Introduction

Personality could be described as relatively stable motivational, emotional, cognitive and behavioral traits of a person impacting on many important life variables ranging from health behavior, longevity, job performance to vulnerability for affective disorders (for an overview see1). Of note, defining personality still remains a highly controversial topic, as a recent discussion paper shows2. Beyond clinical and lexical approaches to study human personality (e.g.3,4,5), many theories attempt to explain the biological basis of personality, including Gray’s reinforcement sensitivity theory, Cloninger’s biosocial theory of personality or Eysenck’s PEN model6.

A relatively new addition to biopsychological-oriented theories of personality has been Panksepp’s Affective Neuroscience Theory7,8. By means of many approaches, including electrical brain stimulation, lesion studies and pharmacological challenges, Panksepp carved out seven primary emotional systems, all being homologously conserved across the mammalian brain9. The homologous conservation of such primary emotional systems across different mammalian species speaks for the idea that such systems endow their carriers with evolutionary advantages—each primary emotional system can be seen as a tool for survival. On the positive side of emotions Panksepp mapped the neuroanatomy and biochemistry underlying the primary emotional systems he labeled SEEKING, LUST, CARE, and PLAY, whereas on the negative side ANGER, FEAR and SADNESS have been similarly illuminated by Panksepp’s research group. In short, these systems provide evolutionary advantages, as they endow mammals with energy to seek for food or a partner (SEEKING), reproduce and transfer one’s own genome (LUST), secure the upbringing of their offspring (CARE) and a system for learning social competencies and motoric skills (PLAY). The negative primary emotional systems help to bring mammals out of the danger zone with a fight system to guard significant resources including one’s own offspring (RAGE/ANGER), a fight/flight/freezing program to cope with physical dangers (FEAR), and maintain protective social contact and avoid separation from caregivers and loved ones resulting in separation distress (SADNESS). For readers interested in the brain structures and neurotransmitter/neuropeptides underlying these primary-process emotional action systems we refer to Panksepp’s seminal work called Affective Neuroscience9 or to a more recent summary10. Also, a detailed introduction into principles of Pankseppian Affective Neuroscience is beyond this brief article, and the interested reader could find an overview in Davis and Montag11.

Based on the Affective Neuroscience Theory7, the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS) assess six of the seven primary emotional systems via self-report, namely SEEKING, ANGER, FEAR, CARE, SADNESS and PLAY. LUST has not been included in the ANPS self-report inventory, given the chance that filling in items on one’s own sexuality could elicit socially desirable answers with a negative spill-over effect on the answers given on the remaining items of the questionnaire.

Accepting the limitations that subcortically-based primary emotions are pre-propositional and that a language-based assessment must necessarily operate in a more tertiary cerebral space, when writing the ANPS items the goal was to ask questions that addressed personal emotional feelings and their behavior elaborations as directly as possible7. In contrast to most personality assessments, items were avoided that required making more cognitive judgments such as “I prefer spending time at a popular beach to an isolated nature reserve” or social comparisons such as “I am more energetic than other people.” More “projective” items such as “I believe that …” were also avoided. The intention was to position people in affective space.

The ANPS illustrates that Pankseppian primary emotional systems do not operate at same strength level in all mammals (including sapiens). Instead, although these primary emotional systems operate in all mammals, they do this to varying energy levels. Montag & Panksepp12, [p. 10] discuss in detail the evolutionary advantage, from a species perspective, of the fluctuation selection concept. They summed up: “The term fluctuation selection (see also Nettle, 200913, p. 64) illustrates that not always is the same trait associated with higher survival, but rather that the trait that is best adapted varies with environmental changes.” Hence, it is good from a species perspective to always have a reservoir of different operating primary emotional systems in the genomic pool, as some of one’s own species will likely be able to adapt to unforeseen changes in the environment.

