Introduction

The human brain is the key organ in stress resilience and vulnerability and acts as a recipient and mediator of stress1. The period between early childhood and adolescence is considered one of the most stress-sensitive phases in human development2,3. Children in institutional care accumulate multiple biological and psychosocial risk factors towards developing adverse health outcomes, reduced well-being, and psychopathology in adulthood4,5,6. Furthermore, children placed in residential care are likely to experience high levels of childhood adversities and potential traumatic exposures7. Childhood adversities (CAs), such as abuse or neglect, are detrimental to neurodevelopment and increase the risk of developing mental disorders8,9,10. However, the relationship between CAs and long-term mental health outcomes is still not fully understood, especially regarding biological parameters beyond the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA-) axis11. For instance, brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) might be a promising biological candidate to investigate the association between CAs and neurodevelopment. Indeed, chronic, or severe early-life stress might adversely impact BDNF levels in the brain12,13.

BDNF

BDNF is one of the highly investigated neuronal growth factors and shows vigorous activity in brain areas necessary for learning, memory, mood, and sleep14,15. As a member of the neurotrophic family, BDNF’s role in neurogenesis is defined by differentiation, survival, and synaptic plasticity of neurons16,17,18. BDNF has a high affinity for the tropomyosin receptor kinase B (TrkB), which plays a crucial role in the maturation and regeneration of neurons14,19. In addition, BDNF-TrkB binding regulates several downstream pathways, including the protein phosphatidylinositol-3′OH-kinase (PI3K), which in turn activates the AKT-mTOR pathway impacting neuronal survival and apoptosis14,20,21. Since neurotrophic factors can cross the blood–brain barrier, serum BDNF concentration (sBDNF) can be detected in peripheral tissue and shows a correlation to brain BDNF levels22,23,24,25. Therefore, sBDNF may be seen as a justified indicator of neuronal BDNF levels26, providing the opportunity to study this promising biomarker.

Stress and BDNF

The potential clinical relevance of BDNF arises from the evidence of declining BDNF concentrations in numerous psychiatric disorders, aging, and neurodegenerative diseases14,15,27,28. Stress experiences could be a trigger in this association. In humans, stress exposure can lead to changes in BDNF expression and thus BDNF levels29. The major driving force in the neuroendocrine stress response is the HPA-axis, with its prominent signaling hormone cortisol30,31,32,33. The release of cortisol regulates response pathways in the body to react to challenging situations, including the secretion of BDNF29,33. Via a negative feedback loop in the brain, cortisol regulates its own release33. Under acute stress, the finely tuned cortisol response maintains a homeostatic balance. However, prolonged or severe stress, as for example experienced in child trauma, can cause prolonged cortisol exposure, leading to multiple impairments including psychological dysfunction34,35,36. Thus, chronic or severe cortisol exposure plays an adverse role in the BDNF dynamics of neuroplasticity and neurogenesis37,38,39. At the same time, prolonged excessive stress levels might also affect behavioral factors, such as healthy sleep habits, which might in turn impact BDNF levels.

Sleep and BDNF

In the last decades, the evidence of how sleep quality may interfere with structural plasticity in the brain and thereby affect memory processing is rising40. High-quality and sufficient sleep benefits neuronal functioning, including cognitive abilities41,42. Sleep supports the learning process immensely by storing and recovering memories43,44. Given that sleep disturbances are a common feature in individuals exposed to chronic or severe stress and are associated with aging and psychopathology23,45,46,47,48. Sleep benefits synaptic plasticity and thereby BDNF seems a good link to investigate the interplay between stress and sleep49. Moreover, several links highlight an association between CAs and sleep disturbance50,51, but only limited research exists on the long-term effects of CAs on sleep quality in adulthood52. Interestingly, a previous study by our group53 showed that sleep might be a mediator of the connection between current stress levels and BDNF. Furthermore, sleep might be relevant when considering the association between stress in early life and BDNF53.

