Introduction

Functional roles of mammalian pheromones have routinely been investigated in an intraspecific context, such as territorial marking, sexual signaling and health status conveyance1. Yet, closely related species in mammalian communities often use similar communication signals2 which facilitates bi-directional (interspecific) olfactory communication3 and lowers the relative cost of maintaining sensory receptors4. This concept appears to apply to olfactory communication signals of sympatric murine rodents, including the brown rat, Rattus norvegicus, and the house mouse, Mus musculus, because there is overlap in pheromone components of female mice and female rats5,6. Native to the plains of Asia7,8, brown rats and house mice co-evolved in a predator–prey relationship, with rats preying on mice9,10. Both of these macrosmatic rodents are prolific scent markers11,12 that rely on their sense of smell during mostly nocturnal activity bouts. Within each species, respective urine marks offer a wealth of information about the signaler, including its age13, health14, breeding status15,16, dominance17, kinship and individual identity18,19. Moreover, rat odor elicits an innate avoidance behavior in mice20,21.

Urine marks of rats and mice also disseminate sex attractant pheromone components. Although rats and mice share some pheromone components (e.g., testosterone, progesterone, estradiol)22, the more volatile sex attractant pheromone components of males differ markedly. The ketone blend in urine marks of male brown rats (2-heptanone, 4-heptanone, 3-ethyl-2-heptanone, 2-octanone, 2-nonanone, 4-nonanone6) bears no resemblance to pheromone components emanating from urine marks of male house mice (3,4-dehydro-exo-brevicomin; 2-sec-butyl-4,5-dihydrothiazole23,24).

While acoustic and visual signals have a fleeting presence, odors and specifically pheromones often linger in the environment25,26. This makes pheromones particularly susceptible to inter-species exploitation12,26,27 which is well known in insects28,29,30,31 but has hardly been studied in mammals4,32,33,34,35. Studies on mammalian prey eavesdropping on the communication of their predators have focused on audio and visual communication signals36,37. Only two studies have demonstrated that rodents recognize the presence of predators based on their major urinary proteins and lacrimal proteins10,38. Compared to these high molecular-weight proteins, volatile sex attractant pheromone components contrive long-range mate attraction39 and thus are particularly susceptible to interspecies-eavesdropping36.

Intercepting scent communication in vertebrate communities has long been studied, or viewed, as a one-way dyadic interaction, with prey sensing predator scent25. For instance, feline and canine odors elicit stereotyped fear and avoidance responses in rodents34. However, expanded views of auditory and visual communication systems now portray a multi-directional eavesdropper community network25,36. For example, mustelid, canid and felid predators exploit mammalian prey scent to locate prey12,40, imposing significant costs on chemical signaling in the prey species41,42,43. Whether vertebrate predator–prey interactions are informed and guided by bi-directional (mutual) eavesdropping, or “counterespionage”, on scent signals is entirely unknown, as are the underlying mechanisms.

Scent marks disseminate a myriad of odorants, only a few of which are pheromones, and hardly any pheromones are known to date. When prey avoided locations scent-marked by predators20,34,44, and predators responded to scent marks of prey12, these animals may simply have recognized generic prey and predator scent without necessarily eavesdropping on pheromone signals of target prey or predator foe. Testing the concept of mutual eavesdropping by predator and prey on each other’s pheromones is contingent upon pheromone identification and the availability of synthetic pheromone. When synthetic volatile sex attractant pheromone components of both brown rats (predator of mice9) and house mice (prey of rats10) became available6,22,23,24,45,46, the stage was set for testing the counterespionage hypothesis that mice co-opt rat pheromone components as cues to avoid rat predation, and rats co-opt mouse pheromone components as cues to facilitate mouse prey location. Testing these hypotheses, we were cognizant that the natural sex pheromone of mice and rats comprises additional constituents (e.g., urinal and lacrimal proteins10,38) which—expense-wise—could not be included in our synthetic pheromone lure, and that these constituents as well as non-pheromonal odors47 may amplify any counterespionage evidence demonstrated in our study.