In the original work by Davis and colleagues7, [p. 60] the ANPS was validated against a reduced set of Goldberg’s Big Five personality markers, which measured Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability (opposite of Neuroticism), and Openness to Experience. The Big Five were seen to represent a theory-free and widely accepted approach to personality assessment. Further, it was hypothesized that studying the lexically derived Big Five personality traits in relation to the Affective Neuroscience Theory, could shed light on evolutionary aspects of the Big Five, namely, which evolutionary ancient brain systems are linked to the Big Five. It could be observed in this early work that SEEKING and Openness to Experience were strongly positively related and the same was true for PLAY and Extraversion. Beyond that, high CARE and low ANGER have been robustly linked to higher Agreeableness, while lower FEAR/ANGER/SADNESS have been linked to higher Emotional Stability. Subsequently, similar observations also have been observed in German, Chinese, French, Italian, Spanish, Turkish and Serbian samples12,14,15,16,17,18.

To sum up our points so far: Primary-process emotional systems are phylogenetically ancient brain systems that arise from subcortical brain regions. In contrast, the Big Five factors have been derived from the statistical analysis of language. This lexical approach speaks in general for a more cerebrally-oriented approach to understanding personality. Viewing the relationship between the ANPS and Big Five scales from an evolutionary brain development framework could begin the process of understanding individual differences in Panksepp’s primary emotional action systems as bottom-up neurobiological underpinnings of the Big Five personality traits (for a more extensive discussion, see Davis and Panksepp3, and more recently, Montag with Davis19).

We acknowledge that the present meta-analysis provides only correlational evidence for this hypothesis. However, additional supportive evidence comes from clinical research – especially for the SADNESS scale as perhaps the most clinically relevant of the ANPS scales. Consistent with affective neuroscience predictions20, Montag and colleagues21 reported that in a group of 55 clinical inpatients being treated for depression the strongest correlation between the Beck Depression Inventory-II and the ANPS scales was with the SADNESS scale (r = 0.53, p < 0.001). This finding that the ANPS SADNESS scale was related to depression was replicated by Fuchshuber and colleagues22 who applied path analysis to a large sample (n = 616 including 147 diagnosed with depression) showing that depressive symptoms as measured by the Brief Symptom Inventory were again most strongly predicted by the ANPS SADNESS scale (beta = 0.52).

Further evidence that ANPS scales are associated with biologically-based psychopathology comes from a pair of studies on bipolar disorder. Savitz and colleaguesl23,24 studied 300 individuals from 47 families including five subgroups: 58 Bipolar I cases, 27 Bipolar II cases, 58 with recurring major depression, 45 with a single depressive episode, and 88 unaffected family members. As expected, in the first study23, they found that Bipolar I diagnosed individuals scored the highest on the ANPS SADNESS scale and significantly higher than unaffected relatives. In the second study with four cases dropping out24, Bipolar II diagnosed individuals predictably scored highest on the ANPS ANGER scale and significantly higher than unaffected relatives. In both studies, ANPS SADNESS and ANGER score decreases in each of the family subgroups were consistent with their pathological severity. Lastly, the SADNESS scale has also been anatomically linked to the amygdala resting state activity in a functional connectivity analysis25, which is consistent with Affective Neuroscience theory (AN theory)9, [pp. 267–268].

The evidence linking the ANPS to Panksepp’s primary emotions and explicitly showing how the activity of these primary emotional systems function as a bottom up subcortical foundation of the Big Five personality traits are in early stages. However, we believe these hypotheses to be highly relevant to an understanding of the biological basis of personality and offer this ANPS-Big Five meta-analysis as a valuable step in that direction.

Since the initial publication of the ANPS in 2003, many studies have investigated the ANPS in the context of different Big Five versions (e.g. NEO-FFI, NEO-PI-R, BFI or TSDI). Moreover, different versions of the ANPS have been administered in these Big Five-ANPS works, namely, the 2003 original ANPS version and the ANPS 2.48. Beyond that, several short forms exist: B-ANPS or ANPS-S. Recently, a short adjective based ANPS-AR has been published26. Please also note that the ANPS is available in many languages, including Spanish, French, German, Turkish, Norwegian, Italian, Polish, Portuguese, Brazilian Portuguese, Chinese, Japanese, Serbian, Persian and soon to be in Hungarian and Dutch. This all said, the present study aims to investigate the overall strength between ANPS-Big Five associations by conducting a meta-analysis on the available data stemming from studies conducted all over the world. Additionally, we explore the use of different assessments of Big Five personality traits as a source of heterogeneity in reported associations, focusing in particular on potential differences between studies using the NEO Inventories vs. alternative operationalizations of the Big Five model.