The current study

Therefore, the aim of this study was to investigate exposure to adversity in childhood and psychological well-being, cortisol concentration, and sleep disturbances in adulthood as predictors for sBDNF concentration in a high-risk population of young adults with a history of residential youth care. Given our previous findings of an association between insomnia symptoms and sBDNF concentration, we also aimed to replicate our previous findings53 in this high-risk population. In line with previous results, we hypothesized that sleep might act as a potential mediator in the association of CAs and psychological well-being with sBDNF. Along with these overall objectives, we investigated the following preregistered hypotheses (https://archive.org/details/osf-registrations-jgs2b-v1): sBDNF concentration is associated with different indicators of stress, such as the severity of CAs (H1a), hair cortisol concentration (H1b), and psychological well-being (H1c). Sleep disturbances are associated with sBDNF concentration (H2a) and indicators of stress, including CAs (H2b), cortisol (H2c), and psychological well-being (H2d). Last, sleep disturbances mediate the association between stress and sBDNF concentration (H3).

For this purpose, data from the “Long-Term Outcomes of Childhood Adversities and Offending Behavior (LOCO)” project was complemented by measuring sBDNF concentration in blood samples collected from these participants6,54. All adolescents have been placed in residential youth care. Most have endured various and severe forms of abuse and neglect during childhood and often still suffer from high levels of psychopathology. CAs, psychological well-being, and sleep were assessed using well-established self-report questionnaires (Childhood Trauma Questionnaire55, World Health Organization Five Well-Being Index56, and Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index subscale Sleep Disturbance57). Biological markers (hair cortisol and sBDNF) were measured using ELISA.

Methods

Participants and study procedure

A total sample of 131 young adults with a history of residential youth care participated in this study (91 males, 40 females). These participants were recruited from the “Swiss Study for Clarification and Goal-Attainment in Child Welfare and Juvenile-Justice Institutions” (German: “Modellversuch Abklärung und Zielerreichung in stationären Massnahmen, (MAZ)”); while placed in one of the participating Swiss youth institutional care centers between 2007 and 2012 (for more details see58,59,60). For this study, the participants were reassessed as part of the follow-up “Youth Welfare Trajectories: Learning from Experience” (German: Jugendhilfeverläufe: Aus Erfahrung Lernen (JAEL)) conducted between 2018 and 2020. Participants completed an online survey (www.weaskyou.ch) and a face-to-face assessment, in which several semi-structured clinical interviews were conducted. As a sub-study of the JAEL study, biological material (hair and blood samples) was collected as part of the Long-Term Outcomes of Childhood Adversities and Offending Behavior (LOCO). For more details, see61,62,63,64,65). Participant inclusion criteria for this study were participation in the LOCO study, excluding participants with common cold/flue or known immunological diseases (e.g., HIV). Of the 180 JAEL participants on site, 111 provided hair samples (Cortisol) and 131 provided blood samples (BDNF). All JAEL /(LOCO) participants signed an informed consent for participation and received a shopping gift voucher of up to CHF 500. Participant characteristics and a description of continuous study variables are provided in Table 1 (results section). An overall participant flow-chart is provided in the Supplementary Material (Supplementary Fig. 1). We did not find any differences in any of the study variables and covariates between the sample included in the analysis and the sample not included (see Supplementary R Markdown Document on the OSF: https://osf.io/92ktp).

Table 1 Participant's characteristics und descriptive statistics of study variables and potential covariates.

Self-report questionnaires

CAs were measured using the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire-Short Version (CTQ-SF)55. The CTQ-SF consists of 28 items that measure childhood physical, emotional, and sexual abuse and physical and emotional neglect on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never experienced) to 5 (very often experienced).

Current psychological well-being was assessed using the World Health Organization-Five Well-Being Index (WHO-5)56. The WHO-5 consists of six statements, ranging from 0 (at no time) to 5 (all the time). With a total score of 25, a score lower than 13 indicates a lack of well-being.

Last, sleep was assessed using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), which consists of seven subcategories, including sleep disturbances57. The questionnaire consists of different response formats depending on the item. For subcategories of sleep disturbances, a 4-point answer scale was used, ranging from 0 (not during the past month) to 3 (three or more times a week). The questions included the following: “During the past month, how often have you had trouble sleeping because you… (1) wake up in the middle of the night or early morning; (2) have to get up to use the bathroom; (3) cannot breathe comfortably; (4) cough or snore loudly; (5) feel too cold; (6) feel too hot; (7) had bad dreams; (8) have pain; (9) other reason(s)”. For this subcategory, sleep disturbances are classified into four component scores (0 = no difficulties, 3 = severe difficulties) based on the sleep disturbances scores summed across nine items (0 total score = 0 component score; 1–9 total score = 1 component score; 10–18 total score = 2 component score; 19–27 total score = 3 component score). Participants were classified as sleep disturbed when scoring above a cut-off score of > 9 total sleep disturbance score (component score of 2 or 3, n = 22, Table 1). Although we pre-registered examining the PSQI total score in addition to the sleep disturbance score, we did not analyze the PSQI Total score due to a high number of missing values due to a technical issue (Table 1).

