Main

For decades, it has been demonstrated that sleep plays an important role in long-term memory consolidation1,2,3,4. Systems-level memory consolidation theory posits that the initial phase of the formation of a declarative memory trace (that is, memories that are accessible to conscious recollection, such as memory for facts and events5) is primarily supported by the hippocampus. Over time, declarative memory representations become increasingly dependent upon the neocortex (a ‘two-stage’ model)4,6,7. A central notion in this model is that embedding novel information in the neocortex relies on offline reactivation of acquired information by the hippocampus around ripple events, primarily during slow-wave sleep4,8. Ripples (brief oscillatory events with frequencies of ~80–120 Hz in humans) occur in and around the hippocampus in the MTL, as well as in the neocortex9,10. Extensive research in rodents has established the role of hippocampal ripples in memory consolidation11 and their widespread impact on neocortical activities during sleep12,13. Slow waves (<4 Hz) reflect regionally synchronous alternations between active states of membrane depolarization and spiking, and inactive states of hyperpolarization and neuronal quiescence14,15. According to the active system consolidation framework, slow-wave active states serve as a temporal frame for offline consolidation via synchronization of thalamocortical sleep spindles (9–16 Hz) and ripple oscillations4,16,17. Thus, sleep-dependent memory consolidation is believed to be mediated by coordinated oscillations across hippocampus, thalamus and neocortex.

Most evidence supporting this theory stems from correlative studies, performed either noninvasively in humans1,4,18,19 or with neuronal recordings in rodents20,21,22,23,24. Causal manipulations of ripple activities in rodents established that ripples are necessary for optimal memory consolidation25,26,27. To date, only a few studies have provided causal support for the role of coordinated hippocampal–neocortical interactions during sleep in mediating memory consolidation20,21. In humans, there is evidence highlighting extensive hippocampal–neocortical interactions during sleep13,28,29, but direct causal evidence linking these interactions with memory consolidation is currently missing30. Here, we designed a closed-loop stimulation protocol to dynamically enhance the temporal coupling between MTL ripples, neocortical slow waves and thalamocortical spindles during non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep to directly test the role of their temporal coupling in overnight consolidation of declarative memory.

Results

Neurosurgical patients with pharmacoresistant epilepsy who were implanted with intracranial depth electrodes for clinical reasons (n = 18, ages 19–47 years, all fluent English speakers; Supplementary Table 1) provided written informed consent before participation in a study approved by the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA) Institutional Review Board. The unique intracranial clinical setup allowed simultaneous recordings of intracranial electroencephalography (iEEG) and single-neuron activity in the MTL and distant neocortical sites. Electrophysiology was complemented by cognitive assessment, whereby participants were tested during two experimental nights (order counterbalanced): an intervention night and an undisturbed night (Fig. 1a and Supplementary Table 2). This within-participant design helped control for individual variability in clinical and memory profiles31. On the intervention night, real-time closed-loop (RTCL) stimulation was performed intermittently in 5-min blocks for a total of ~90 min during early NREM sleep (Methods, Fig. 1b, Extended Data Fig. 1 and Supplementary Table 3). One iEEG electrode in the MTL served as a synchronization probe for determining the timing of closed-loop control, while a second neocortical iEEG electrode served as the stimulation site (typically, in orbitofrontal cortex white matter (15 of 19 stimulation nights); Fig. 1c,d and Extended Data Figs. 2 and 3). Slow-wave activity in the MTL probe was monitored and analyzed in real time to trigger brief (50 ms) high-frequency (100 Hz) electrical stimulation events in the neocortical stimulation site roughly once every 4 s (Methods). The closed-loop intervention had two modes of operation (Methods and Extended Data Fig. 3)—either (i) ‘synchronized (sync) stimulation’ (Fig. 1c) or (ii) ‘mixed-phase stimulation’—which were applied in two separate groups of participants. Sync-stimulation involved neocortical stimulation that was time-locked to the MTL slow-wave active phase, aimed at synchronizing MTL with thalamocortical activities. During these active phases, corresponding to the iEEG negative peak (Fig. 1c)32, ripples occur more frequently and their prevalence is believed to be key for hippocampal–cortical communication20,28,33,34. During mixed-phase stimulation, performed in a separate group of participants, identical neocortical stimulations were applied but their timing was without regard to the MTL slow-wave phase (Methods and Extended Data Fig. 3). Given our previous work on local sleep oscillations, where MTL slow waves can be phase-shifted or even entirely independent from neocortical slow waves32,35,36, we hypothesized that sync-stimulation would be key in increasing hippocampo–neocortical coupling and that stimulating white-matter electrodes during sleep would allow local low-amplitude stimulation to affect wide territories36,37.

Fig. 1: Neocortical stimulation synchronized to medial temporal lobe sleep activity improves overnight recognition memory accuracy.
figure 1

a, Experimental design. Each individual participated in two overnight sessions (order counterbalanced), an undisturbed sleep session and another session with RTCL neocortical stimulation. Memory was assessed immediately following evening learning and following sleep. b, Top, representative spectrogram of iEEG during overnight sleep session (short-time Fourier transform; Methods). Black rectangles mark slow-wave (0.5–4 Hz) and sleep spindle (9–16 Hz) frequency bands used for NREM detection (white dots). Middle, RTCL intervention lasted 45–90 min with alternating 5-min stimulation (STIM) and PAUSE intervals. PRE interval (interval before the first stimulation block) is used for some analyses. Bottom, schematic of RTCL approach where MTL slow-wave active states (blue iEEG troughs, co-occurring with neuronal activity, bars) are used to trigger neocortical stimulation pulses (red). c, Representative RTCL input and DBS sites: (i) Coronal magnetic resonance (MR) images denoting iEEG electrode locations. Blue, RTCL input’s MTL location; red, prefrontal white-matter DBS location; yellow, iEEG contacts on same electrodes. (ii) Single-trial MTL probe iEEG signals (each row denotes a trial): increased voltage (warm colors) triggered stimulation in a neocortical site at t = 0 s (participant 2, n = 244 stimulations). Average and s.e.m. of the MTL probe signal are superimposed (black; scale bar, 100 μV). (iii) Average iEEG signal adjacent to the neocortical stimulation site, aligned to stimulation pulses. d, All pairs of MTL probe (blue) and stimulation site electrodes (black), overlaid on a standard (Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI)) brain template (n = 18 participants). Line color depicts stimulation type. Red, synchronizing stimulation (sync-stim) in prefrontal cortex; brown, sync-stimulation in temporal neocortical regions; gray, mixed-phase stimulation in prefrontal neocortex. e, Learning and memory paradigm presented image pairs of celebrities and animals, followed by recognition memory testing (Methods). f, Overnight change in recognition memory accuracy following undisturbed sleep versus sleep with RTCL stimulation. Line colors as in d. g, Within-participant difference in overnight recognition memory accuracy between intervention night and undisturbed sleep. All participants with sync-stimulation in orbitofrontal cortex (red) show superior performance in stimulation nights (P = 0.01, binomial test), while none of mixed-phase stimulation participants (gray) do.

Source data

Synchronized stimulation improved memory accuracy

To assess the effects of the intervention on overnight memory consolidation, participants performed a visual paired-association task before sleep, learning 25 pairings between photos of famous people and animals (contextualized as ‘pet owners’ and their pets) on the evening before each experimental night (Fig. 1e and Methods). A different set of images was used each night. Two different measures assessed two separate aspects of memory performance. First, we evaluated recognition memory via responses to learned images and a set of lures, quantifying recognition memory accuracy as the difference between hit rate and false-detection rate (Methods). Second, we evaluated the successful pairing (association) of each person to their associated animal. Performance on these two measures was assessed four times for each participant (two time points × two nights), without any feedback on responses: in the evening (several minutes following learning) and the morning (following overnight sleep), separately around an intervention night and an undisturbed sleep night (Fig. 1a). Twelve participants completed this full cognitive testing suite, and six additional participants were only included in neurophysiological analyses (Methods and Supplementary Tables 2 and 3). In 6/6 participants receiving sync-stimulation in prefrontal cortex white matter (Fig. 1f,g), recognition memory accuracy following the intervention night was superior to that following undisturbed sleep (*P = 0.01 based on a binomial probability distribution; Methods and Extended Data Fig. 4c–e). Mixed results were observed for sync-stimulation delivered in other posterior neocortical regions (Fig. 1f,g; n = 3), and a trend for degraded performance was observed for participants who received mixed-phase stimulation (Fig. 1f,g; n = 3). Sync-stimulation did not reliably affect the pairing (association) accuracy (Extended Data Fig. 4a,b). We did not find significant correlation between baseline recognition memory accuracy on the first evening test and intervention efficacy (Spearman correlation; n = 9 sync-stimulation participants, ρ = 0.04, P = 0.9), suggesting memory improvements were not unique to participants with lower performance. Sync-stimulation did not significantly alter reaction times during memory recall compared with undistributed sleep (Wilcoxon rank-sum test: P = 0.65; Extended Data Fig. 4f). Conversely, overnight decrease in reaction times, representing an improvement in a visual psychomotor vigilance task (PVT; Methods38) was significantly lower following sleep with sync-stimulation as compared with undisturbed sleep (Extended Data Fig. 4g), indicating that memory improvement does not reflect an across-the-board improvement in behavioral performance. Therefore, memory accuracy improvement associated with sync-stimulation likely reflects an enhanced stabilizing effect of sleep to reduce forgetting3,4.