From the literature review, we expected that high SEEKING is associated with high Openness to Experience, high PLAY with high Extraversion, low ANGER and high CARE with high Agreeableness and high FEAR, SADNESS and ANGER with high Neuroticism. From correlation strength we expect ANGER to be less strongly associated with Neuroticism than the FEAR/SADNESS associations. Regarding SEEKING in the literature, associations with Extraversion turned out to be more heterogenous, but we nevertheless expected a positive association. Finally associations with Conscientiousness and primary emotions should be in the lower area due to Conscientiousness being the most cerebrally-focused personality dimension out of the Big Five personality measures.

Method

Literature search

In order to identify papers investigating the association between ANPS and Big Five personality traits, we followed PRISMA guidelines27 and implemented a study selection strategy based on predetermined eligibility criteria and involving literature searches in the Scopus, ISI Web of Science, and PubMed citation databases. In order to keep the search results as broad as possible, in querying the databases we looked for all papers mentioning use of ANPS instruments using the following strings of keywords: “affective neuroscience personality scale” OR “affective neuroscience personality scales”.

The database search was finalized in April 2020. An additional search was performed by inspecting citations within publications identified as eligible to be included in this meta-analysis. A flowchart illustrating the selection process is shown in Fig. 1.

Figure 1
figure 1

Flow chart of study selection.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Papers identified through database and reference searches were screened for the following inclusion criteria: 1. Studies had to include assessments of both ANPS and Big Five personality traits; 2. Studies had to provide information about the effect size of the association between ANPS and Big Five personality traits.

Exclusion criteria were the following: 1. Studies were excluded from quantitative analyses if we could not retrieve effect size information by inspecting the paper, and this information could not be obtained from the authors; 2. Non-independence of collected data. Studies were considered as non-independent when fulfilling the following criteria: 1. Studies were performed on samples including the same group of participants, and 2. Effect-sizes were computed between the same ANPS and Big Five measures. When two or more studies were found to be non-independent according to these criteria, we selected the study performed on the largest sample for the purpose of inclusion in the quantitative analyses. When a complete overlap between samples was found, we selected the earliest study.

Strategy of analyses

For the purpose of this study, we use Pearson’s correlation coefficients as the effect-size of choice for representing the relationship between ANPS and Big Five personality traits. In the case we could not collect Pearson’s correlations by inspecting the paper (e.g., examined correlations were not reported in full in the results section), we contacted the authors of the study and asked them to provide us with missing correlations.

We conducted a separate meta-analysis for each combination of ANPS and Big Five trait, resulting in a total of 30 distinct meta-analyses (6 ANPS × 5 Big Five traits). We performed the meta-analyses using a random-effects model, as we expected significant heterogeneity in effect-sizes due the varying characteristics of questionnaires used to assess Big Five personality, as well as due to the diversity of cultural and demographic characteristics of the samples. For the purpose of meta-analytical computations, we decided not to transform correlations into Fisher’s z scores because this transformation leads to an overestimation of associations when compared with the original correlation metric28.

Heterogeneity of effect-sizes was determined using the following statistics: the Q test of heterogeneity, the Ƭ2 and Ƭ statistic statistics (i.e., between-study variance and standard deviation of effect-sizes), and the I2 statistic representing the proportion of variance in observed effects due to true heterogeneity (as opposed to random sampling error). In discussing emerging meta-analytic correlations, we refer to Cohen’s well-known classification of correlation effect-sizes, and distinguish between small (0.10 ≤ r < 0.30), medium (0.30 ≤ r < 0.50), and strong correlations (r ≥ 0.50).