Potential covariates were assessed using a sociodemographic questionnaire to measure age and sex. Childhood socio-economic status (SES) was estimated using a 4-point Likert item “Did you have social or financial problems in your family of origin?”. The answers “does not apply” and “does rather not apply” to the question were considered high SES while the answers “does somewhat apply” and “does apply” were considered low SES. Migration background was assessed with a single yes–no item. Biologically relevant covariates – such as medication, Body Mass Index (BMI, measured as weight and height), and cigarette smoking (“do you currently smoke?”, “how many cigarettes do you smoke per day?”), as well as physical activity, food intake, caffeine intake, and alcohol intake less than 2 h prior to blood withdrawal – were assessed using an in-house questionnaire.

Determination of hair cortisol concentration

The determination of cortisol was done from hair samples collected from the posterior vertex region of the scalp. All participants provided hair of 2 cm adjacent to the scalp, approximately reflecting cortisol secretion over the last eight weeks. The extraction of hair cortisol was done according to66. According to the manufacturer's protocols, hair cortisol concentration was measured using the salivary cortisol ELISA kit (Salimetrics Europe, UK). Evaporated samples were resuspended in assay diluent provided by the manufacturer and finally, the absorbance was measured using the Cytation 3 Cell Imaging Multimode Reader (BioTek, USA). The intra-assay coefficient of variation (CV) was 2.3% and the inter-assay CV was 6.1%. Samples were run in duplicates, and mean values of respective concentrations were calculated in pg/mg.

Assessment of serum BDNF concentration

To assess BDNF levels, a blood sample was collected in the morning (between 8 and 11 am), according to a standardized protocol, using serum Vacutainer tubes (Becton Dickinson). The serum tube was centrifuged at 3000 × g for 10 min. Aliquots were stored at -80 °C prior to the assay. Serum BDNF levels were determined using an enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) kit (Biosensis® Mature BDNF RapidTM ELISA Kit: Human, Mouse, Rat; Thebarton, SA, Australia)67,68. Serum samples were appropriately diluted (1:100) and BDNF determination was conducted on a pre-coated mouse monoclonal anti-mature BDNF 96-well plate as described in the manufacturer's protocol. Absorbance was measured within 5 min of the addition of the stop solution in a microplate reader set at 450 nm with a correction wavelength of 690 nm to determine BDNF concentrations according to the standard curve calculated from a 4-parameter logistic curve fit. The intra-assay CV was 2.4% and the inter-assay CV was 8.2% for the BDNF measurements.

Statistical analysis

The analysis of our study was done in line with our preregistration (https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/JGS2B). In the first step, we determined potential covariates using bivariate correlations between covariates and study variables. Covariates that were significantly associated with the respective dependent variables (H1/H3: BDNF, H2: sleep) were included in the respective statistical models. We then tested our hypotheses using bootstrap regression models, applying 5000 bootstraps resamples. All analyses were conducted using the statistical software R (version 4.2.0), including the packages ggplot2 (version 3.3.6)69 and car (version 3.1–0)70 for bootstrap regression models. We prespecified that Hypothesis 3 would only be tested if at least one sub-hypothesis of H1 or H2 were confirmed. Analyses were repeated with missing values imputed for independent variables and covariates.

Ethical approval

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of Northwestern Switzerland (EKNZ; Ref. 2017–00718; 30.06.2017).

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Results

Descriptive analysis

All participant characteristics, biological covariates, and descriptive values of study variables are shown in Table 1.

None of the tested potential covariates were significantly associated with serum BDNF concentration or hair cortisol concentration (see Supplementary R Markdown Document: https://osf.io/92ktp). However, women had higher CTQ total scores (t(112.26) = −5.12, p < 0.001, d = −0.81), lower WHO-5 scores (t(133.30) = 2.83, p = 0.005, d = 0.42), and higher PSQI sleep disturbance scores (t(115.49) = −3.22, p = 0.002, d = −0.51) compared to men. Furthermore, we found significant differences in PSQI-SD between high and low SES (t(110.41) =  −1.46, p = 0.148, d = −0.26), between participant with versus without migration status (t(160.50) =  −2.30, p = 0.023, d = -0.35, and between participants taking medication versus those who did not (t(82.21) =  −2.93, p = 0.004, d = −0.55). All correlation coefficients between study variables and potential covariates can be found in the Supplementary R Markdown Document (https://osf.io/92ktp). Assumptions for linear regression models were violated regarding (log)-normal distribution of residuals for H1 and H2, even after transformation or windsorizing of the data. Therefore, we used robust bootstrapped linear regression models, using 5000 bootstrapping resamples for all hypotheses.

