social stimuli (Fig. 1e) and measure behavior in a more data-driven way27.

Figure 1
figure 1

Social memory paradigms. (a) Schematic showing standard trial-based social memory assays. (b) Schematic of classic 3-chamber social memory assay. (c) Modified chamber assay apparatus. (d) Operant task where subject learns to associate familiar social stimuli with a non-social outcome; social discrimination is judged based on subject’s learned behavioral output. (e) Sociobox apparatus designed to test social memory against multiple present familiar social stimuli. Left shows phase one for familiarization and right is social recognition phase.

Social interactions between individuals vary depending on social history and other biological factors (Table 2). For example, male mice and rats have been shown to have higher levels of baseline social stimulus investigation as compared to females28,29, but female rats have been shown to retain social recognition for longer periods of time30. Rodents can exhibit social recognition up to 3–7 days later after a two-minute exposure to a social stimulus31,32,33 while, paternal male mice can remember their offspring 5–6 weeks after separation34. Considering the many social relationships that mice and rats have and the wide use of rodent models, we quantified the types of memories studied and how they are being tested in the neuroscience field. To help shape future social memory research, we systematically quantified gaps that exist in the current literature and outline factors and suggestions to fill these gaps in future social memory studies.

Table 2 Evidence of social investigation metrics changing with social and biological variables.

Methods

We searched four databases: Web of Science, Embase, PubMed, and PsycINFO, all on January 14, 2022. We focused our search on the abstract text, as such the scope of this review is limited by the terminology used by the primary research authors in the abstract (Fig. 2—search criteria). Our goal was to review those articles where social memory was at the center of the goal of the study, rather than those where social recognition experiments served as controls. Studies were selected by several parameters: (1) the term “social” was used to describe the type of memory, discrimination or recognition being investigated in the abstract, (2) we excluded purely behavioral studies by requiring the use of the term “neuro” in the abstract. While this limited our search, we believe using these boundaries allowed us to focus on studies pertaining to the neural mechanisms of social memory without further author bias to categorize other memory studies. Each paper was reviewed to confirm whether the article satisfied all criteria to be included in this review (Fig. 2). After each paper was reviewed for criteria satisfaction, 672 qualified articles remained. Each article was analyzed and quantified for various methodological variables. EndNote Online and Zotero were used as reference managers. Detailed results can be found in the supplemental materials.

Figure 2
figure 2

Methodological flowchart. Databases searched with number (n) of articles found per site alongside key terms and search criteria, number of duplicates, and list of exclusion criteria with the number (n) of articles removed for each item.

To satisfy our criteria for testing social memory, an article must have included a behavioral experiment consisting of more than one exposure to the same social stimulus or social odor (e.g. bedding or urine from a conspecific). Additionally, the authors must have reported some behavioral metric that indicated social recognition. Any significant behavioral differences due to familiar or novel social stimuli exposure qualified. Sniffing time, exploration time, interaction time etc. were all recorded as social investigation time. If a subject was exposed multiple times to the same social stimulus but no behaviors were measured and compared across exposures to confirm recognition of an individual social stimulus by the subject, the experiment did not satisfy our criteria. Examples of paradigms that did not meet our social memory criteria were chronic social defeat studies (no behavioral metric indicating recognition) and social transmission of food preference (single exposure to a conspecific).

All experimental conditions used in an article were analyzed and a single article can be represented multiple times in a figure if it used different subjects, social stimuli, intertrial intervals, etc. If no ovariectomy procedures were mentioned, female subjects and social stimuli were assumed to be intact. If ovariectomies were mentioned, but no age, female social stimuli were assumed to be adults. Housing conditions were divided into four groups: chronic isolation, acute isolation, group-housed, or not specified. Chronic isolation was defined as being singly housed for longer than a week. Familiarity levels for familiar social stimuli were either categorized by the ethologically relevant relationship to the subject, e.g., littermate, or were defined as the total time of exposure prior to a test trial.