Next, we evaluate the impact of the different operationalizations of the Big Five model as a source of heterogeneity. More in detail, we use the Q test for heterogeneity to compare correlations between ANPS and Big Five traits emerging from studies employing NEO inventories to assess the Big Five traits, and studies employing other Big Five operationalizations. Please note that because we expected that relevant differences might exist across these groups in terms of cultural and demographic characteristics of recruited samples, in performing the Q tests we do not assume homogeneity of variances of effect sizes across these groups (i.e., we use separate estimates of Ƭ2, as opposed to a pooled estimate).

Finally, we inspected results for potential publication bias by investigating the existence of asymmetry in the funnel plots visualizing the association between studies’ effect sizes and their relative standard error. For detecting asymmetry of the funnel plots, estimation was performed on transformed effect sizes (Fisher’s z transformation). Symmetry of the funnel plot was determined using Egger’s intercept test29. If the Egger’s test detected a significant asymmetry in the funnel plot, we used Duval and Tweedie's Trim and Fill procedure30 to impute effect-size data for potentially missing studies and compute the unbiased meta-analytical correlation. All analyses were performed using Comprehensive Meta-analysis 331.

Results

Overview of included studies

In total, we identified 21 documents including data on both ANPS and Big Five personality scales. After inspection for non-independence, we found n = 8 document included non-independent studies, i.e., studies that were performed on samples including overlapping participants assessed using the same set of ANPS and/or Big Five measures7,8,12,17,32,33,34,35. The study by Montag and Panksepp12 included information about German and Chinese samples that overlapped with two samples examined in Sindermann and colleagues35. To resolve the issue, we selected the largest German sample (i.e., the sample examined in12), and the largest Chinese sample (i.e., the sample examined in35). Next, we found a (likely) overlap in the German samples examined in studies by Özkarar-Gradwohl and colleagues32, Plieger and colleagues33, and Reuter and colleagues34, which we resolved by selecting the study with the largest sample34. However, while all participants from the sample in Reuter and colleagues34 were assessed using paper and pencil questionnaires, a subset (N = 71) of the German sample examined in32 was assessed via online questionnaires (and on the Big Five side the B5S was administered), therefore showing no overlap. This subset was retained in the dataset for the purpose of performing the meta-analytical calculations. We also found two papers by Özkarar-Gradwohl and colleagues17,32 which included analyses performed on the same sample of Turkish participants; in this case, we selected the earliest study17 for inclusion in the meta-analysis. Finally, we found that two studies by Davis and colleagues7 and Davis and Panksepp8 presented results from the same sample; again, the earliest study7 was selected for inclusion.

Eventually, we ended up with 19 documents, including data on both ANPS and Big Five personality collected on 21 independent samples, resulting in 612 distinct effect sizes representing the association between ANPS and Big Five personality scales: For all selected studies we were able to retrieve effect-size information about the association between all the ANPS behavioral scales (ANGER, CARE, FEAR, PLAY, SADNESS, and SEEKING) and Big Five traits (Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Neuroticism, and Openness), except for a study by Yu36, which only reported information about ANPS scales and two Big Five traits (Agreeableness, Consciousness). Characteristics of selected studies are reported in Table 1.

Table 1 Characteristics of studies included in the meta-analysis.

Mean sample size was 473.29, ranging from 5237 to 1,837 participants34, with an overall combined sample size of 9,939 individuals. The majority of samples were recruited among the general population (n = 19), while a minority (n = 2) were clinical samples. Included studies varied in terms of nationality: the majority of studies were performed on samples recruited in Germany (n = 6), followed by USA (n = 3), Hong-Kong (n = 2), Italy (n = 2), Austria (n = 1), China (n = 1), France (n = 1), Japan (n = 1), Poland (n = 1), Serbia (n = 1), Spain (n = 1), and Turkey (n = 1).