Supplementary Table 1 in the Supplementary Materials shows the preliminary bivariate associations between the study variables and tested covariates.

Hypothesis 1

Childhood adversity, current cortisol levels and well-being are associated with serum BDNF concentration.

Because none of the potential covariates was associated with BDNF concentration, we did not adjust the models for any tested covariates according to pre-registration. Despite a significant non-parametric bivariate correlation between CTQ total score and BDNF concentration (Supplementary Table 1), we did not find significant associations between any of the stress indicators (hair cortisol concentration, CTQ total score, WHO-5 score) with BDNF concentration using robust bootstrap regression analysis (Table 2). The associations are illustrated in Fig. 1. Thus, hypothesis 1 was not supported in the data from this high-risk sample. Running the models in the imputed data set did not change interpretation of the results (Supplementary R Markdown Document, https://osf.io/92ktp).

Table 2 Results from bootstrap regression analyses for Hypothesis 1 with BDNF concentration as the dependent variable.
Figure 1
figure 1

Linear associations of serum BDNF with with (a) CTQ Total Score (H1a); (b) Hair Cortisol Concentration (H1b); and (c) WHO-5 Total Score (H1c). The dots represent the individual subjects, with the darker dots representing overlapping dots. Abbreviations: sBDNF = serum Brain-derived neurotrophic factor; CTQ = Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; WHO-5: World Health Organization Questionnaire.

Hypothesis 2

Sleep disturbances are associated with serum BDNF concentration and indicators of stress.

We found that sex, socioeconomic status, migration status, and chronic medication were positively associated with PSQI sleep disturbance score (Supplementary Table 1). Therefore, we ran additional analyzes adjusted for these covariates. The associations of BDNF and stress indicators with sleep disturbances are illustrated in Fig. 2.

Figure 2
figure 2

Linear associations of PSQI Sleep Disturbance Score with (a) BDNF concentration (H2a); (b) CTQ Total Score (H2b); (c) hair cortisol concentration (H2c); and d) WHO-5 Total Score (H2d). The dots represent the individual subjects, with the darker dots representing overlapping dots. Abbreviations: BDNF = Brain-derived neurotrophic factor; CTQ = Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; PSQI: Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Inventory; WHO-5: World Health Organization Questionnaire.

We did not find support for Hypothesis H2a and H2c: PSQI sleep disturbance scores were neither associated with BDNF concentration nor with cortisol concentration (Table 3). Adjusting for the binary covariates sex, socioeconomic and migration status, and chronic medication did not change the interpretation of these result (Table 3). Age was not controlled for, as it was not correlated with any of the study variables, probably due to the limited age range.

Table 3 Results from bootstrap regression analyses for Hypothesis 2 with sleep disturbance as a dependent variable.

In contrast, we found support for Hypotheses 2b and 2d: Higher CTQ total score and lower WHO-5 score were associated with higher PSQI sleep disturbance scores and lower WHO-5 score (Table 3). Both predictors remained significant after adjustment for the covariates sex and medication (Table 3). The associations illustrating hypothesis 2 can be found in Fig. 2. Running the models in the imputed data set did not change interpretation of the results (Supplementary R Markdown Document, https://osf.io/92ktp).

Hypothesis 3

The association between stress and BDNF concentration are influenced by sleep healthy and sleep disturbed participants.