To substantiate that any quantified methodological variable was used significantly more than any other, a two-sample chi-square test was done for the two most used conditions such that the second most used condition was the observed frequency, and the most used condition was the expected frequency. Other and not specified are not homogenous groups and therefore were not used in any chi-square tests.

Results

While social memory research has increased, the subjects remain adult male mice

Social memory, and its underlying neural mechanisms, has been growing as a research topic. The number of yearly published papers more than doubled between 2017 and 2021 (Fig. 3). We next investigated for each of these articles, what type of subjects, social stimuli and social memories were tested.

Figure 3
figure 3

Social Memory Articles Published in the 2000's. Articles that satisfied our inclusion criteria and were included in this studied plotted by the year they were published.

We defined the subject as the experimental animal whose social memory is being tested by recording their behavior. We found mouse models were more popular than rat models with 60% of articles using mice and 41% using rats, and 1% of articles using both. Given the published work showing social behavior differences across strains of mice and rats23,35,36, we quantified which strains dominate social memory experiments. Mouse research widely used the inbred C57BL6 strain (C57BL6 vs C57BL6 mix, two sample chi-square, X2 = 492.97, n = 406, p = 3.22 × 10–109; Fig. 4a). Amongst the rat studies, two outbred strains, Wistar and Sprague–Dawley, were predominant (Fig. 4b). When it came to the age of the subjects, very few articles (n = 14 out of 672) used aged subjects (72 weeks and older) for social memory experiments (Fig. 4c). Although the sex gap in social memory research is decreasing (Fig. 4e), males still have significantly greater representation than females (female vs male, two sample chi-square, X2 = 2048.52, n = 672, p = 0; Fig. 4d). For housing conditions, group-housing has become the dominant practice since 2006 (Fig. 4g). Of the 169 articles utilizing chronic or acute isolation, 17% explicitly assessed the effects of social isolation on social memory. For the other studies, many used social isolation to increase social motivation, establish home-cage territory, or to allow surgical recovery. Despite the documented effects of social housing on many behavioral and neurophysiological variables37, an average of 18% of articles in the past decade did not report housing conditions for subjects (Fig. 4f).

Figure 4
figure 4

Subject Identity in social memory studies. (a) Number of articles using different strains of mice (n = 406 from 672 articles). Other strains for mice included: BALB/c, BTBR, A/J, C3H, FVB/NJ, ICR, OF1, NMRI, DBA, CF-1, and wild mice. C57BL/6 mix vs C57BL6 two sample chi-square, X2 = 492.96, p = 3.22 × 10–109. (b) Number of articles using different strains of rats (n = 275 from 672). Other strains for rats included Lewis, SHR, and WAG/Rjj. Sprague Dawley vs Wistar two sample chi-square, X2 = 110.53, p = 7.50 × 10–26. Note that the 1% articles that included both rats and mice and are represented in both (a) and (b). (c) Ages of subjects across all articles reviewed. Infant < 3 weeks, juvenile 3–7 weeks, adult > 7 & < 72 weeks, aged > 72 weeks. Juveniles vs adults two sample chi-square, X2 = 3,637.64, p = 0. (d) Pie chart showing the percentage of all articles (n = 672) from years 2000–2022 that use one, both or unspecified sex of mice or rats. (e) Percent of articles from years 2000–2021 (n = 665) that use one, both, or unspecified sexes of mice or rats. If no sexes were stated, we marked papers as “sex not specified”. (f) Number of articles from years 2000–2021 (n = 665) that use group-housing, acute isolation (over a day—less than a week), chronic isolation (over a week), or do not specify the housing history of the subjects. Chronic isolation vs group housed two sample chi-square, X2 = 310.09, p = 2.09 × 10–69. (g) Number of articles from 2000 to 2022 reporting each type of housing history.