Results

Hypothesis 1: mice co-opt rat pheromone as a cue to avoid rat predation

In mouse-infested sites, trap pairs (see Fig. 1 for the general experimental design) baited with synthetic sex pheromone components of male rats captured 3.05 times fewer mice than trap pairs baited with synthetic pheromone components of male mice (χ2 = 19.75, P < 0.0001) (Fig. 2, top), suggesting that mice avoided macro-locations indicative of rat presence. Moreover, traps baited with male mouse pheromone components captured 15-times more adult female mice and 2.4-times more juvenile female mice than control traps baited with testosterone alone (adult females: χ2 = 10.56, P < 0.01; juvenile females: χ2 = 5.30, P < 0.05) (Fig. 3, bottom), confirming a synergistic effect of testosterone, brevicomin and thiazole on attraction of female mice22. Captures of adult male mice (2) and juvenile male mice (6) were insufficient to warrant statistical analysis. Conversely, traps baited with male rat pheromone components failed to capture a single female mouse, whereas corresponding (testosterone-only) control traps captured one adult female mouse and 13 juvenile female mice (χ2 = 11.01, P < 0.01) (Fig. 3, top), further indicating recognition and avoidance of micro-locations indicative of rat presence. Captures of adult male mice (2) and juvenile male mice (4) in traps baited with male rat lures were insufficient for statistical analyses.

Figure 1
figure 1

Photographs illustrating (a) the double-set, paired trap box design of an experimental replicate, and (b) details of snap trap, food bait and pheromone lure. Each experimental replicate (n = 157) consisted of two pairs of large trap boxes (placed in rat-infested sites), or two pairs of small trap boxes (placed in mouse-infested sites; not shown in this figure), for capturing rats and mice, respectively, with 0.5-m spacing between the boxes in each pair, and at least 2 m between pairs. Numbers refer to: 1 = trap box; 2 = snap trap with food bait69 for capturing (killing) responding rodents; 3 = filter paper treated with synthetic testosterone (a pheromone component shared by male brown rats and male house mice); 4–6 = a 20-ml glass scintillation vial containing plain mineral oil (4; control stimulus) or mineral oil laced with sex attractant pheromone components of either male house mice (5) or male brown rats (6). Note: the smaller trap boxes for mice (not shown here) were fitted with glass scintillation vials reduced in height (cut to size).

Figure 2
figure 2

Trap catch data revealing that house mice are averse to macro-locations (trap box pairs; see Fig. 1) indicative of brown rat presence. The treatment trap in each pair received the volatile synthetic sex attractant pheromone components of male house mice (testosterone, 3,4-dehydro-exo-brevicomin, 2-sec-butyl-4,5-dihydrothiazole) or brown rats (testosterone, 2-heptanone, 4-heptanone, 3-ethyl-2-heptanone, 2-octanone, 2-nonanone, 4-nonanone), whereas corresponding control trap boxes received testosterone only. Trap pair locations with rat pheromone components captured 3.05 times fewer mice than trap pair locations with mouse pheromone components, whereas trap pair locations with rat or mouse pheromone components captured equal numbers of rats, revealing predator-aversion behavior by mice and no evidence for prey-seeking behavior by rats. The asterisks indicate a significant difference in the number of mice captured in paired traps (χ2-tests with Yate’s correction for continuity compared against a theoretical 50:50 distribution, ** P < 0.01).

Figure 3
figure 3

Trap catch data revealing that female house mice stay away from and seek micro-locations (specific trap boxes) indicative of male brown rat and male house mouse presence, respectively. The treatment trap in each pair received the volatile synthetic sex attractant pheromone components of male house mice (testosterone, 3,4-dehydro-exo-brevicomin, 2-sec-butyl-4,5-dihydrothiazole) or male brown rats (testosterone, 2-heptanone, 4-heptanone, 3-ethyl-2-heptanone, 2-octanone, 2-nonanone, 4-nonanone), whereas corresponding control trap boxes received testosterone only. The asterisks indicate a significant difference in the number of mice captured in treatment and control traps (χ2-tests with Yate’s correction for continuity compared against a theoretical 50:50 distribution; * P < 0.05, ** P < 0.01).

Hypothesis 2: rats co-opt mouse pheromone as a cue to facilitate mouse-prey location

In rat-infested sites, trap pairs baited with synthetic male mouse pheromone components captured as many rats as trap pairs baited with synthetic male rat pheromone components (χ2 = 0.01, P > 0.05) (Fig. 2, bottom), revealing that foraging rats did not actively seek macro-locations indicative of mouse prey. On the contrary, traps baited with male mouse pheromone captured significantly fewer male and female rats than (testosterone-only) control traps (χ2 = 5.30, P < 0.05) (Fig. 4, top). Traps baited with a blend of male rat pheromone components—expectedly—captured significantly more females and significantly fewer males than (testosterone-only) control traps (females: χ2 = 4.08, P < 0.05; males: χ2 = 9.48, P < 0.01) (Fig. 4, bottom), confirming the reported attractiveness and deterrence of male rat pheromone components to female and male rats, respectively6.