Since we did not find an association between stress or sleep and BDNF concentration, and there was also no difference between sleep disturbance groups in BDNF concentration (t(33.6) =  −0.362, p = 0.720), we did not conduct further mediation analyzes. However, in line with our previous findings, we examined instead the association between BDNF and stress in participants with and without indication of sleep disturbance53,71. In line with our previous results, a higher CTQ score was associated with lower BDNF concentration in the “sleep healthy” group (n = 108; intercept: b = 39.165, 95%-CI = 30.146 – 48.609; CTQ total score: b = −0.216, boot 95%-CI = −0.384 – −0.061, ß = −0.197), but not in the “sleep disturbed” group (n = 21; intercept: b = 22.194, 95%-CI = −2.631– 38.22; CTQ total score: b = 0.104, 95%-CI = −0.103 – 0.415, ß = 0.147). In contrast, when analyzed separately, we did not find any significant associations between BDNF concentration and WHO-5 scores or hair cortisol concentration in any of these two groups. Results are illustrated in Fig. 3 and all parameters are provided in the Supplementary R Markdown Document: https://osf.io/92ktp). Running the models in the imputed data set did not change interpretation of the results (Supplementary R Markdown Document: https://osf.io/92ktp).

Figure 3
figure 3

Linear associations by PSQI Sleep Disturbance group between BDNF concentration and (a) CTQ total score; (b) hair cortisol concentration; and (c) WHO-5 score. Abbreviations: BDNF = Brain-derived neurotrophic factor; CTQ = Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; PSQI: Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Inventory; WHO-5: World Health Organization Questionnaire.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to examine the interplay between stress and sleep regarding BDNF levels in a high-risk sample of young adults with previous residential care placements. Especially children in institutional care are highly prone to adverse health outcomes later in life72,73,74, which makes the investigation of this vulnerable group highly essential. Despite no robust associations of the tested psychological (CAs or well-being) and biological indicators of stress (hair cortisol) or sleep with peripheral BDNF concentration, we found an association between stress during childhood and BDNF concentration in sleep-healthy adolescents and adults, but not in those suffering from sleep disturbances, replicating our previous findings.

Association between stress and BDNF

Despite a significant non-parametric association between CTQ and BDNF concentration, we did not observe a robust link between the CTQ score or any of the concomitant stress parameters and serum BDNF across the entire sample. Thus, data only provided weak support for Hypothesis 1a (association between CAs and BDNF) and no support for Hypothesis 1b (association between cortisol and BDNF) or 1c (association between psychological well-being and BDNF). Our inconsistent findings align with previous studies investigating CAs and BDNF with contradicting results, specifically by indicating both, BDNF increases75,76 or declines76,77 in individuals with a history of CAs. In a systematic review78 comparing twenty-one studies, no significant differences were found in BDNF concentration between CAs-exposed and non-exposed groups. The review furthermore revealed that the correlation between CAs and BDNF might depend on age. Specifically, studies in children and adolescents younger than twenty years found higher BDNF levels after exposure to CAs, while the correlation diminished later in life. This elevated BDNF level was discussed to be a compensatory mechanism to counteract the stress-related impairment in the brain of young adults79. This compensation may be dampened in matured adulthood, leading to a higher vulnerability to develop psychological disorders by maintaining a stressful environment80,81. Given that the association between ELA and BDNF might be age dependent and that the age range in this study spanned from adolescence to adulthood (age range 16 – 38 years), we speculate that a potential association might have been masked in our age-diverse sample.

In addition, the association between stress and BDNF might depend on whether someone suffers from sleep disturbances or not53,71.

Association of sleep with BDNF and stress

Despite the apparent role of sleep in the association between stress and BDNF, sleep itself might impact BDNF concentration53,71,82. Therefore, we also examined the direct link between sleep and BDNF concentration (Hypothesis 2a). Although several studies indicated a relationship between good sleep and higher BDNF levels71,82, we observed no association between sleep disturbances and serum BDNF concentration. This might be due to the specific risk-profile of our sample. Nevertheless, in line with our expectations and a rich body of literature, we found a significant association between sleep disturbances and participant-reported stress, including CAs (Hypothesis 2b) and current well-being (Hypothesis 2d). These findings further support the health concerns caused by sleep disturbance83, potentially due to higher levels of perceived stress84. In particular, our findings indicate a long-term effect of CAs experiences on sleep quality, potentially mediated by lower levels of well-being (WHO-5). Sleep disturbances are often related to increased susceptibility to stress, making them a common feature in the pathogenesis of stress-related disorders85. Thus, our results highlight the importance of subjective indicators of perceived stress and well-being in relation to sleep, thereby replicating this known association in a rare high-risk sample of residential care leavers86,87,88. In contrast to subjective reports of CAs and well-being, there was no link between hair cortisol concentration (an objective indicator of stress) and sleep disturbances (Hypothesis 2c). However, previous studies have yielded inconsistent findings regarding this association89. Our finding suggests that changes in HPA axis activity did not underly the association between CAs and sleep disturbances. Another variable to consider is the potential variation in the prevalence of sleep disturbance among our sample of individuals who have experienced a high level of childhood adversity compared to the general population. According to previous research, the prevalence of sleep problems in the general population varies between 20 to 41.7%90, while Western Europe reports a prevalence of 31%91. The observed prevalence of 30%—as determined by a screening question on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 (SCID-592) aligns with the global range of prevalence. Nevertheless, the PSQI score indicates a marginally decreased prevalence of sleep disruption by 17%. The lower prevalence observed in our sample could be attributed to our small sample size or the younger age of participants (Agemean = 26.3 years).