Correlated enhancement of sleep spindles and memory accuracy

To test whether changes in sleep electrophysiology underlie the observed behavioral changes, we first examined how stimulation modulates slow waves and spindles, known to be tightly linked to memory consolidation21,39,40,41,42. We used two complementary analysis approaches, in the power domain and in the time domain. In both analyses, a within-session approach compared the modulation in every iEEG contact to its session-specific baseline (see Methods for baseline selection). We evaluated any measure of interest separately for sync-stimulation or mixed-phase stimulation modes (Methods) to test how the temporal accuracy of stimulation affects sleep electrophysiology. First, spectral analysis was used to test whether time–frequency representations (spectrograms) of iEEG signals in multiple brain areas reveal changes in spindle power in an a priori-defined frequency range of 9–16 Hz following stimulation events (Methods). We used a protocol in which short stimulation bursts (50 ms) were delivered, spaced by more than 4 s, during 5-min stimulation blocks interleaved with 5-min pause blocks, during which no stimulation was provided (Methods and Fig. 1b). We first studied the immediate effects of stimulations (as observed within stimulation blocks), during 3-s periods following single stimulation bursts. Sync-stimulation bursts immediately increased sigma (spindle) power relative to a 1-s pre-stimulation baseline across the brain (Fig. 2a and Methods). Importantly, this increase went above and beyond the expected tendency of spindle power to increase around slow-wave active states35,43 because it was significantly greater than that found in sham-stimulation moments during intermittent ‘pause’ blocks that had identical delays from MTL slow-wave peaks (Methods; Fig. 2a(ii): Wilcoxon signed-rank test found a significant increase in spindle-band for sync-stimulation contacts; n = 565 iEEG contacts from MTL and neocortical sites, P < 10−30). Conversely, we did not find any immediate change in spindle power in the mixed-phase stimulation group (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, n = 215, P = 0.73). Nor did we find an increase in post-stimulation power when examining a control frequency range (20–27 Hz). Next, we compared the immediate effects of sync-stimulation and mixed-phase stimulation modes on slow waves and spindles in the time domain, by selectively identifying individual slow-wave and spindle events on each iEEG contact, according to established detection algorithms35,44 (Fig. 2b and Methods). The probability of slow-wave events was reduced for both sync-stimulation and mixed-phase conditions immediately following stimulation (Extended Data Fig. 5a). However, a significant increase in spindle detection probability was observed immediately following sync-stimulation (in the 3-s inter-stimulus interval) compared to sham-stimulation time points with identical delays from MTL slow-wave peaks (Fig. 2c(i); Wilcoxon signed-rank test, P < 10−4). Conversely, in the mixed-phase condition, immediate iEEG spindle detection probability was significantly decreased (Fig. 2c(ii); P < 10−8, Extended Data Fig. 5b). Critically, when considering all participants (both sync-stimulation and mixed-phase stimulation), behavioral changes in recognition memory accuracy were highly correlated with the degree to which stimulation affected immediate spindle occurrence (Fig. 2c(iii); Spearman correlation ρ = 0.69, P = 0.013; n = 12 participants). A high correlation coefficient between immediate spindle increase and memory increase was also observed when focusing on the smaller group of nine participants receiving sync-stimulation, but this did not reach statistical significance (Fig. 2c(iii); ρ = 0.53, P = 0.148).

Fig. 2: Synchronized stimulation enhancement of sleep spindles correlates with memory accuracy improvements.
figure 2

a, Immediate (<3 s) changes in sleep spindle activity during STIM blocks (yellow highlight) compared to sham intervals during PAUSE blocks. (i) Representative average time–frequency response (TFR; induced power) following stimulation in orbitofrontal cortex iEEG shows immediate increase in spindle power (9–16 Hz, white rectangle). (ii) Enhanced spindle power following sync-stimulation compared to sham stimulation (n = 565 iEEG electrodes). P = 1.4 × 10−39 via Wilcoxon paired signed-rank test. b, Representative spindles (blue asterisks) in simultaneously recorded iEEGs (black time-courses, z-scored for visualization) of two participants. Bottom time course (blue), MTL signal used for stimulation timing, superimposed with slow-wave-filtered (<2.5 Hz) signal showing active (pink) versus inactive (brown) phases; R, right; L, left; OF, orbitofrontal cortex; EC, entorhinal cortex; AH, anterior hippocampus; A, amygdala; PHG, parahippocampal gyrus. c, Spindle detection probability increases immediately following sync-stimulations (i, n = 509 iEEG electrodes) and decreases following mixed-phase stimulation (iii, n = 212 iEEG electrodes) relative to sham moments. Black crosses denote the median. P = 1.6 × 10−6 for sync-stimulation versus sham. P = 2.44 × 10−10 for mixed-phase stimulation versus sham via Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. P = 2.66 × 10−14 for sync versus mixed-phase stimulation via Wilcoxon rank-sum test. (ii) Individual memory accuracy enhancement by intervention (y axis, as in Fig. 1g) correlates with immediate spindle enhancement (Spearman correlation ρ = 0.69, P = 0.013, n = 12 participants). Spindle enhancement distribution across all iEEG contacts is shown for each participant; colors as in Fig. 1d. Black crosses indicate the median per participant. Black solid or dashed lines show the linear fit for all participants or sync-stim participants alone, respectively. d, Top: Prolonged stimulation-driven change in spindle rate in the 1 min following STIM blocks (post-stim; yellow) compared to the 1 min at the end of pause blocks (pre-stim; gray). Middle: Prolonged stimulation-driven enhancement scores (Methods) in slow-wave event rate (y axis) versus sleep spindle event rate (x axis). Each dot depicts an iEEG electrode (n = 275, 90 and 175 iEEG contacts for red, brown and gray groups, respectively; colors as in Fig. 1c). Spindle rates are elevated in sync-stimulation participants but not in mixed-phase participants. Statistical comparisons via Wilcoxon rank-sum test: P = 2.98 × 10−9 for slow waves and P = 1.42 × 10–10 for spindle indices. Whiskers depict the 25th–75th percentiles for sync-stim (red; all stim locations) and mixed-phase contacts (gray). Bottom: Prolonged enhancement of spindle rate following sync-stimulation blocks is widespread across both cortical hemispheres. Each circle (n = 275) marks an iEEG electrode in sync-stimulation participants. Circle color represents spindle event enhancement score (Methods); contact location is overlaid on a standard MNI brain template.

Source data

Next, we also tested for prolonged effects of stimulation occurring beyond the 5-min stimulation blocks, by comparing the rates of iEEG sleep oscillations in the 1-min following each stimulation block with the 1 min at the end of each ‘pause’ block, using a normalized pre/post index (Methods). We found that sync-stimulation led to prolonged enhancement of spindle rate, whereas mixed-phase stimulation led to prolonged decrease in spindle rate (Fig. 2d and Extended Data Fig. 5e; Wilcoxon rank-sum test comparing sync-stim and mixed-phase spindle enhancement index distributions; P < 10−8). Interestingly, prolonged spindle rate increase was observed in both hemispheres across wide cortical territories including MTL and neocortical electrodes (Fig. 2d and Extended Data Fig. 5d–i; Wilcoxon rank-sum test comparing neocortical and MTL spindle enhancement index distributions, P = 0.13; Methods). While slow-wave rates following sync-stimulation were comparable before and after stimulation blocks, mixed-phase stimulation led to a significant reduction in slow-wave occurrence when assessing prolonged effects (Fig. 2d and Extended Data Fig. 5d; Wilcoxon rank-sum test comparing sync-stim and mixed-phase slow-wave enhancement index distributions, P < 10−7). Together, analysis of slow waves and sleep spindles revealed that deep brain stimulation (DBS) that is synchronized to MTL slow-wave active phases leads to robust and widespread effects that persist beyond stimulation blocks, particularly enhancing spindles in a manner correlated with memory benefits.