Figure 4
figure 4

Trap catch data revealing that brown rats stay away from micro-locations (specific trap boxes) indicative of male mouse presence, and that female and male brown rats seek and avoid micro-locations indicative of prospective mates and rival males, respectively. The treatment trap in each pair received the volatile synthetic sex attractant pheromone components of male house mice (testosterone, 3,4-dehydro-exo-brevicomin, 2-sec-butyl-4,5-dihydrothiazole) or male brown rats (testosterone, 2-heptanone, 4-heptanone, 3-ethyl-2-heptanone, 2-octanone, 2-nonanone, 4-nonanone), whereas corresponding control trap boxes received testosterone only. The asterisks indicate a significant difference in the number of rats captured in treatment and control traps (χ2-tests with Yate’s correction for continuity compared against a theoretical 50:50 distribution; * P < 0.05, ** P < 0.01).

Discussion

Our data support the “counterespionage” hypothesis. Mice and rats did eavesdrop on each other’s sex pheromone but they used the information they gleaned in a way only partially in keeping with our predictions for motive. This is the first evidence for bi-directional interspecific recognition of sex pheromones within a guild of mammals and between mammalian prey and predator. Our data also reveal that the sex attractant pheromone components of male mice (brevicomin and thiazole) and male rats (ketone blend) are underlying mechanisms that impart species-specificity to pheromonal communication between these murine rodents.

We deemed field experiments with pheromone-baited traps the most effective way to test our “counterespionage” hypothesis that predator and prey co-opt each other’s pheromone as a cue to locate prey or evade predation. We considered trap captures of wild male and female mice, and wild male and female rats, an excellent means to reveal attraction or deterrence of these murine rodents to their own pheromone and that of their mouse prey or rat foe. For future studies, however, we plan on video recording the behavior of rats and mice near select trap boxes to (i) reveal subtleties of behavioral responses indicative of attraction or fear according to the lure presented, and (ii) document the number of rodents that are approaching trap boxes but are not getting captured, indicating the proportion of the population that generates the data. Testing wild rodents in their natural environments was imperative because domesticated rodents in laboratory settings are known to behave differently than their wild counterparts48,49,50. As mice and rats typically do not share the same habitat51, we needed to run experiments in locations infested with either mice or rats.

As predicted, female house mice co-opted the sex pheromone of male rats as a cue indicative of rat presence and potential predation risk by rats. Female mice largely avoided locations of paired traps disseminating synthetic male rat pheromone (Fig. 2), and not one single mouse female entered a trap box baited with rat sex pheromone (Fig. 3). These results are not surprising given that predator avoidance behavior is critical to the survival of mice, whereas rats do not avoid the odors of their predators, at least not when collecting food in relatively safe and familiar habitats52. Recognizing scent marks of predators such as rats and cats enables mice to detect and avoid locations frequented by these predators, or to adjust their temporal foraging pattern accordingly53. Sensory neurons in the vomeronasal organs of mice detect specific major urinary proteins in urine scent marks of rats and cats which ultimately cause avoidance responses by mice10,54,55. Similarly, a lacrimal protein of rats (rat CRPI) decreases locomotion of mice and lowers their body temperature and heart rate38. However, all behavioral responses by mice in these studies to urinary and lacrimal proteins of rats were recorded in the confines of very small laboratory bioassay arenas where even “heavy” proteins could invoke behavior-modifying effects. Our field data obtained with populations of wild mice and rats conclusively show that the volatile sex attractant pheromone components of male rats have a long-distance aversion effect (Fig. 2) and a short-distance avoidance effect (Fig. 3) on female mice.