Secondary findings

As our previous analyses did not reveal a significant relationship between the different stress parameters or sleep and BDNF in the entire sample, it was unlikely that sleep disturbances would have an indirect effect or even mediate a relationship between stress and BDNF. Therefore, according to our previous study, we examined the association between stress and BDNF stratified by sleep disturbances: in sleep healthy subjects and those suffering from sleep disturbances. Indeed, our findings were consistent with our previous study53, which demonstrated an association between perceived chronic stress and BDNF concentration only in those who did not suffer from insomnia. Therefore, we extend previous findings by showing similar effects when considering CAs instead of current perceived stress levels. Comparing to our previous study, it must be pointed out that different sleep quality parameters were accessed in the two studies: Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) and PSQI. However, several groups comparing the ISI and PSQI demonstrated a moderate to strong correlation between these sleep indicators93,94. Therefore, our data underscore our earlier key findings regarding a complex interplay between stress, sleep, and BDNF.

Clinical relevance

BDNF administration has a potential therapeutic capability to counteract neurodegeneration as an antidepressant tool14,19,95. From our results, sleep quality is an additional parameter to consider in a potential therapeutic approach against stress-induced depression. Others have shown that insufficient sleep triggers depression by affecting neuronal circuitries96. Sleep is essential in regulating neuronal differentiation and -survival, whereby chronic sleep disturbance may impair the BDNF circuitry in response to stress97,98. From the perspective that children in institutional care acquire biological and mental risk factors towards developing psychopathology, improving sleep might constitute an effective and economical way to counteract some of the adverse effects of CAs and their conveyed risk for depression, thereby enhancing well-being99.

This study investigated a vulnerable population accumulating multiple biological and psychosocial risk factors leading to severe mental health impairment later in life6,73,74. Using this high-risk sample is essential in investigating potential triggers and biological markers for psychopathology. Despite this strength, a high-risk sample also conveys some limitations. One concern is the cross-sectional nature of our data. A longitudinal study design with multiple biological sampling timepoints after adversities and using lagged models might help a more causal understanding of the interplay between CAs, sleep, stress, and BDNF. Especially the long-term tracking of stress, sleep quality, and BDNF concentration in the context of CAs would be promising in disentangling potential links between these variables. This may be the reason why we did not find an association between the different stress indexes and BDNF. A time-distal measure of BDNF has the risk for a diminished impact of CAs on this biomarker, particularly considering age-dependent findings. The age dependence also highlights the importance of the sample collection timing based on participant age yielding more homogenous age groups when using BDNF as a quantifiable biomarker for CAs. In this way, the biological pathways through which CAs negatively impact physical and mental health may be elucidated in greater depth by future studies.

A major concern regarding our data set was the large number of missing values in the PSQI questionnaire (particularly on the subjective sleep quality component) and the following inability to calculate a reliable PSQI total score for the majority of participations. As we pre-registered our variables prior to examination of the data, we were unaware of this large number of missing values. Therefore, we choose to focus on the PSQI sleep disturbance component as described in the pre-registration to preserve the credibility of our findings. We argue that the main issue with this questionnaire was not the non-compliance of the study participants, but rather a technical problem: Some questions had an unfortunate answer format that was unsuited for completion on a tablet device. In contrast to the subjective sleep quality component, the sleep disturbance component questions were answered by all but one participant, making the use of the sleep disturbance score more reliable. This score consisted of items on physical discomfort during bedtime such as feeling too hot or too cold, breathing problems, bad dreams, pain, coughing, snoring, and bathroom usage during the night. However, it should be noted that experiencing one or two of these parameters alone is not enough to indicate impaired sleep, as even healthy individuals may experience them from time to time. Furthermore, the accuracy of the PSQI was validated using one insomnia screening question on a conducted semi-structured clinical interview (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5; SCID-5, first92). Overall, there was a positive association between the SCID5 Insomnia Screening question and the PSQI sleep disruption category (W = 1091.5; p < 0.001). Furthermore, when the insomnia screening question was used to stratify the participants into a sleep healthy and a sleep disturbed group, all results from hypothesis 3 were replicated.