In the selected studies, ANPS scales were assessed using the original ANPS version (n = 13)7, the revised ANPS 2.4 (n = 7)8, and the Brief ANPS (n = 1)38. In selected studies, Big Five traits were assessed using either original or revised versions of the following Big Five personality questionnaires: the Big Five Inventory (n = 6; BFI)39 , NEO-Five Factor Inventory (n = 5; NEO-FFI; n = 1 NEO-FFI-R)40, the NEO Personality Inventory (n = 1; NEO PI-R)40, the Big Five Scales (n = 5; B5S)7, the Big Five short scales (n = 2)41, and Big Five scales from the Internet Personality Item Pool (n =1).

Meta-analytic computations

Mean effect size

In order to establish the magnitude of associations between ANPS and Big Five personality scales, we conducted 30 separate meta-analyses, one for each combination of ANPS and Big Five scales. Because one study did not include information about all Big Five traits, the number of effect sizes included in the meta-analyses varied depending on the specific Big Five trait (Openness: n = 20; Conscientiousness: n = 21; Extraversion: n = 20; Agreeableness: n = 21; Neuroticism: n = 20).

For each combination of ANPS and Big Five personality scales, forest plots of meta-analytical correlations are presented in Fig. 2; the meta-analytic correlations are also presented in Table 2, alongside Q tests for heterogeneity, Ƭ2 and I2 statistics. Given the large number of examined study-level effect-sizes (n = 612), these are visualized in Table 2, and reported in the Supplementary material alongside the relative 95% confidence interval (Table S1).

Figure 2
figure 2

Forest-plots of meta-analytic correlations between ANPS and Big Five Personality scales.

Table 2 Meta-analytic correlations between ANPS and Big Five personality: Central tendency and heterogeneity statistics.

Overall, we found the ANGER component of the ANPS showed a moderately positive meta-analytical correlation with Neuroticism (r = 0.46), a small negative correlation with Conscientiousness (r = − 0.17), and a moderate negative correlation with Agreeableness (r = − 0.41). The meta-analytical correlations for the associations between ANGER and Extraversion (r = − 0.05) and between ANGER and Openness size (r = − 0.05) were near zero.

Regarding the CARE component of the ANPS, findings showed small positive meta-analytical correlations with the Extraversion (r = 0.27), Openness (r = 0.23) and Conscientiousness (r = 0.14) traits, and a moderate correlation with the Agreeableness trait (r = 0.40). The meta-analytical correlation between CARE and Neuroticism was near zero (r = 0.03).

The FEAR component of the ANPS showed a strong positive meta-analytical correlation with Neuroticism (r = 0.69), and small negative meta-analytical correlations with the Extraversion (r = − 0.28), Conscientiousness (r = − 0.13), and Agreeableness (r = − 0.11) traits. The meta-analytical correlation between FEAR and Openness was near zero (r = − 0.03).

The PLAY component of the ANPS showed a strong positive meta-analytical correlation with Extraversion (r = 0.55), and small positive meta-analytical correlations with the Agreeableness (r = 0.27) and Openness (r = 0.18) traits. Additionally, a small negative meta-analytical correlation emerged between PLAY and Neuroticism (r = − 0.28). The meta-analytical correlation between the PLAY component and Conscientiousness was positive (r = 0.06), but negligible in size.

Concerning the SADNESS component of the ANPS, we found a strong positive meta-analytical correlation with Neuroticism (r = 0.62), and small negative meta-analytical correlations with Extraversion (r = − 0.23), Conscientiousness (r = − 0.16), and Agreeableness (r = − 0.10). The meta-analytical correlation between SADNESS and Openness was not significant (r = 0.02).

Regarding the SEEKING component of the ANPS, we found moderate positive meta-analytical correlations with Openness (r = 0.47) and Extraversion (r = 0.34), small positive meta-analytical correlations with Conscientiousness (r = 0.23) and Agreeableness (r = 0.15), and a small negative meta-analytical correlation with Neuroticism (r = − 0.19).

Looking at heterogeneity statistics, we found the results of Q tests were significant for all combinations of ANPS and Big Five traits, supporting the use of the random effect model to compute the meta-analytical correlations. Still, it is worthy to note that for all trait combinations, Ƭ2 ranged between < 0.01 and 0.03, indicating low between-study heterogeneity (i.e., between-study variance in effect-sizes). Additionally, for all combinations, observed dispersion of effect sizes was largely due to true heterogeneity, as opposed to sampling error (93.28 ≥ I2 ≥ 50.61).