When described, social stimuli tend to be young males

Social interaction and social memory are modified by the identities or behavioral state of both the subject and the social stimulus38,39,40. Therefore, it is important to consider what types of social stimuli have been used. Despite the potential effects that the identity of social stimulus can have on social interactions (Table 2), we found that most studies lacked information concerning social stimuli characteristics. The strain of social stimuli was often not reported (Fig. 5b, c) and unless clearly stated in the methods of a given article, we did not assume that the social stimulus was of the same strain as the subject. When information was reported, the most commonly used social stimulus was a male juvenile (Fig. 5a, e) with males being used considerably more than females (female vs male, two sample chi-square, X2 = 333.95, n = 672, p = 1.32 × 10–74; Fig. 4e). Of the adult female social stimuli, almost half were ovariectomized (Fig. 5f). We encountered a one article that used ovariectomized juvenile females41. Of note, it has been shown that estrogen disruptions in subjects, as is caused by ovariectomies, can cause disruptions in social memory42,43,44. As social interactions are dynamic, where the social stimulus can affect the behavior of the subject23, it may be possible that using ovariectomized social stimuli affects the social memory of the subject, but this remains unknown. The most common variable not reported was the housing condition of the social stimulus (Fig. 5d). Overall, we found that social stimuli characteristics are less reported than those of the subject.

Figure 5
figure 5

Social stimulus identities in social memory studies. (a) Age of social stimuli across all articles, adult vs juveniles two sample chi-square, X2 = 154.76, n = 672, p = 1.58 × 10–35. (b) For 406 mouse articles that included social stimuli, mouse strains that were used for social stimuli. Other mouse strains include BALB/c, BTBR, A/J, C3H, FVB/NJ, ICR, OF1, NMRI, DBA, and wild mice. Swiss vs C57BL/6 two sample chi-square, X2 = 239.51, p = 5.02 × 10–54. (c) For 279 rat articles that included social stimuli, rat strains that were used for social stimuli. Other rat strains include Lewis, Lister Hooded, WAG/Rij and F344. Sprague Dawley vs Wistar two sample chi-square, X2 = 83.64, p = 5.95 × 10–20. (d) Number of articles that used group-housing, acute isolation (over a day—less than a week), chronic isolation (over a week), or do not specify the housing history for social stimuli. Chronic isolation vs group housed two sample chi-square, X2 = 241.70, n = 672, p = 1.67 × 10–54. (e) Percent of all articles (n = 672) that use one, both, or unspecified sexes of mice or rats for social stimuli. (f) Of those articles that used female social stimuli (n = 220), percentage of those that used juvenile, ovariectomized, adult intact or unspecified age females.

Few studies measure long-term, ethologically relevant relationships

Levels of familiarity between the familiar social stimulus and subject being tested were typically five minutes or less (6–120 min vs 5 min or less, two sample chi-square, X2 = 230.68, n = 672, p = 4.25 × 10–52; Fig. 6a, b) with most subjects spending at most 2 h with the social stimulus prior to being tested for social recognition. Of the few articles (n = 25 out of 672) that tested longer-term relationships for social recognition, most studied social recognition of littermates (Fig. 6c). For parent offspring relationships, 3 studies tested an infant’s ability to recognize the odor or presence of their mother vs. a novel dam45,46,47. Yang et al., tested the offspring’s recognition of its mother in adulthood as well. These studies demonstrate that the capacity for social memory develops early in life and recruits the hippocampal CA2, demonstrating that the role of CA2 in social memory starts early in development and spans both short and long-term social memories. Additionally, by using a foster mother, Laham et al., confirms that offspring’s maternal preferences are not innate genetic preferences but require experience for social recognition. One study demonstrated the ability of a male parent to recognize its offspring after 5–6 weeks of separation34. Only one paper tested the memory of a female subject for its mate by measuring pregnancy blocking, a female’s ability to terminate pregnancy in the presence of a novel male48 (Fig. 6c). In this study the authors showed that the memory for the mate is dependent on oxytocin action, demonstrating some potential overlap with social memory mechanisms and pregnancy blocking. Studies that use ethologically relevant long-term relationships enrich our fundamental knowledge of social memory and could facilitate identifying mechanisms that are distinct for short vs long-term familiarity. Despite the importance of social hierarchies and of the effects of social rank in behavior and brain function, most social memory studies do not consider social rank. Given the literature of the increased attention subordinates give towards dominant animals49, it is likely that the neural mechanisms of long-term social memory are modulated by social rank to facilitate quick behavioral discrimination of social rank. Of all the articles that used group-housed adult rodents, only one article (Fig. 6c) studied the interplay between social rank and social memory. Cordero and Sandi found that stress potentiates long-term maintenance, or memory, of a social hierarchy50. Our results highlight the lack of inclusion and consideration in the past literature for long-term ethologically relevant social memory mechanisms.