The present study's limitations also include the participants low recruitment efficacy. First, residential and foster care placements present instability in the long-term, with adverse psychological, social, and behavioral factors making recruitment difficult100. Thus, the small sample size in the current investigation might underpower our analysis considering the small effect sizes. Second, another limitation linked to our sample pool is the homogeneity of the subjects regarding a high psychosocial burden in these at-risk young adults, which could dampen any potential effects affecting the interplay between biomarkers and psychological and behavioral parameters. Third, the sample consisted of young adults with high levels of childhood adversity, which might still cause substantial burden as confirmed by a significant association between CTQ and WHO-5. Interestingly, it seemed that it was the more the distal stress experience (CA) that was related to BDNF concentration, rather than the more proximal stress measures, such that results for hypothesis 3 remained stable even when controlling WHO-5 or HCC, suggesting that potential molecular programing mechanisms might be at play. Therefore, future studies could examine the association between CA and patterns in BDNF DNA methylation. Fourth, the lack of a control group without residential care compromises the comparability of our study. Hereby, an unbiased control is a potential factor to tackle the variance constraints and increase our sample dimension. Fifth, although the estimated prevalence of posttraumatic stress disorder was low, we were not able to make in-depth comparisons between those suffering from PTSD and those without PTSD. Nevertheless, additional analyzes revealed that sBDNF, HCC, and WHO-5 score were not associated with PTSD while PSQI sleep disturbance was related to PTSD (data not shown). However, including PTSD as a potential covariate did not change interpretation of any results. Last, due to the need for more parameters in the self-reports, comparisons between several key indicators could not be conducted. For example, it would be interesting to investigate other variables of well-being, resilience, and health outcomes.

Despite these shortcomings, this study provides rare insight into a well-phenotype high-risk population comparing CAs, sleep disturbance and serum BDNF levels. The opportunity to access biological parameters in young adults with previous residential care placements is rare, making our findings more valuable as they provide a glimpse at this vulnerable population. With our analysis in this high-risk group, we could replicate and strengthen our previous findings and thereby show the importance of considering sleep quality in the association between BDNF and stress in a high-risk sample with a history of youth care placement.

Our study using a highly burdened subgroup of residential care leavers allows us to understand the long-term effects of CAs on the brain to overcome potential deficits leading to psychological diseases, such as depression101,102,103. At the same time, this highly specific group limits the generalizability of our findings. Nevertheless, the lack of available research on CAs and neuronal molecules, such as BDNF, is a fundamental issue78. Therefore, our findings provide a small contribution to gain insights into CAs' long-term effects on BDNF for future researchers.

Conclusion

In summary, this study investigated the interplay between sleep, stress and BDNF in a high-risk sample of young adults with a history of residential care placement. Although we did not find a robust association of CAs, psychological well-being, hair cortisol concentration, or sleep with sBDNF concentration in the entire sample, we found that subjects without sleep disturbances showed an association between CAs and sBDNF concentration. We further demonstrated an association between stress and sleep quality, replicating previous findings in a high-risk sample. While our evaluation suggests that sleep disturbance plays a critical role in the relationship between stress and BDNF, further clinical studies, especially in highly diverse groups, are necessary to validate our findings and thoroughly explore the complex interplay between these factors. Furthermore, it is essential to clarify the role of BDNF as a neuronal modulator in this interplay. Despite limitations, the presented findings are valuable considering the importance of sleep quality on the perception and adaptation of stress. Overall, our findings contribute to a growing body of evidence suggesting sleep impairment as a crucial factor when considering a potential association between stress and BDNF, thereby supporting our previous study53. Further studies are warranted to increase our understanding of how stress, sleep, and sBDNF concentration play together to shape health and well-being. Moreover, the inclusion of highly vulnerable participants with a high psychosocial risk complements previous studies in community and clinical populations. Particularly in high-risk populations, the long-term consequences and severity of trauma and other adverse stress events must be taken into account to improve, prevent, and counteract the risks of adversities for young adults.

Supplementary materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded from the OSF project at https://osf.io/erw24/.