Stimulation increased spike phase-locking to medial temporal lobe slow waves

Next, we assessed the effects of sync-stimulation on phase locking of spiking in neural units to MTL. We quantified how sync-stimulation affected the timing of neuronal action potential discharges recorded on individual microwires (Extended Data Fig. 6a(i)) across multiple brain regions, with respect to MTL iEEG slow-wave phase (0.5–4 Hz; this signal was used for stimulation timing; Supplementary Table 4). For each neuronal unit (spike-sorted offline; Methods), we calculated the phase of each spike relative to the MTL slow wave and fitted each distribution with a cosine function to evaluate the depth of phase locking and preferred firing phase (Methods and Extended Data Fig. 6b; n = 325 neuronal units in eight sync-stimulation participants with microwire recordings). First, we evaluated the changes in neuronal firing by comparing phase-locking depth during stimulation blocks (apart from intervals around stimulation bursts) and pause blocks to several baseline options (Methods, Extended Data Fig. 6c,d and Fig. 3a). To include as many action potential events in this analysis—even for low firing rate units—we focused on long intervals, combining all 5-min stimulation blocks (‘stim’) and comparing them with 10–15 min periods of baseline activity before the first stimulation block (‘PRE’). We found that sync-stimulation increased the proportion of neuronal units outside the MTL with significant phase locking to MTL iEEG active states from 34% to 50% (Fig. 3b), but this percentage was only modestly altered for MTL units (46.5% to 50.5%; Fig. 3b). Next, as examined for sleep spindles, we investigated potential prolonged effects in the 1-min intervals beyond stimulation blocks to better understand the dynamics and regional variability of phase-locking changes (Methods). In the first minute following every stimulation block, the percentage of phase-locked cells returned to baseline (34% for units outside MTL) but units increased the phase-locking depth relative to baseline, and this was specific to neural units outside the MTL where observed effects were significant (Fig. 3c, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; units outside the MTL: P = 0.007/n = 47; units in the MTL: P = 0.9/n = 26, but Wilcoxon rank-sum test comparing populations P = 0.1). Even when restricting the data to compare only the first minute following each stimulation block with the last minute of the previous pause block, we found significantly higher depth of phase locking immediately after sync-stimulation (Fig. 3d; Wilcoxon signed-rank test when aggregating all neural units—P < 10−3). This result represents a prolonged effect with dynamics similar to the decay profile we observed for spindles and slow waves, decreasing within minutes. Prolonged effects were validated via two shuffling procedures (Methods)—by verifying that real data significantly differed from a shuffled distribution with randomly assigned condition labels (Extended Data Fig. 6c; Wilcoxon rank-sum test, n = 57 neural units, P = 0.02), and by verifying that phase locking is not dependent on possible changes in mean firing rates (Extended Data Fig. 6c; Kolmogorov–Smirnov two-sample test, P = 0.7). The presence of robust prolonged effects allowed us to confidently interpret changes as true differences in synchronization rather than potential contamination by stimulation artifacts. Together, we found that sync-stimulation led to prolonged increases in phase locking of neuronal spiking activity, particularly in distant regions, to MTL slow waves, which decayed within minutes.

Fig. 3: Neuronal spiking across the brain phase locks to medial temporal lobe slow-wave activity following synchronized stimulation.
figure 3

a, Two representative examples show 4 s of orbitofrontal cortex spiking activity during sleep before (left) and during (right) stimulation. Rows (top to bottom) show prefrontal iEEG (black, filtered 0–30 Hz), spiking in four neuronal clusters (black ticks) and MTL probe iEEG (blue) superimposed with slow-wave active (pink) versus inactive (brown) phases. Spiking activity before stimulation is scattered and becomes phase locked to MTL active phase (pink) during stimulation blocks. iEEGs were z-scored for visualization. b, Analysis across neuronal population: Fraction of units showing significant phase locking to MTL ‘ON’ phase (pink), to MTL ‘OFF’ phase (brown) or no significant (NS) phase locking (white). Top row, outside MTL (n = 190), the percentage of locked clusters increased from 34.0% during baseline (‘PRE’, gray block, left pie chart) to 50.0% during stimulation block (‘STIM’, blue, right pie chart). In MTL, the percentage of phase-locked units (n = 107) remained stable (46.5% pre-stim (left) and 50.5% during stim block (right)). c, Prolonged increase (1 min after stimulation, yellow intervals) in phase locking to MTL slow waves (quantified by locking depth change; Methods) is widespread across cortex in both hemispheres regardless of stimulation location. Each circle shows the anatomical location of neuronal clusters overlaid on a standard (MNI) brain template. Circle color represents changes in phase locking for that region (color bar on right). Circle size reflects the number of units detected in that region (largest spheres have numbers overlaid). Bottom, locking depth change distributions by regions. MTL includes hippocampus, entorhinal cortex and parahippocampal gyrus. Am, amygdala; OF, orbitofrontal cortex; AF, anterior prefrontal cortex; In, insula; TG, temporal gyrus; Par, parietal cortex; Occ, occipital cortex. d, Prolonged neural phase-locking increase (Methods, time periods as in Fig. 2d, yellow versus gray periods highlighted in top illustration). Locking depth change distributions are stacked for neural units located in MTL (blue) and other areas (dark gray); n = 57 units met firing rate criteria; P = 5 × 10−4 via Wilcoxon signed-rank test.

Source data

Increased ripple-slow wave-spindle coupling correlates with memory

Next, we focused on the coordination between hippocampal ripples and thalamocortical slow waves and spindles, previously suggested to mediate sleep-dependent memory consolidation. To this end, we focused on a subset of 16 participants where hippocampal and prefrontal activities were simultaneously monitored. Ripples (Fig. 4a; n = 7,172 events) were detected in MTL iEEG electrodes during pre-stimulation sleep using an automated algorithm44 (Methods), using bipolar referencing to minimize volume conduction effects. Most events were detected in the hippocampus (CA1, CA3/DG and subiculum), but also in adjacent entorhinal cortex and in the parahippocampal gyrus (see Extended Data Fig. 7 for breakdown to different MTL sub-areas), where ripples have been previously reported28,45,46,47. An extensive inspection of detected ripple events, along with their narrow-band frequency profile around 80–120 Hz, attests to largely successful separation from pathological high-frequency MTL oscillations and interictal epileptiform discharges (IEDs), which exhibited a distinct spectral profile with wide-band higher-frequency pathological activity (Extended Data Fig. 8a).

Fig. 4: Synchronized stimulation increases triple co-occurrence of medial temporal lobe ripples, neocortical slow waves and thalamocortical spindles.
figure 4

a, (i) Example ripples (brown asterisks) detected in MTL (parahippocampal gyrus) iEEG: top and bottom rows show iEEG signal filtered (0–300 Hz or 80–100 Hz, respectively). Middle row, spiking activity on adjacent microelectrodes. (ii) Grand average of unfiltered iEEG aligned to the maximum ripple peak (mean ± s.e.m., n = 7,172 ripple detections in 28/13 iEEG channels/participants during pre-stim epochs). (iii) Average ripple-peak-locked TFR (percentage change from 1-s baseline) highlights the band-limited frequency profile of detected ripples. b, All pairs of neocortical (black) and MTL (blue) iEEG electrodes used in subsequent co-occurrence analysis, overlaid on a standard (MNI) brain template (n = 41 iEEG electrode pairs, 15 participants). Line colors as in Fig. 1d. c, Double co-occurrence of MTL ripples and neocortical slow waves: (i) Example: simultaneous recording of neocortical slow wave (top; purple asterisk shows positive iEEG peak) and MTL ripple (middle and bottom; brown asterisk shows detected ripple). (ii) Incidence of double co-occurrence significantly increased in the 1-min interval post-stimulation blocks (yellow) relative to 1-min end of pause block (gray). Inset, box plot of differences between post-stim and pre-stim incidence rates (n = 25 electrode pairs in 10 participants in sync-stimulation group; P = 8.2 × 10−4, right-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank test). Line colors as in Fig. 1d. See Extended Data Fig. 9d for mixed-phase distribution. d, Triple co-occurrence of MTL ripples, neocortical slow waves and thalamocortical spindles: (i) Example signals as in c. Pink asterisk denotes spindle identified shortly after slow-wave/ripple event. (ii) Incidence of triple co-occurrence significantly increased in the 1-min interval post-stimulation blocks (yellow) relative to 1-min end of interval block (gray). Inset, box plot of differences between post-stim and pre-stim incidence rates (n = 5 electrode pairs in 3 participants, P = 0.03, right-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank test). See Extended Data Fig. 9e for mixed-phase distribution. Box plots in panels c and d represent interquartile range, whiskers mark the 1–99 percentiles. e, Memory accuracy enhancement following intervention (y axis) correlates with increase in MTL ripples-neocortical slow waves double co-occurrence (x axis; Spearman correlation, ρ = 0.8, P = 0.007; n = 30 MTL–neocortical electrode pairs in 8 participants). Markers are median values per participant, bars are the s.e.m.; colors as in Fig. 1d; black line shows linear fit based on median values per participant. Subpanel shows correlation between memory accuracy enhancement and stringent triple co-occurrence criteria (MTL ripples, neocortical slow waves and spindles; n = 12 pairs, 6 participants, ρ = 0.7, P = 0.2). iEEGs were z-scored for visualization.