Finally, we look at the impact of different operationalization of the Big Five model on meta-analytic correlations between ANPS and Big Five personality traits. Results of Q tests, as well as emerging meta-analytical correlations, are shown in Table 3. Meta-analytic correlations between PLAY and both Extraversion and Neuroticism, and between Neuroticism and both SADNESS and SEEKING, were significantly stronger in size among studies employing NEO inventories (i.e., NEO-FFI, NEO-FFI-R, and NEO-PI-R inventories) when compared with studies using different Big Five operationalizations (e.g., the BFI, B5S, Big Five Short Scales and IPIP assessments; combined group was contrasted). Remaining ANPS-Big Five correlations showed no significant differences across the two groups of studies.

Table 3 Meta-analytic correlations between ANPS and Big Five personality traits by Big Five operationalization.

Publication bias

We inspected the funnel plots of standard error versus the correlation (see Supplementary materials, Figures S1-S30). Egger's regression tests (Table 4) indicated no significant evidence of asymmetry in the funnel plot for all combinations of ANPS and Big Five scales, except for the association between the ANGER component of the ANPS and Neuroticism. In this case, because of this potential indication of publication bias, we used the Duval and Tweedie’s procedure to detect potential missing studies on each side of the funnel plot: the procedure detected no (n = 0) missing effect-sizes (i.e., studies) on the left side and right side of the funnel plot. Overall, this step of analysis showed that no significant indications of publication bias were present in the examined literature.

Table 4 Publication bias analyses: Egger's regression test for funnel plot asymmetry.

Discussion

The aim of the present meta-analysis was to investigate how the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS) map onto the Big Five personality traits. The investigation of 21 samples with 612 effect sizes available led to a clear picture. As observed in Montag and Panksepp12 and in the original ANPS study7, links between higher ANGER, SADNESS, FEAR and higher Neuroticism are robust with the highest effect sizes describing the association with SADNESS and FEAR. High SEEKING scores linked to high Openness to Experience. High CARE and low ANGER map onto high Agreeableness, whereas higher PLAY is strongly associated with higher Extraversion.

Considering the effort to measure the expression of primary mammalian emotions with the ANPS versus the derivation of the Big Five from the statistical analysis of descriptive adjectives, the original paper7 suggested that the Big Five “represents a human language reconfiguration of underlying primary mammalian affective systems into useful phenotypic descriptive systems”7, [p. 67]. However, the risk in the Big Five “lumping” is the potential for clouding the important impact each of these separate emotional forces have in our lives.

In this context, we also mention a moderately strong association in this meta-analysis between higher SEEKING and higher Extraversion, which had been observed/discussed previously, for example in12,42. Such a correlation pattern is also in line with the idea that Extraversion is linked to what cognitive neuroscientists would call “reward processing” (for a discussion see43), with the SEEKING system being of high relevance as an energetic “Go get it system”. Yet, it is also possible that the Extraversion/SEEKING correlation is derived from Five Factor Model data with adjective-based lexically derived Big Five type assessments showing more modest Extraversion/SEEKING correlations. However, additional studies will be required to answer such questions.

We believe our findings to be remarkable from different perspectives. First of all, different Big Five and ANPS measures have been used in the present work (although preliminary, we see in the present meta-analysis four significantly stronger associations with the NEO-inventories in contrast to the remaining group of Big Five inventories which use adjectives, perhaps reflecting that both the ANPS and the NEO-PI-R inventories work with formulated items). Despite the variety of measures used, the overall pattern of associations carved out strongly supports the idea of robust links between language measures of primary emotional systems and the Big Five with the exception of Conscientiousness, likely being the most cerebral dimension of the Big Five and that has so far only been observed in highly encephalized primates. Nevertheless, our summary at this point is limited by the fact that the SEEKING scale had the highest correlation with Conscientiousness at 0.23 (but still accounted for rather small proportion of variance). Conscientiousness might be seen as a personality trait that is not strongly linked to primary emotional systems, but clearly impacting upon the activity levels of these systems: Higher Conscientiousness might go along with higher top-down control of primary emotional systems.