Figure 6
figure 6

Types of social memories studied and how they were measured. (a) Number (No.) of articles that use various levels of familiarity. 6–120 min vs 5 min or less, two sample chi-square, X2 = 230.68, n = 672, p = 4.25 × 10–52. (b) Histogram showing all familiarity levels used from 6 to 120 min. (c) A breakdown of all the types of ethologically relevant relationships tested in a social memory paradigm in n = 25 articles. (d) Number of articles that use various intertrial interval (ITI) lengths; ITI being the time in between trials, both learning and test trials. 1–6 h vs 6–59 min two sample chi-square, X2 = 99.00, n = 672, p = 2.53 × 10–23. (e) Metrics cited across all articles. Investigation time refers to interaction time, sniffing time, and contact time while number of interactions refers to a unit of interaction bouts rather than a cumulative time metric; time in region of interest (ROI) includes time spent near or in a chamber or other target area. Time in ROI vs investigation time two sample chi-square, X2 = 3025.27, n = 672, p = 0.

Few studies investigate long-term social memories

In addition to quantifying the familiarity levels between subject and social stimulus, we quantified the length of the social memory at the time of testing. Some studies have demonstrated that the neural mechanisms of short vs long term social memory are distinct51,52. In addition, there is a vast literature demonstrating that for non-social memories the neural mechanisms and circuits underlying short- and long-term memories can differ53,54,55. Therefore, we asked whether social memory studies represented these equally. We found that most of the studies tested very short-term memories (see Table 1 for definitions). The intertrial intervals used (ITI; the time between learning, encoding, or familiarization with a social stimulus, and the memory test trial) were short. Short ITI’s and low levels of familiarity result in those experiments testing short-term memories. Less than 2% of articles tested social memories that had more than 24 h since encoding began, and almost half of the articles tested social memories shorter than an hour (Fig. 6d). Thus, for most neuroscience research probing social memory, only short-term memories of short-term familiarity have been investigated. In addition to the implications of this gap for our fundamental understanding of the brain, filling this gap is important for understanding how to treat brain diseases that affect long- and short-term memories differently, such as Alzheimer’s and other dementias56,57.

Social memory metrics focus on single variables

Investigation time generally decreases upon repeated exposures to a social stimulus and increases upon exposure to a novel social stimulus. We found that this change in investigation time is homogenously used as the readout of social memory. Of the 672 articles reviewed, only 2% quantified behaviors beyond time or frequency of investigation of the social stimulus (i.e., sniffing time, time spent in proximity, number of interactions etc.) as the behavioral metric to measure social memory (Fig. 6e). Two studies had specific additional interaction qualifications such as latency to approach or time spent following58,59. Quantifying and comparing the ways in which subjects interact with social stimuli could provide insight that total investigation time may not. For example, Netser et al., quantified a variety of behavioral metrics during a social discrimination paradigm using rats. Between novel and familiar social stimuli investigation, the most significant difference was found when analyzing long bouts of investigation (> 19 s) as opposed to the insignificant difference amongst short bouts of investigation (< 6 s). Four articles compared ultrasonic vocalizations across social stimulus exposures60,61,62,63, one quantified urine markings in response to a novel vs familiar stimulus64, and one measured the rate of pregnancy termination in the presence of an unfamiliar vs. a familiar mate48. Kavaliers measured the effects of familiarity with social odors on risk taking behavior and mate preference65,66. Finally, Cordero and Sandi used maintenance of social hierarchies through competition outcomes as a measure of social recognition.