Source data

Ripple detection probability decreased immediately following stimulation bursts (in 3-s inter-stimulus intervals) relative to sham moments in stimulation-free intervals, in both sync-stimulation and mixed-phase stimulation (Extended Data Fig. 9a and Methods; Wilcoxon signed-rank test: P = 0.004/n = 18 and P = 0.0.01/n = 8 for sync-stim and mixed-phase stim iEEG contacts, respectively, no significant difference between groups). However, this reduction did not extend beyond stimulation blocks, as we did not find a prolonged change in ripple rate when focusing on the first 1-min intervals of pause periods as in other analyses (Methods and Extended Data Fig. 9b; Wilcoxon signed-rank test yielded nonsignificant P values for both stimulation modes and no significant difference was found between groups).

Although we did not find an increase in the incidence of ripple events, we asked whether synchronizing stimulation induced an increase in temporal coupling between hippocampal ripples and oscillations outside the MTL, namely neocortical slow waves and thalamocortical spindles. We evaluated the coupling incidence in cross-brain electrode pairs such that one electrode was placed in MTL and exhibited ripples during pre-stimulation sleep and the other was located in the neocortex with robust spindle activity (all neocortical contacts except five were located in the orbitofrontal cortex; Fig. 4b and Methods). We further focused on fast (>11 Hz) sleep spindles (Methods and Extended Data Fig. 10), because these are preferentially associated with memory consolidation42,48, are more synchronized with slow-wave active phases48, and are associated with hippocampus activation49, even though they are not as prevalent in prefrontal cortex35.

We found that sync-stimulation, but not mixed-phase stimulation, enhanced the temporal coupling between MTL ripples and neocortical slow waves in the 1-min following every stimulation block, relative to 1 min preceding stimulation blocks (prolonged effects as in other electrophysiology analyses; Methods and Fig. 4c, right-tailed Wilcoxon sign-rank test: **P < 10−3, n = 25 pairs, for sync-stimulation; P > 0.5, n = 13 pairs for mixed-phase stimulation; rank-sum Wilcoxon test comparing distributions of changes in both stimulation modes, P < 0.05; Extended Data Fig. 9d). Furthermore, using a more stringent triple co-occurrence criterion of cortical slow waves, thalamocortical sleep spindles and MTL ripples (Methods), we also found that sync-stimulation increased co-occurrence of hippocampal and thalamocortical sleep oscillations (Fig. 4d; right-tailed Wilcoxon sign-rank test: *P = 0.03/n = 5 and P > 0.5/n = 7, for sync-stimulation and mixed-stimulation groups, respectively, rank-sum Wilcoxon test comparing distributions of changes in both stimulation modes: P = 0.01; Extended Data Fig. 9e).

Finally, to test our initial hypothesis, that is, whether increased co-occurrence of hippocampal and thalamocortical sleep oscillations mediates overnight memory consolidation, we examined the relationship between electrophysiology and memory (Fig. 4e). We observed a robust correlation between recognition memory accuracy change and increase in co-occurrence of sleep oscillations—MTL ripples and neocortical slow waves (n = 30 MTL-cortical electrode pairs in eight participants; Spearman correlation calculated for all pairs ρ = 0.8, P = 0.007; Methods). When focusing only on smaller subsets of data (for example, only sync-stimulation data, or triple co-occurrence of ripples, spindles and slow waves; Fig. 4e), we observed positive correlations that did not reach significance (ρ = 0.8, P = 0.1, n = 18 pairs; and ρ = 0.7, P = 0.2, n = 12 pairs, respectively). Together, our results support the notion that improving the temporal coupling between human MTL ripples and thalamocortical sleep oscillations is key for overnight memory consolidation.

Discussion

Dynamically modulating the coupling between MTL ripples, cortical slow waves and thalamocortical spindles in human sleep via intracranial DBS synchronized in real time with MTL slow-wave active periods reveals a robust relation between MTL–neocortical coupling and overnight consolidation of recognition memory. In addition, we show that sync-stimulation enhances spindle activity and improves phase locking of brain-wide neuronal spiking activity to MTL slow waves. Importantly, across all participants receiving either sync-stimulation or mixed-phase stimulation, individual overnight changes in memory accuracy are tightly correlated with these electrophysiological effects. The efficacy of the closed-loop intervention was observed when applying temporally precise stimulation relative to local MTL activity—made possible by its real-time monitoring—but not with stimulation that was not precisely timed.

Our data demonstrate that multiple neocortical areas distant from the stimulation focus, even in the contralateral hemisphere, undergo similar coupling to the MTL following prefrontal sync-stimulation, as does the prefrontal cortex adjacent to the stimulation site. Wide-brain prolonged effects are likely made possible by the unique state of brain activity during sleep that allows local, low-amplitude intracranial stimulation to propagate effectively across wide cortical territories50. While we establish that prefrontal white-matter DBS leads to robust electrophysiological and memory effects, stimulation in other neocortical sites may also lead to similar effects. Nevertheless, the effects observed with our stimulation sites are consistent both with the efficacy of white-matter stimulation37,51,52,53,54 and with the known role of MTL–prefrontal interactions in memory20,22,33,34.

Paired-associate learning (PAL) paradigms have been widely used in studies of human sleep and declarative memory ever since the early pioneering studies by Jenkins and Dallenbach1. Typically, word-pair associations are used to reveal that sleep is associated with reduced forgetting compared with wake intervals39,42. In this study, we used a visual paired-associate learning (vPAL) task55 of image associations, using a naturalistic approach suited for clinical settings in which learning took place during a one-shot viewing session. We tested participants on their memory for the associations, as well as introducing ‘lure’ images to test for recognition memory. The same lures were used in evening and morning testing, so the morning test can be described as a source recognition test56 requiring participants to distinguish learned images from lures they have seen before but in a different context. With this task, we found that our intervention improved recognition memory accuracy, measured by the difference between correct recollection (hit rate) and false lure acceptance (false alarms; Fig. 1g and Extended Data Fig. 4c), whereas pairing performance was only modestly increased (Extended Data Fig. 4a,b). Our data suggest that the dominant factor in the performance increase associated with sync-stimulation was a reduction in falsely tagging lures (Extended Data Fig. 4e), in line with previous studies that pointed to the effect of sleep on minimizing false memory formation57,58,59. The fact that sleep specifically benefited recognition accuracy in this memory task was also reported in a separate cohort of healthy participants55. Several factors may contribute to the difference between our results, where association performance was not significantly impacted, and previous PAL-based studies. Such factors include lower initial memory strength related to the lack of rehearsal during the learning phase, the use of an immediate memory retrieval that may constitute a re-consolidation step, and the type of stimuli used (easily recognizable images of celebrities versus words; see also Discussion in ref. 55). Additional studies are needed to determine how sleep affects consolidation of memory accuracy and association.

The participants in the current study were individuals with medically refractory epilepsy. Their pathology and medication can affect sleep in multiple dimensions (reviewed in ref. 60). Briefly, refractory temporal lobe epilepsy is associated with excessive daytime sleepiness and changes to sleep architecture61 when nocturnal seizures may occur. Anti-epileptic drugs reduce the amount of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep62, and changes in the dynamics and power of slow waves and sleep spindles have been observed in this population61,63. In addition, IEDs occur preferentially during NREM sleep64, whereby elevated neuronal synchrony within thalamocortical networks facilitates the spread of focal IEDs to distant brain areas65. While we acknowledge that these aspects represent potential confounding factors, several observations suggest that these issues do not likely play a major role in our findings. First, anticipating variability in participant age, cognitive abilities, epilepsy profile and medication regime (Supplementary Table 2), we designed our cognitive paradigm as a ‘within-participant’ design (rather than a within-group comparison) thereby minimizing the contributions of these factors. Second, we observed similar behavioral and electrophysiology results despite variability in clinical profiles (Supplementary Table 1) and medication regimes (Supplementary Table 2), arguing against a major role. Third, while previous studies suggest that IEDs may impair memory by interfering with physiological hippocampal–cortical coupling66, we did not observe an association between recognition memory accuracy and the degree to which stimulation affected IEDs (Methods and Extended Data Fig. 8b), arguing against the possibility that memory benefits reported here are driven by IED reduction. Additional studies are needed to generalize findings based on this participant population to the general population.

Where do our results stand in relation to previous literature on boosting memory via closed-loop manipulations and sleep interventions? In humans, both open-loop and closed-loop intracranial electrical stimulation during the encoding phase have been previously reported in awake participants—by our laboratory and others—to benefit memory performance51,67,68,69. The current study highlights an alternative approach of interventions during offline memory consolidation, where sleep offers a privileged window of opportunity70. Previous studies have used closed-loop acoustic stimulation to demonstrate enhancement of neocortical slow oscillations and sleep spindles that improves memory40,71,72, although some studies could not replicate memory effects despite strong effects on slow waves and spindles73,74. One unresolved issue in all these studies is what impact the stimulation had on hippocampal ripples. In our data, we find that even though stimulation did not increase the number of ripples, the temporal co-occurrence of neocortical slow waves and hippocampal ripples is a critical predictor of memory accuracy. Possibly, the degree to which previous manipulations modulated this co-occurrence may account for the discrepancies observed in the behavioral results but, unfortunately, hippocampal ripples were not measured in those studies due to the absence of deep recording electrodes. Thus, additional studies will be required to further address the degree to which modulation of hippocampal ripples per se is necessary to induce memory benefits via stimulation.