Second, the present findings are noteworthy, because the investigated samples stem from around the globe, with many samples from Asia (China, Hong Kong, Japan and Eurasian Turkey), but also Europe (France, Germany, Italy, Poland, Spain, Serbia) and the USA. In so far, we believe that the findings observed here are indeed supportive of “a global ancestral neuro-biological effect” as described in12 (p. 6). But again, the present meta-analysis does not deal with neuroscientific experimental manipulations, but “only” correlational questionnaire data assessing individual differences in the Big Five personality traits and primary emotional systems. This said, the investigation of the Big Five personality traits in relation to Pankseppian Affective Neuroscience Theory framework seems to shed light on the evolutionary foundations of personality as measured by the Big Five. We mentioned earlier that primary emotional systems have been homologously observed across the mammalian brain, as they endow mammals with tools for survival. Still, individual differences in primary emotional systems exist – meaningfully covarying with (four out of the) Big Five as presented in this work—probably best understood with the concept of fluctuation selection. In other words: From a general psychologist’s perspective it makes evolutionary sense that all mammals are endowed with primary emotional systems, but from a personality psychologist’s perspective this also makes sense, because in some niches different operating levels of a primary emotional system might be favorable.

Third, the overlap between Panksepp’s primary emotional systems and the Big Five provide researchers a route to study the biological basis of the Big Five. Whereas the Big Five personality traits have been merely described using a lexical approach (and basing on this formulated items such as with the NEO-inventories) and therefore do not guide researchers towards an understanding of their biological bases, the substantial overlap of primary emotional systems and the Big Five—as described in this paper—show that neuroanatomical / neurochemical knowledge about the primary emotional systems might also guide neuroscientific studies on the Big Five (how to apply AN theory for such a research endeavor, please see42). But again, the present research must be supplemented by neuroscientific techniques and needs further evidence that the ANPS adequately captures the neurobiology of the primary emotional systems as described by Panksepp in much detail9; see also the recent review paper on the ANPS by Montag et al.44. As some primary emotional systems are not uniquely associated with one Big Five personality trait, (e.g. ANGER negatively links to Agreeableness and positively to Neuroticism), it will be also of high interest to understand the brain mechanisms leading to multiple primary emotional brain systems being combined in different personality traits—likely via different excitatory or inhibitory effect levels.

What are the future directions? Only recently, researchers became interested in investigating the ANPS and the Big Five also on the facets level16,26. For instance, Montag et al.16 observed a particular strong association between Neuroticism’s facet anger hostility and ANGER (0.69). Another example: Regarding the primary emotional system of CARE relations with Agreeableness facets tender-mindedness (0.48) and altruism (0.46) were pronounced. As only very few investigations have been carried out in this area, many more studies need to be published with such a research focus to understand whether primary emotional systems based on the background of Pankseppian AN theory are multifaceted and if so linked to Big Five facets. Although the ANPS has been translated into many languages already (see introduction), we want to encourage researchers to translate the ANPS also to further languages and ideally validate it with the Big Five (as presented in this work). Finally, other personality theories or extensions of the Big Five model need to be more strongly linked to AN theory. For instance, the work by Knezevic and colleagues45 investigated the ANPS in the context of the HEXACO model, thereby also investigating how Honesty/Humility is linked to individual differences in primary emotional systems.

Beyond that most studies in the field are correlational and it would be highly interesting to see longitudinal works assessing covariations between primary emotional systems and the Big Five across the life span (at best also supplemented by neuroscientific data). Concluding, the present meta-analysis provides solid evidence for the idea that primary emotional systems as measured by the ANPS are meaningfully linked with the Big Five personality traits. Moreover, studying individual differences of the Big Five personality traits using a Pankseppian Affective Neuroscience Theory framework also provides researchers an evolutionary approach to understand the lexically derived Big Five personality constructs.