Social recognition between two familiar conspecifics and social identity are understudied

The limitation in assays and behavioral metrics have led to the social memory field almost exclusively studying social memory via discrimination of a single familiar animal from a novel animal. We found only one study that measured social memory mechanisms during the discrimination of two familiar animals. This article tested the discrimination between two familiar stimuli by different behavioral outputs from the subject; van Wingerden and van den Bos used rate of risk-taking behavior to distinguish between a “trustworthy” vs “untrustworthy” stimulus (defined as conspecifics associated with different reward probabilities26). Since we performed our meta-analysis, a new study was published showcasing a novel go/no-go task where subject mice discriminate between two familiar mice to obtain a reward25. This design allowed the authors to study the neural dynamics of social identity encoding. We foresee that creative assays, such as these, will propel the field forward in our mechanistic understanding of social memory by dissociating mechanisms of novelty preferences from social identity encoding and recalling social memories of familiar individuals.

Discussion

Our results indicate that social memories studies in the past two decades are biased towards short-term memories of short-term familiarity. This bias has led to underrepresentation of long-term relationships that are relevant across animal models such as kin, cagemates, members of their hierarchies and mates. Our results also show that there is little research on the neural mechanisms of long-term social memories. This is of particular importance, given that neuropathologies that affect memories, such as Alzheimer’s, tend to disrupt short-term and long-term memory in different stages of the disease56,57. Understanding the circuits underlying short vs long-term social memory may design and inform therapies for distinct deficits. We recognize that the exact design of a study and which social factors are included as a variable will depend on the goal of the scientific study and more behavioral testing is not always better. The goal of the study must be considered to choose the experimental design, but awareness of gaps, biases and factors that affect social behavior will help optimize future experiments in social memory. Below we detail potential directions and metrics to increase our understanding of social memories and what information we can gain in future studies.

Incorporating long-term familiarity memories

Our results show that most social memory studies focus on short-term familiarity, with few studies involving long-term social memory. Using classical social recognition assays, limited exposure to social stimuli (15 min or less), results in social recognition that can last up to an hour in individually housed rodents19,67 and up to 7 days for group-housed rodents33. These studies suggest that it may take long-term familiarity to facilitate the long-term encoding of social identity. These differences in how long the social memories last based on exposure time suggest the possibility of differences in mechanisms for social memories of short- vs long-term familiarity. A large body of literature shows mechanistic differences between short- and long-term non-social memories54,68,69. There are also some reported differences in the neural mechanisms of short and long-term social memories51,52. Importantly, a social memory with short-term familiarity (e.g. 10 min) could be studied as a short or a long-term memory by simply varying the interval between the acquisition vs recall. On the other hand, long-term familiarity social memories, such as memories of cagemate identity, are long-term memories that get updated as new interactions occur. For improving our fundamental knowledge of the mechanisms of social memory, the field would benefit from future studies comparing the neural mechanisms of short vs long-term social memories, as well as the comparison of short vs long-term familiarity. Incorporating the use of social stimuli with long-term ethologically-relevant relationships can facilitate the study of long-term social memory. In addition, to combat the challenges of short-term familiarity memories being forgotten, operant-based assays17,25,26 could make social memories of individuals with short-term familiarity more salient by pairing them with rewarding or aversive outcomes.