Several features of our experimental design make it unique among previous reports. These include: (i) using intracranial electrical stimulation in frontal lobe white matter aiming to influence prefrontal–hippocampal pathways34; (ii) timing the stimulations in real time based on MTL slow-wave active periods, which are not necessarily in sync with slow-wave up-states as recorded on the scalp32; this was made possible here because of our access to deep brain iEEG signals; (iii) we developed a brief, high-frequency stimulation scheme (Methods); (iv) our setup allowed simultaneous iEEG recordings from deep brain structures to evaluate coupling between MTL ripples to slow wave and spindle events; and (v) our ability to assess the effects of intervention on neuronal spiking activities. Our results suggest that timing the interventions to moments of MTL active periods is key to achieving the memory-enhancing effects given the difference between sync stimulation and mixed-phase control stimulation. This result is in agreement both with rodent studies, pointing to cross-brain synchrony during slow-wave active states as an important factor supporting successful learning and memory consolidation20,21,24 and with studies in humans, supporting the idea that these states offer a privileged window for interventions targeting memory consolidation36,40,75. However, we do not claim that the precise timing we have chosen in this report is necessarily optimal. Additional studies should test whether synchrony could be further enhanced by refinement of the stimulation timing.

To conclude, using a rare opportunity to perform an active intracranial intervention during natural sleep and while recording detailed iEEG signals and single-unit spiking from humans, we found a tight correlation between electrophysiological signatures of NREM sleep and overnight memory consolidation. Our results support present models of systems-level consolidation, whereby functional coupling between hippocampal ripples and thalamocortical sleep spindles and slow waves mediates fine-tuned communication between the human hippocampus and neocortex during sleep. Finally, the present study suggests an approach to benefit memory consolidation during the privileged period of sleep, that may be beneficial in future development of closed-loop clinical DBS devices for memory disorders and dementia37.

Methods

Participants

Eighteen participants with pharmacologically intractable epilepsy (11 women, 7 men, based on self-reported gender identity; Supplementary Table 1) who met clinical criteria for depth electrode placement at UCLA for identification of seizure foci and preparation for surgical treatment76. Participants were not compensated for participation in the study. The UCLA Institutional Review Board approved the study protocol. All participants provided written consent to participate in the study. Electrode location was based solely on clinical criteria. Predetermined clinical criteria guided placement of 9–14 Behnke–Fried electrodes (Adtech Medical, Racine WI) in each individual. Electrodes were implanted stereotactically with the aid of digital subtraction angiography or computed tomography (CT) angiography as well as magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)76. Each Behnke–Fried macro–micro depth electrode contained at least seven macroelectrode contacts (1.5 mm wide) spaced 1.5–3.5 mm apart along the shaft, and a Behnke–Fried inner platinum-iridium microwire bundle (California Fine Wire)76 (Extended Data Fig. 6a(i)). All surgeries were performed by I.F. Sixteen participants were tested in two experimental sessions, as detailed below (Supplementary Table 2), while two participants underwent mixed-phase stimulation on intervention nights (without a night of undisturbed sleep), designed to serve as a control for physiological effects. Each participant’s drug regimen at the time of recording is listed in Supplementary Table 2.

Experimental design

Participants were tested in two experimental sessions: an intervention condition (sleep with RTCL stimulation) and a control condition (undisturbed sleep), with the order of conditions and test versions (image pairs), counterbalanced across participants (Fig. 1a and Supplementary Table 2). The participants’ two experimental conditions were separated by an interval of 1–5 d (Supplementary Table 2). On the day of each experimental session, participants did not take any naps nor drank coffee in the 6 h preceding the experiment. In each condition, participants (i) first performed a declarative memory task (image-pair associates, below) between 20:00 and 22:30 (learning phase), (ii) were tested on their memory (after a short break) following learning, (iii) went to sleep; in stimulation nights, RTCL stimulation started after at least 30 min of consolidated sleep when online polysomnography indicated unequivocal NREM sleep and was discontinued 90–140 min later, and (iv) completed a memory recall examination session (retrieval phase) in the morning 30–60 min after awakening (Supplementary Table 2). Of 16 participants tested on two experimental nights, three exhibited poor memory accuracy scores (recognition memory accuracy < 0.1 in one of the tests, suggesting low attention or misunderstanding of the task, marked with an asterisk in Supplementary Table 2) and were excluded from further memory accuracy analysis (but included in neurophysiological analysis). One additional participant underwent cognitive testing but had a very low number of stimulations delivered (<100) and was excluded altogether from the cognitive cohort (Supplementary Table 2).

Cognitive testing and overnight memory paradigm

Paired associative learning (PAL) has previously shown to be sensitive to the effect of sleep1,40,77,78,79. We adapted the task for participants to make it relatively short (20 min for the learning phase, 5–10 min for the retrieval phase), and based on visual stimuli (vPAL; see also ref. 55). Twenty-five color images of famous people were paired with 25 different animals (their ‘pets’). Image pairs were presented on a laptop computer at the participant’s bedside. Participants studied each image pair for 2 s followed by 2 s of fixation and were asked to memorize the pair and to name the animal’s type. After viewing each pair once, participants were given a short break in which the rest of the experiment was explained. Next, single images of people were presented for 2 s (25 learned images were mixed with 15 novel (‘lure’) images of famous people). First, participants were asked whether the person was a previously learned pet owner, without any feedback on their answer. Next, if they had identified a person as a ‘pet owner’, they were asked what kind of animal they owned (pairing). For each image, we determined whether it was correctly identified as new (lure) or old, the reaction time of that response, and whether the associated pet was correctly identified (when applicable). After an overnight sleep period (Supplementary Table 2; average delay ± standard deviation between the first and second tests, 10.7 ± 1.3 h on intervention night and 10.3 h ± 2.7 h on undisturbed nights, respectively), participants were retested (same 40 images as in the evening test, in a scrambled order) without feedback, and behavioral measures were compared for pre-sleep and post-sleep tests.

Recognition accuracy for each test was defined as:

$$\mathrm{Accuracy} = \frac{{N_{\mathrm{correct}}\;{\mathrm{recognition}}}}{{25}} - \frac{{N_{\mathrm{false}}\;{\mathrm{recognition}}}}{{15}}$$

Overnight memory change was quantified as:

$$\Delta {\mathrm{Accuracy}} = {\mathrm{Accuracy}_{morning}} - {\mathrm{Accuracy}_{evening}}$$

The efficacy of RTCL stimulation was evaluated by comparing overnight performance changes:

$$\begin{array}{l}\mathrm{Intervention}\;\mathrm{efficacy}\\ = {{\Delta }}\mathrm{Accuracy}_{\mathrm{RTCL}\;\mathrm{intervention}\;\mathrm{night}} - {{\Delta }}\mathrm{Accuracy}_{\mathrm{undisturbed}\;\mathrm{sleep}\;\mathrm{night}}\end{array}$$

This within-participant comparison was performed to distill the effects of the intervention (sleep with RTCL stimulation compared to undisturbed sleep), independent of the expected inter-participant variability in baseline long-term memory performance31. If the intervention had no consistent effect, we would expect by chance that the intervention night would show superior performance in about half the participants. To test this, we used the binomial cumulative distribution function (binocdf, MATLAB, MathWorks) to assess the probability of our data (observing superior performance on intervention night relative to undisturbed night for 6/6 participants), against the null hypothesis that the probability for each participant to have superior performance on stimulation nights is 0.5. To obtain estimates and 95% confidence intervals of the percentage of participants who had superior performance on intervention nights, we computed the likelihood of the data given a binomial model (binofit, MATLAB, MathWorks). To estimate confidence intervals for individual participants’ intervention efficacy, we ran a bootstrapping procedure as follows: for every test set, we selected 25 images and 15 lures (with repetitions) out of each test’s image set and calculated participant scores based on the selected set. We repeated this 1,000 times for each participant. We calculated the mean and standard deviation of the bootstrapped scores for correct recognitions, false alarms and recognition memory accuracy for each participant (plotted in Extended Data Fig. 4c–e). We also computed the mean across participants for each bootstrap iteration to estimate a distribution of means (shown in the insets of Extended Data Fig. 4c–e).