The hypothesis that rats co-opt the sex pheromone of male mice as a cue to locate mouse prey was not supported by our data. In rat-infested sites, locations of paired traps disseminating synthetic male mouse pheromone did not yield more captures of foraging rats than locations of paired traps disseminating synthetic male rat pheromone (Fig. 2). Remarkably, both male and female rats recognized the male mouse sex pheromone, and many stayed away from trap boxes, or “burrows”, apparently occupied by a male mouse (Fig. 4). While female rats may have simply recognized the “message” of an inappropriate (heterospecific) mate, the aversion responses of male rats can only be explained in a context other than sexual communication and mate recognition. Irrespective, rats did not exploit male mouse pheromone to locate mouse prey. Rather, they showed the propensity to avoid encounters with potential male mouse prey. There are several explanations for this seemingly peculiar behavior. First, brown rats are omnivores and only opportunistic predators of mice, which are not a primary food source for rats in the urban and industrial settings where we trapped. Second, all of our trapping sites had an abundant and constant supply of food other than live mouse prey, making rats not reliant on predation success for survival. Third (and perhaps least likely), brown rats may have traded the nutritional benefits of a proteinaceous male mouse meal for not risking injury during predation bouts.

Our study has shown that mammalian pheromones, comparable to auditory or visual communication signals, are under surveillance by a network of eavesdroppers. “Designed” for incessant information flow, rodent sex pheromone components and their delivery systems are particularly susceptible to eavesdropping on these signals by illicit recipients, such as predators or prey. Major urinary proteins in urine scent marks of mice and rats serve as dissemination conduits for the volatile sex attractant pheromone components56,57,58. These delivery systems are so sophisticated that they even have inherent timestamps, informing the signal recipient of how recently the message was placed59. The functional role of mouse and rat major urinary proteins could not be assessed in our field study, but we surmise that these proteins would have contributed to the behavioral effects prompted by the sex attractant pheromone components.

Our findings that brown rats and house mice recognize each other’s sex pheromone engender exciting new research opportunities, particularly in conservation ecology. The long-distance aversion effect of brown rat pheromone components on house mice (Figs. 2, 3) could be used as a means to expel mice from biodiverse hotspots in island communities, where rat control has prompted harmful outbreaks of mice60. The tactic of exploiting predator scent for pest control35,61 was successful in various wildlife conservation projects62,63,64 but sourcing of scent directly from predators is impractical and would not be necessary if synthetic rat pheromone was used for manipulation of mice. The failure of some studies to achieve repellent effects with predator odors for pest control34 has likely multiple reasons, one of which being insuffcient longevity of predator urine or feces odors. Slow-release formulations of synthetic pheromone components, possibly presented in combination with some non-pheromonal predator odors47, may not only prolong the effect of predator scent on prey but make this tactic more affordable than sourcing of scent directly from predators.

If synthetic mouse sex pheromones were experimentally shown to attract feral cats, synthetic mouse pheromone lures could be developed for capturing, and subsequent neutering of feral cats that otherwise would continue to reproduce prolifically, extending their already devastating impact on bird populations65. The same pheromone lures could be deployed for trapping feral cats that have invaded, or were deliberately introduced to, island communities where they now threaten seabird colonies66 and many endemic reptiles67. If the eavesdroppers’ network were to include other mesopredators of murine rodents such as the red fox, Vulpes vulpes, or striped skunk, Mephitis mephitis, then synthetic rodent pheromone could be used to help eliminate diseases from mesopredator populations. For example, adding synthetic rodent pheromone to baits laced with oral rabies vaccine68 would likely make these baits olfactorily more apparent to foraging predators and thus expedite bait location and disease elimination.

Materials and methods

General design of field experiments

Parallel field experiments for trapping house mice and brown rats were run between March–June 2017 and October 2016–November 2019 in mouse-infested sites (Exps. 1, 2; 81 paired trap boxes each for mice and rats) and in rat-infested sites (Exps. 3, 4; 76 paired trap boxes each for mice and rats) in the Fraser Valley of British Columbia, Canada. The four sites infested with rats (inferred by the presence of 0.6- to 1.3-cm long fecal pellets with pointed ends) included a food production facility, a food bank, and two recycling centers, whereas the two sites infested with mice (inferred by the presence of 0.6-cm long fecal pellets with blunt ends) included a duck farm and a bird sanctuary. Based on fecal pellet evidence, all sites were exclusively infested with either rats or mice. Population densities in these sites were likely weak to moderate based on infrequent rodent sightings, the amount of feces present, and the time needed to generate the trap catch data. In all sites, rodents had steady access to animal or human food and were exposed to predation by feral cats and owls. Mouse-infested sites had been used in previous research projects with mice5,22,46,69 but were not used for one year prior to the onset of our study. All sites were subject to rodent control measures mainly in the form of poison bait stations.