Operant-based social memory assays to study social identity

Our results show that there is little work to investigate how social identity memories may be represented. Relying only on investigation time, the most common behavior metric used, limits research to novelty discrimination (e.g. discrimination between a novel vs familiar animal). If social recognition is not detected with a novelty preference-based assay, is there a failure to discern and/or prefer the novelty of an animal or a failure to recognize the familiarity of a familiar one? With classical novelty-based social memory assays it is not possible to distinguish between these two possibilities. Assays that do not rely on novelty preferences allow dissociating between individual recognition and novelty discrimination. In particular, operant-based assays allow studying behavioral discrimination between multiple familiar individuals and social identity encoding (see Table 3 and Fig. 1).

Table 3 Diverse social memory metrics and their potential benefits for future studies.

Additional metrics of social memory

A recent review discusses the limitations of the three-chamber social assays and a potential integrative, multimodal approach with simultaneous behavioral and physiological measurements at high temporal resolution21. In addition, recent studies have shown that short vs long bouts of social investigation are of relevance for social recognition for both mice and rats23,70. Both species have longer bouts of investigation with the novel stimuli, providing an additional metric beyond total investigation time. Our systematic review identified four studies that used ultrasonic vocalizations (USV) as a metric for social memory in female mice of different strains60,61,62,63. In these studies, USV rate decreased when interacting with familiar same-sex individuals. Another study identified in our systematic review used a data-driven approach to determining regions of interest in a social recognition test, and using this approach compared to the traditional user-defined region of interest improved the signal to noise in social recognition indices27. Using diverse social memory metrics could be important for identifying potential treatments for social deficits in mouse models for disease. It is possible that only some metrics may be differentially affected by disease states and thus those metrics could serve for studying potential rescue mechanisms. On the other hand, interventions that affect multiple metrics of social memory, rather than just one, could have more potential for translation. We summarize potential benefits of using additional behavioral metrics in Table 3.

Considering biological factors in subject and social stimuli that may affect social memory

Our data also demonstrates a chronic lack of reporting of information regarding social stimuli utilized, which poses challenges for reproducibility. The omission of these details prevents the understanding of any potential differences in behavior elicited by the specifics of social stimulus identity such as sex, strain, and housing. Recent studies have shown that social stimuli state and behavior affect subject behavior. Active behavior by the social stimulus is necessary for social recognition38, stimuli movement can either enhance or deter social investigation from the subject23, and even drug administration to a constrained stimuli can affect sociability of a freely moving subject71, all of which highlight the importance of the two-way interaction. These studies suggest that the behavioral state (isolation, stress, age, etc.) of the social stimulus could affect the behavior of the subject and potentially the social memory metrics.

Other important factors that can impact the ethological and biological relevance of social relationships are housing condition, sex, and age. Our data demonstrate that these factors are often not reported or considered, yet they can be crucial for interpretation of results as they affect social interactions in rodents (see Table 2). Our results also show that adult male subjects dominate the social memory literature. A recent study showed that the neural mechanisms of social recognition differ between adults and adolescent rats72, demonstrating the importance of studying social memory at different developmental stages. Although there is ample evidence that aging affects memory, only 2% of our surveyed articles studied social memory in aged rodents. Studying aging and how it impacts social memory mechanisms is relevant for translating basic research to treat dementia and other aging related memory problems in humans. In addition, studying both male and female animals is important for translating pre-clinical research since it has been shown that many brain disorders have different rates in women and men and symptomology can differ across sexes73. Lastly, we also found that very few studies considered social rank in their group housed animals. Given the reports of subordinates increased attention towards dominant animals49, the neural mechanisms of long-term social memory may be modulated by social rank to facilitate quick behavioral discrimination of social rank.

In summary, we have quantified the volume of social memory literature utilizing different methodologies, outlined how different social and biological factors affect social memory, and the limitations with which social memory has been measured. We hope to provide context for considerations for future research design to better understand the neural mechanisms of social memory.