Before the evening learning session and following the morning testing session, participants performed a face/non-face categorization PVT as described in ref. 38 to quantify vigilance. In brief, during each block, four face images and two non-face images (places or animals) were presented on a laptop computer for 200 ms while participants performed a face/non-face categorization task. Each picture was presented 24 times in a pseudorandom order (total of 144 trials), with long pseudorandomized inter-stimulus intervals of 2–8 s (uniform distribution), as in classical PVT designs80. Participants were instructed to press one of two buttons (for face versus non-face) as quickly as possible. We used the difference between median reaction times between morning and evening in the visual PVT task to assess changes in vigilance following intervention nights and undisturbed nights (Extended Data Fig. 4f,g). Subjective sleepiness was assessed at the beginning of each experimental session using the Stanford Sleepiness Scale and a visual analog rating of sleepiness.

Electrode localization

Depth electrode placement was determined solely based on clinical considerations. Before electrode implantation, we obtained for each participant a T1-weighted 1-mm isometric structural MR scan using a 3-Tesla scanner. After implantation, a CT scan was acquired and co-registered via an affine transform to the preoperative anatomical MR scan (after skull stripping) using FSL’s BET and FLIRT toolbox (FMRIB v6.0)81,82,83. This allowed visualization of the CT scan superimposed with the preoperative MRI scan (Fig. 1c and Extended Data Fig. 2). Individual recording sites were then identified visually on the co-registered CT and manually marked in each participant’s preoperative MRI native space using BioImage Suite (RRID: SCR_016109)84. Co-registration and electrode localization were performed using the iELVIS toolbox84: The preimplantation three-dimensional T1 MR scan was processed using FreeSurfer to segment the white matter, deep gray matter structures and cortex; and to parcellate the neocortex according to gyral anatomy85,86. Each iEEG electrode was then attributed to a cortical region according to automated parcellation in FreeSurfer85. We warped the aligned electrodes onto a standard brain template (using MNI template) to facilitate group-level visualization (Figs. 1d, 2d, 3c and 4b). The MNI reconstruction was performed for visualization purposes only, and electrode localizations were always determined in native MR space. All stimulation sites were verified to reside in white matter and all MTL probe locations in gray matter by a neurologist and a neurosurgeon. iEEG electrode contacts used for ripple detection (see below) were verified to be in hippocampus/entorhinal cortex/parahippocampal gyrus gray matter. We defined MTL–neocortical iEEG couples in the following manner (Fig. 4b): the ‘ripple channel’ was an MTL iEEG electrode with ripple detections in pre-stimulation sleep, while the ‘prefrontal channel’ was a prefrontal iEEG electrode with the highest number of spindle event detections during pre-stimulation sleep in the same hemisphere (in one pair/participant, we paired MTL and prefrontal contacts from opposite hemispheres). When using the MNI brain template to demonstrate widespread effects (Fig. 2d), score values outside the 5–95 percentiles are displayed in minimum/maximum colors for better visualization.

Stimulation

A board-certified neurologist was present in each stimulation session to monitor the clinical iEEG recordings for after-discharges and ensure participant safety. Stimulation of epileptogenic areas was avoided when possible and the neurologist validated stimulation site and impedance before each session. Before every experimental session, each participant was given a short series of test stimulation pulses while a neurologist monitored the clinical iEEG recordings for after-discharges and ensuring stimulations were correctly delivered. Unaware of the exact timing of stimulation onset, participants were asked at the end of each session (in the morning) to report any unusual feelings or sensations. Participants did not report any effects of stimulation, nor could they indicate when stimulation occurred during the night. In 12/19 sessions, experiments occurred >10 h since last seizure, and in 7 sessions, experiments occurred >2 h since last seizure. A board-certified neurologist validated that no seizures were detected during intervention nights. Stimulation was current regulated and charge balanced, with pulses set below the threshold for after discharge, which was identified based on pretesting (range: 1.0–2.0 mA). Stimulation electrode impedance was measured immediately before testing (range, 1–4 kΩ, using clinical Neurofax EEG-1200A system, Nihon Koden). Stimulations were delivered in one of two schemes (Supplementary Table 3): (i) bipolar stimulation (participants 1–3) used a CereStim R96 Macro-stimulator (BlackRock Microsystems) to deliver electrical stimulation to the Behnke–Fried depth electrode bipolar macro-contacts spaced 3.5 mm apart (surface area, 0.06 cm2)69, and (ii) unipolar stimulation (participants 4–18) with the electrodes referenced to the EEG GND electrode. Each 50-ms-long stimulation event included five rectangular pulses (pulse width of 100 μs) at a frequency of 100 Hz, with the current ranging from 0.5 to 1.5 mA. Stimulation ranged between 2.5 and 7.6 μC of charge per square centimeter per phase, which is well below the safe maximum used for long-term and short-term stimulation (30 and 57 μC, respectively)87,88.

Timing of real-time closed-loop stimulation by online detection of slow waves in medial temporal lobe probe

A neural signal processor (NSP; Cerebrus system, BlackRock Microsystems) connected to a separate laptop was used to detect slow waves online to time electrical stimulation events. With this setup, the predefined probe’s iEEG signal was fed to a custom code running on the laptop (MATLAB, MathWorks) in parallel to its recording by the NSP. The signal was low-pass filtered at <500 Hz and sampled at 2,000 Hz by the NSP. A custom-made script (running on MATLAB, MathWorks) using the NSP’s application programming interface (BlackRock Microsystems) enabled responding to the incoming iEEG data in real time. To this end, the iEEG signal was further band-passed filtered between 0.5 Hz and 4 Hz (50th order FIR filter). Thus, during 5-min stimulation blocks, each time the low-passed signal crossed an adaptive threshold toward larger positive values, the electrical stimulation (details above) was triggered with a delay (details below). By default, the threshold was set to 80 µV, and the delay was either based on pre-recorded sleep iEEG from participants or set to be 400 ms. The threshold was updated every 400 s to be the median iEEG amplitude of slow waves detected within the preceding a 400-s interval, and the delay from detected peak to putative active phase was updated based on average values of peak to trough of detected slow events in the same interval. This algorithm ensured a reliable way to continuously detect slow waves with dynamic amplitudes (for example, modulated by sleep depth and other factors) by their positive half-wave peaks40. The detection routine was resumed 3 s after the delivery of stimulation to ensure sufficient buffers to evaluate slow-wave activity between stimulation events. After recording 7–15 min of uninterrupted NREM sleep, stimulation blocks were administered in 5-min blocks, interleaved with 5-min pause blocks (‘pause’ intervals; Fig. 1b). At least 15–20 min of post-stimulation uninterrupted sleep data were recorded after the last stimulation block. In a post hoc analysis, we measured the delay between each stimulation to the MTL probe’s peak that immediately preceded it (corresponding to the down/inactive phase in iEEG signals32) and found the mean delay to be 241.3 ms for the sync-stimulation group and 373.3 ms for the mixed-stimulation group. We used post hoc analysis to quantify the degree to which stimulation timings were in phase with MTL active periods: We required >55% of stimulations to be within 80–280 ms of the closest peak to be included in the sync-stimulation group (Extended Data Fig. 3b). For participant 17 where post hoc analysis was not possible due to noise issues, we used the median delay from the peak based on the real-time log.

Sleep scoring

Our approach focused on detecting epochs of NREM sleep based on iEEG signals. We were able to reliably detect NREM epochs in every recording, even when full polysomnography (which would allow reliable separation of wakefulness from REM sleep) was not available. To guide the initiation of stimulation blocks online, visual detection of NREM sleep epochs was performed at the participant bedside by a physician board certified in clinical neurophysiology. Visual detection focused on unequivocal presence of slow waves and sleep spindles in iEEG data. To guide offline detailed analysis, automatic detection of NREM sleep was performed focusing on the presence of robust slow waves and sleep spindle activities in iEEG data89 as follows. First, for each participant separately, we selected a neocortical iEEG channel distant from the seizure onset zone, where interictal activity was minimal, and—whenever possible—from a contact placed in medial prefrontal/parietal cortex with prominent sleep spindle occurrence35. Second, after removing residual interictal discharge activities (see ‘iEEG preprocessing and detection of pathological events’), we calculated the short-time Fourier transform (30-s window, no overlap, 0–40 Hz range, 0.2-Hz resolution; Fig. 1b). Note that an additional normalization by a two-dimensional Gaussian filter (σ = 3) was used for visualization purposes in the figure but not for scoring. Third, we averaged the power in the slow-wave (0.5–4 Hz) and spindle (9–16 Hz) frequency bands for each 30-s time point, resulting in two vectors representing slow-wave and spindle power. Fourth, we fit a two-component Gaussian mixture distribution to the slow-wave–spindle joint vectors to represent synchronized NREM epochs versus desynchronized (REM/wakefulness) epochs. Fifth, we calculated the posterior probability for each component given each time point (fitgmdist and posterior, MATLAB, MathWorks) and tagged each time point according to the maximal posterior probability (NREM versus desynchronized; Extended Data Fig. 1a). Finally, standalone NREM detections (30 s only) that were >1 min away from other NREM detections were discarded according to AASM guidelines90.