In each site, experimental replicates for mice and rats were set up along interior or exterior walls of buildings (Fig. 1). Each replicate consisted of two sets of paired trap boxes (PROTECTA Mouse or Rat, Bell Laboratories Inc., Madison, WI 53,704, USA), with 0.5-m spacing between the boxes in each pair, and at least 2 m between pairs (Fig. 1). Each trap box contained a Victor snap trap (M325 M7 Pro mouse or M326 M7 Pro rat Woodstream Co., Lititz, PA 175,543, USA) that was set with a food bait69 which prompted feeding and thus capture of responding mice or rats. Twice or 3-times every week, traps were checked, and food baits and pheromone lures (see below) replaced. Captured rodents were assessed for their age (juvenile or adult) based on genitalia development70, and for their sex based on ano-genital distance71 or PCR genotyping carried out on DNA extracted from ear or tail clips72. Whenever a mouse or a rat had been captured, a new trap box and snap trap were deployed. This procedure ensured that the odor of captured mice or rats did not affect future captures. The position of the treatment and the control trap box within a trap box pair was re-randomized after each capture. The research protocol was approved and supported by the Animal Care Committee of Simon Fraser University (protocol #1159B-15 and #1295B-19) which abides by the Canadian Council on Animal Care guidelines.

Synthetic sex pheromone components tested

Both the treatment and the control trap box in each trap box pair received testosterone, a pheromone component of low volatility shared between house mouse and brown rat males22. Adding the volatile sex attractant pheromone components of either male mice or male rats (see below) to testosterone, we could then test whether these components impart species-specificity to the sex pheromone blend and enable cross-recognition of predator or prey communication signals. This plain experimental design was guided by recent studies already showing that: (1) synthetic testosterone on its own tested versus an unbaited control strongly attracts female mice and female rats22; (2) traps baited with synthetic sex attractant pheromone components of male mice (brevicomin & thiazole; see below), or of male rats (ketone blend; see below), capture significantly more female mice46, and more female rats6, than unbaited control traps; and (3) synthetic trap lures containing both testosterone (or androstenone) and sex attractant pheromone components of male rats or male mice synergistically attract more female rats22, and more female mice22,73, than partial pheromone lures containing either the sex steroid or the sex attractant pheromone components. As the more complete pheromone lure for mice and rats is clearly more effective than partial pheromone lures, there is no need for testing it further versus unbaited controls.

Testosterone was dissolved in acetonitrile (50 μl) and applied to a piece of filter paper at the biologically relevant dose of 750 ng (about five times the amount of testosterone a single male mouse discharged with urine during one day)22. The treatment box in each pair received synthetic sex attractant pheromone components of either male house mice [3,4-dehydro-exo-7-ethyl-5-methyl-6.8-dioxabicyclo[3.2.1]octane (= 3,4-dehydro-exo-brevicomin = brevicomin); 2-sec-butyl-4,5-dihydrothiazole (= thiazole)] or male brown rats (2-heptanone, 4-heptanone, 3-ethyl-2-heptanone, 2-octanone, 2-nonanone, 4-nonanone). The house mouse pheromone components brevicomin and thiazole were each formulated at 1 mg in mineral oil (10 ml) and contained in a 20-ml glass scintillation vial (VWR International, LLC Randor, PA 19,087, USA). This formulation afforded the release of brevicomin and thiazole at rates of 180 ng h−1 and 75 ng h−1, respectively, very similar to the hourly release rates of these two compounds from bedding material soiled by laboratory-kept male mice46. The sex attractant pheromone components of male brown rats were formulated as a 1-mg blend at the same ratio [2-heptanone (100), 4-heptanone (10), 3-ethyl-2-heptanone (10), 2-octanone (1), 2-nonanone (1), 4-nonanone (10)]) as found in headspace odorants of male rat urine, and afforded release rates comparable to those from soiled bedding material of laboratory-kept rats6. The potential of glassware or mineral oil to modulate the effects of brevicomin and thiazole or the blend of ketones was minimized by fitting treatment and control trap boxes in each trap pair with the same glassware and volume of mineral oil.

Statistical analyses

We analyzed all data with R 3.5.074. For each of experiments 1–4, we compared the proportion of captures in treatment and control traps against a theoretical 50:50 distribution, using a χ2-test with Yate’s correction for continuity. We also used paired χ2-tests to compare total captures of mice and of rats in traps baited with synthetic pheromone components of male mice or male rats in mouse- and rat-infested sites.