We validated our approach by comparing the automated iEEG-based scheme used here with gold-standard polysomnography-based sleep scoring performed by an expert using AASM guidelines with three pre-scored overnight recordings from an independent dataset32. The output of the data-driven Gaussian mixture approach was highly concordant with manual scoring (comparing the first 3 h of sleep in the three different overnight datasets, we found a <4% change in the lengths of NREM sleep bouts). The approach used here was more robust to inter-participant variability compared with other automated versions based only on predefined thresholds of delta power89. Importantly, post hoc analysis based on the iEEG-based sleep scoring confirmed that 76% ± 5.1% (average and s.e.m. over n = 19 sessions) of stimulation events occurred during NREM sleep.

Electrophysiology data acquisition and offline spike sorting

In each participant, 8–14 depth electrodes were implanted targeting medial brain areas. Each depth electrode had eight platinum iEEG contacts along the shaft (Extended Data Fig. 6a(i), referenced to the scalp). Both scalp and depth iEEG data were continuously recorded at a sampling rate of 2 kHz, band-pass filtered between 0.1 and 500 Hz, using either Blackrock or Neuralynx data acquisition systems. Each electrode terminated in eight 40-mm platinum-iridium microwires from which extracellular signals were continuously recorded (Extended Data Fig. 6a; referenced locally to a ninth non-insulated microwire) at a sampling rate of 28 or 30 kHz and band-pass-filtered between 1 and 6,000 Hz.

Spike sorting

Neuronal clusters were identified using the ‘Wave Clus v2’ software package91 as described previously32: Action potentials were detected by high-pass filtering the extracellular recordings above 300 Hz and applying a threshold at 5 s.d. above the median noise level. Detected events were clustered (or categorized as noise) using automatic superparamagnetic clustering of wavelet coefficients, followed by manual refinement based on the consistency of spike waveforms and inter-spike interval distributions (see example in Extended Data Fig. 6a(iii)). Unit stability throughout stimulation sessions was confirmed by verifying that spike waveforms and inter-spike interval distributions were consistent and distinct in the interval ranging from pre-stimulation through post-stimulation time points (1–2 h; Supplementary Table 2 and Extended Data Fig. 6a(iv)). Of 386 neural clusters identified by ‘Wave Clus’ (8 patients), 325 clusters (84%) were verified to be stable throughout the session and were included in further analysis.

Intracranial electroencephalography preprocessing and detection of pathological events

Data analysis was performed with MATLAB (MathWorks), using the FieldTrip92 and CircStat93 toolboxes as well as custom scripts. Preprocessing of the iEEG data began with line noise removal (2-Hz band-stop filters centered at 60 Hz and its harmonics) and followed by an automated algorithm to identify pathological events and electrical artifacts, as follows. First, for all data intervals occurring during NREM sleep, each time point was converted into a z-score based on the participant-specific and stage-specific mean and s.d. of absolute amplitude, gradient (the amplitude difference between two adjacent time points) and amplitude of the data after applying a 250-Hz high-pass filter. Next, epileptiform interictal spikes were detected automatically in iEEG signals by identifying events whose envelope of the high-passed signal was larger than a threshold of +5 s.d., or a conjunction of absolute amplitude and gradient both passing a threshold of +5 s.d. and whose duration was <70 ms35. Points that passed the detection condition and occurred in close temporal proximity (<50 ms) were merged as one interictal spike. Subsequent analysis of iEEG data was performed after detecting pathological events in each channel separately: we used a semi-manual process, identifying channels with gross deviations of kurtosis/amplitude/skewness relative to other channels on the same electrode and used visual validation as well as independent clinical neurologist channel-tagging to remove channels with high rates of interictal activity (>5 events per minute) or with electrical noise. In channels included in further analysis, 500 ms preceding and following any interictal spike detection were removed.

Single-event detection of sleep oscillations

Slow waves, sleep spindle and MTL ripple events were identified independently for each participant and channel, based on established detection algorithms35,44.

Slow waves

Slow waves were detected as in ref. 44. First, artifact-free iEEG signals from the NREM sleep stage were filtered between 0.16 Hz and 1.25 Hz (two-pass FIR band-pass filter, order = three cycles of the low-frequency cutoff). Second, all zero-crossings were determined in the filtered signal, and event duration was determined for slow-wave candidates (that is, events consisting of an inactive/‘OFF’ period corresponding to iEEG peak, followed by an active/’ON’ period corresponding to iEEG trough32) as the time between two successive negative-to-positive zero-crossings. For events whose duration was between 0.8 s and 2 s, event amplitudes were determined (peak-to-trough amplitude between two negative-to-positive zero-crossings). Events that also met the amplitude criteria (≥75% percentile of candidate amplitudes, that is, the 25% of events with the largest amplitudes) were considered as slow waves.

Sleep spindles

Spindles were detected automatically via a two-step process based on ref. 35. First, to minimize false detections, only channels with robust spindle activity in NREM sleep were chosen for further analysis. To this end, in each individual channel, sigma (9–16 Hz) power in NREM sleep was compared with a fitted 1/fα model (both were estimated across multiple 10-s epochs) and channels with a difference that was statistically significant at P < 0.001 (unpaired t-test for maximal peak) were further considered. Second, putative spindle events were selected based on their power and duration: iEEG signals were band-pass filtered between 9 Hz and 16 Hz using a zero-phase fourth-order Butterworth filter. The instantaneous amplitude was computed via the Hilbert transform and two thresholds were defined based on this amplitude time course across artifact-free sleep epochs. A detection threshold was set at the mean + 3 s.d. and amplitudes exceeding this threshold were considered potential spindles. A start/end threshold was set at the mean + 1 s.d., and events whose duration was between 0.5 s and 2 s were further considered. Detections within 1 s were merged as single events. We verified the spectral specificity of each spindle by excluding any detection that coincided with control events that were above the mean + 5 s.d in the 20–30 Hz range. For single-event co-occurrence analysis (see below), we used a subpopulation of fast spindles where detection required a minimum frequency > 11 Hz. For every detected spindle, the peaks and troughs were detected as the maxima and minima of the filtered signal, and the maximal peak was designated as the time point that represented the respective spindle in time (for example, for single-event co-occurrence analysis and population average).

Medial temporal lobe ripples

We utilized bipolar referencing to minimize effects of volume conduction by identifying, in each electrode shaft targeting the MTL separately, a contact residing in white matter to be used as a reference for single-ripple detection, using preoperative and postoperative CT and MRI data. We then used an automated detection algorithm as in ref. 44. First, data were filtered between 80 Hz and 100 Hz (two-pass FIR band-pass filter, order = three cycles of the low-frequency cutoff), and only artifact-free data from NREM sleep were used for event detection. Second, the root-mean-square (RMS) signal was calculated for the filtered signal using a moving average of 20 ms, and the ripple amplitude criterion was defined as the 99% percentile of RMS values. Third, whenever the signal exceeded this threshold for a minimum of 38 ms (encompassing ~3 cycles at 80 Hz) a putative ripple event was marked. In addition, to avoid sharp broadband events, only those putative ripple events representing a true oscillatory pattern were considered for further analysis. Accordingly, we focused on events with at least three discrete peaks or three discrete troughs in the raw signal corresponding to the above-threshold RMS segment. This was accomplished by identifying local maxima or minima in the respective raw signal segments after applying a one-pass moving average filter including the two adjacent data points. We demanded a detection of 20 ripples in pre-sleep baseline to include a contact as a ripple channel. Of 45 candidate MTL channels (18 participants), 17 were excluded because ripple rate was too low (13) or baseline noise was too high (7).

Single-event co-occurrence

Slow-wave–spindle sequences (Extended Data Fig. 5c,f) were defined similarly to ref. 20 as epochs where spindle peaks occurred up to 1.5 s following iEEG slow-wave positive peak (down/OFF phase) on a specific iEEG contact. We also evaluated coupling incidence of single sleep events in cross-brain electrode pairs. In each participant, we paired one contact from MTL electrodes with one contact from frontal-cortex electrode on the same hemisphere (when possible, the superior temporal gyrus was used when the frontal electrode was not available, one participant only has a pair from opposite hemispheres), with maximal spindle activity. Of 55 candidate MTL–neocortical electrode pairs (18 participants), 13 were excluded because MTL channel was excluded from ripple analysis (see above). Of the resulting 42 couples, in one pair MTL contact and neocortical contact were in different hemispheres, hence it was not included in the Fig. 4b visualization. MTL ripple–cortical slow-wave couples corresponded to epochs where ripple peaks were 50–400 ms away from the slow-wave positive peak (down/OFF phase), including cortical contacts with >10 slow waves in the evaluated period. Co-occurrence of MTL ripple and neocortical slow-wave/spindle sequences corresponded (similar to ref. 20) to an MTL ripple peak preceding a slow-wave–spindle sequence by 50–400 ms.

Stimulation-locked time–frequency analysis

Stimulation-triggered analyses were performed for stimulation time points confirmed to occur during NREM sleep following post hoc sleep scoring (‘Sleep scoring’). TFRs (Figs. 2a and 4a) were extracted by calculating a spectrogram around stimulation events (0–2.5 s) and subtracting from it the pre-stimulation (−1 to 0 s) baseline spectrogram44. Spectrograms were calculated using ft_specest_mtmconvol (FieldTrip toolbox92, MATLAB, MathWorks, frequencies 5–30 Hz, 1-Hz resolution) using a sliding Hanning-tapered window with a variable, frequency-dependent, length that comprised at least five cycles44. Time-locked TFRs of all stimulation events were then normalized as the percentage change from pre-event baseline and were averaged for each session (Fig. 2a).

To estimate stimulation-locked average TFR increase in spindle frequency band, above and beyond the expected based on these time periods during slow-wave active phases, we generated a set of sham-stimulation points as detailed and used those to calculate a sham-locked TFR as a baseline for comparison (Fig. 2(aii)). Slow-wave peaks were detected in ‘pause’ (stimulation-free) 5-min blocks and a random subset of them was selected (equal in number to the number of real stimulations in the same session), sham events were then selected with equal delay from peaks as the stimulations in the preceding block. Time-locked TFRs of all stimulation events and sham events were normalized as the percentage change from pre-event baseline and averaged per session for each iEEG channel (Fig. 2(aii)).

Sham points were selected offline using the same algorithm as used for online timing of stimulation events, such that they also reflected MTL OFF–ON transitions, to control for the tendency of ON periods to be associated with greater spindle activity. We also performed this analysis with an alternative selection of 1,000 random sham points during ‘pause’ sessions and observed similar results.

Single-event probability and event-rate estimation

To assess the probability of slow waves, spindles, slow-wave–spindle couples and ripples following stimulations (Figs. 2 and 4), we detected single events (see above) on each iEEG contact separately. To evaluate the immediate effect of stimulation on sleep oscillations (Fig. 2a and Extended Data Figs. 5a–c and 9a), we counted the detections during the 3 s following stimulation events (for slow waves and spindles) and during 200 ms following stimulation events (for ripples). Probabilities were calculated as the sum of detections during immediate short periods following stimulation bursts, divided by the number of stimulations in each session vectors. Note that for 3-s time vectors, event rates and probability values were very similar for slow waves and spindles, as they typically do not occur more than once during these time periods35. As a within-session control, we used an equal number of sham time points (explained in the previous section, above) to assess the degree to which stimulation increased event-detection probabilities, beyond the endogenous rates during active phases of slow waves. iEEG contacts with no detections in one of the terms were excluded from the analysis. We normalized this change by contrasting the probability (P) to detect an event following stimulation with the probability calculated for sham time points: \(\frac{{P_{\mathrm{stim}} - P_{\mathrm{sham}}}}{{P_{\mathrm{stim}} + P_{\mathrm{sham}}}}\). Spindle enhancement score per participant (Fig. 2d) was defined as the median of all spindle-increase scores for all iEEG contacts, excluding contacts that did not have any spindle detections in either one of the conditions (stim/sham).

For longer time epochs (the prolonged condition in Figs. 2 and 4 and Extended Data Figs. 5, 8 and 9), event rates were calculated as the number of detected events divided by the accumulated length of time. Event enhancement score in the prolonged condition (Fig. 2d) was similarly calculated as a contrast index between post-stimulation time vectors (1 min post-stimulation block) and the furthest equal-length time period in the remaining ‘pause’ block (that is, 1 min before the following stimulation block): \(\frac{\mathrm{Event}\;\mathrm{rate}_{\mathrm{post} - \mathrm{stim}} - \mathrm{Event}\;\mathrm{rate}_{\mathrm{pre} - \mathrm{stim}}}{{\mathrm{Event}\;\mathrm{rate}_{\mathrm{post} - \mathrm{stim}} + \mathrm{Event}\;\mathrm{rate}_{\mathrm{pre} - \mathrm{stim}}}}\).

Estimating phase locking of neural spiking activity by fitting a cosine function

All analyses were performed using MATLAB (MathWorks). To quantify the degree of phase locking between MTL slow-wave oscillations and neuronal spiking activity in different brain regions (Fig. 3), we fitted a cosine function to the distribution of spike phases relative to phase values of the MTL probe’s iEEG signal94. For each neural cluster and each condition (baseline, stimulation as described below), we repeated the fitting procedure to create a unique lock-depth measure for each condition (Extended Data Fig. 6b). First, we computed the instantaneous amplitude of the MTL probe’s iEEG signal via the Hilbert transform following a Butterworth band-pass filter between 0.5 Hz and 2 Hz (zero-phase filtering via filtfilt). We then extracted the probe’s phase for each neural spike. We used 20-degree bins to create a histogram of spike phases (−180 < φ < 180). We fitted every spike-phase histogram with the follomatlabwing function: \(f(\varphi ) = a \times \cos (\varphi + b) + c\) (Extended Data Fig. 6b). We computed the R2 value between the original spike-histogram and the fit. We found R2 > 0.25 to have a good correspondence with the Rayleigh test for non-uniformity of phases (calculated by circ_rtest), as >90% of distributions over this value had passed P < 0.05, but the fitting procedure was less sensitive to variations in distribution shape than the circular statistics.

We defined an index to capture the phase-locking depth (LD) of oscillatory modulation in our fit as follows: \(\mathrm{LD} = \frac{{2a}}{{(2 \times a + c)}}\).

We included in our analysis 325 neural units validated as having a stable inter-spike-interval probability distribution (see above). For each condition, we analyzed phase locking for neural units with a minimum mean firing rate of 0.1 Hz. We included in the phase-locking change analysis (Fig. 3c and Extended Data Fig. 6c) neural units with significant phase locking during evaluated conditions (Rayleigh test, P < 0.05). To describe in full the immediate and prolonged changes in phase locking, we defined several conditions for the evaluation of spiking activity. To assess the change during stimulation blocks, baseline values were based on neural activity during the uninterrupted 5-min sleep period before the first intervention (‘PRE’; Fig. 1b). During stimulation blocks (used in Fig. 3b), we excluded spikes that occurred within the 500 ms following stimulation events. We defined a prolonged condition as all 1-min post-stimulation blocks. Phase-locking depth change was evaluated by the following index (Fig. 3c,d and Extended Data Fig. 6c, d): \(\frac{{\mathrm{LD}_{\mathrm{post} - \mathrm{stim}} - \mathrm{LD}_{\mathrm{baseline}}}}{{\mathrm{LD}_{\mathrm{post} - \mathrm{stim}} + \mathrm{LD}_{\mathrm{baseline}}}}\).

We compared locking depth for units that had significant phase locking in compared conditions (Rayleigh test, P < 0.05). Note that the number of clusters with significant phase locking increased during the stimulation session, as demonstrated in Fig. 3b. These changes resulted in a varying number of units for each evaluated condition. We calculated lock-depth change for all various combinations of evaluated conditions versus the two different baselines used in the main text (‘PRE’ sleep and 1-min pre-stimulation blocks), all in agreement with the main analysis reported in Fig. 3c,d (see full distributions and statistics in Extended Data Fig. 6c).

We performed two separate shuffling procedures to validate the prolonged effect in phase locking—comparing the prolonged condition to the 1-min period before stimulation blocks (Fig. 3d): (1) We assigned lock-depth values to the tested condition or baseline randomly (randperm, MATLAB, MathWorks) 10,000 times and tested whether the randomly shuffled distribution differed significantly from the calculated distribution (Extended Data Fig. 6c). (2) To test against the hypothesis that changes in firing rates bias our phase-locking calculation, we performed the following procedure 1,000 times—for each comparison between conditions, we selected a set of X spikes from each condition; X totaled 90% of the minimum spike count of the two conditions. We refit a cosine as described above for both selected sets and recalculated phase-locking change. The resulting distribution was not significantly different from the distribution calculated based on the full number of spikes and significantly passed the Wilcoxon sign-rank test as the distribution reported in Fig. 3d (Extended Data Fig. 6c).

Statistical analyses

We used parametric methods for statistical testing of normal data. For non-normal data or small sample sizes, we used Wilcoxon signed-rank/rank-sum tests. To compare two distributions, we used the Kolmogorov–Smirnov two-sample test. All statistical tests were two sided unless stated otherwise. In violin plots representing estimated distributions of data (generated with violinplot, FieldTrip toolbox92, MATLAB, MathWorks), lines represent 5, 50 and 95 percentiles. No statistical methods were used to predetermine sample sizes but our sample sizes are similar to those generally used in previous publications44,73,95. Data collection and analysis were not performed blind to the conditions of the experiments.

Reporting summary

Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.