Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Registered Report
  • Published:

An experimental manipulation of the value of effort

Abstract

People who take on challenges and persevere longer are more likely to succeed in life. But individuals often avoid exerting effort, and there is limited experimental research investigating whether we can learn to value effort. We developed a paradigm to test the hypothesis that people can learn to value effort and will seek effortful challenges if directly incentivized to do so. We also dissociate the effects of rewarding people for choosing effortful challenges and performing well. The results provide limited evidence that rewarding effort increased people’s willingness to choose harder tasks when rewards were no longer offered (near transfer). There was also mixed evidence that rewarding effort increased willingness to choose harder tasks in another unrelated and unrewarded task (far transfer). These heterogeneous results highlight the need for further research to understand when this paradigm may be the most effective for increasing and generalizing the value of effort.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Design.
Fig. 2: Bayesian posterior densities for the effect of condition on effort preferences.
Fig. 3: Effort preference relative to baseline preference in the pre-training section.
Fig. 4: Exploratory analyses and Bayesian posterior densities for the effect of condition on task accuracy.
Fig. 5: Effort preference correlations.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

Data and materials are provided at the repository https://osf.io/9unj5Source data are provided with this paper.

Code availability

Code is available at the repository https://osf.io/9unj5

References

  1. Disabato, D. J., Goodman, F. R. & Kashdan, T. B. Is grit relevant to well‐being and strengths? Evidence across the globe for separating perseverance of effort and consistency of interests. J. Personal. 87, 194–211 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Shoda, Y., Mischel, W. & Peake, P. K. Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification: identifying diagnostic conditions. Dev. Psychol. 26, 978–986 (1990).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Watts, T. W., Duncan, G. J. & Quan, H. Revisiting the marshmallow test: a conceptual replication investigating links between early delay of gratification and later outcomes. Psychol. Sci. 29, 1–19 (2018).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  4. Credé, M., Tynan, M. C. & Harms, P. D. Much ado about grit: a meta-analytic synthesis of the grit literature. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 113, 492–511 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. de Ridder, D. T., Lensvelt-Mulders, G., Finkenauer, C., Stok, F. M. & Baumeister, R. F. Taking stock of self-control: a meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 16, 76–99 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Moffitt, T. E. et al. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 2693–2698 (2011).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Inzlicht, M., Werner, K. M., Briskin, J. L. & Roberts, B. W. Integrating models of self-regulation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 72, 319–345 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Jackson, C. K. What do test scores miss? The importance of teacher effects on non–test score outcomes. J. Political Econ. 126, 2072–2107 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Kwon, H. W. The sociology of grit: exploring grit as a sociological variable and its potential role in social stratification. Sociol. Compass 11, e12544 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Roberts, B. W., Kuncel, N. R., Shiner, R., Caspi, A. & Goldberg, L. R. The power of personality: the comparative validity of personality traits, socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2, 313–345 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Tough, P. How kids learn resilience. Atlantic 317, 56–66 (2016).

    Google Scholar 

  12. Hull, C. L. Principles of Behavior: an Introduction to Behavior Theory (Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1943).

  13. Kool, W. & Botvinick, M. Mental labour. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 899–908 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B. & Botvinick, M. M. Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 139, 665–682 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Kurzban, R. The sense of effort. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 7, 67–70 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Shenhav, A. et al. Toward a rational and mechanistic account of mental effort. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 40, 99–124 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Westbrook, A., Kester, D. & Braver, T. S. What is the subjective cost of cognitive effort? Load, trait, and aging effects revealed by economic preference. PLoS ONE 8, e68210 (2013).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Friedrich, A. M. & Zentall, T. R. Pigeons shift their preference toward locations of food that take more effort to obtain. Behav. Process. 67, 405–415 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Inzlicht, M., Shenhav, A. & Olivola, C. Y. The effort paradox: effort is both costly and valued. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 337–349 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Lydall, E. S., Gilmour, G. & Dwyer, D. M. Rats place greater value on rewards produced by high effort: an animal analogue of the ‘effort justification’ effect. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 1134–1137 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Norton, M. I., Mochon, D. & Ariely, D. The IKEA effect: when labor leads to love. J. Consum. Psychol. 22, 453–460 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Dunn, T. L., Gaspar, C. & Risko, E. F. Cue awareness in avoiding effortful control. Neuropsychologia 123, 77–91 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Dunn, T. L., Inzlicht, M. & Risko, E. F. Anticipating cognitive effort: roles of perceived error-likelihood and time demands. Psychol. Res. 83, 1033–1056 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Constantinidis, C. & Klingberg, T. The neuroscience of working memory capacity and training. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 17, 1–12 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Enriquez-Geppert, S., Huster, R. J. & Herrmann, C. S. Boosting brain functions: improving executive functions with behavioral training, neurostimulation, and neurofeedback. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 88, 1–16 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J. & Shah, P. Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 10081–10086 (2011).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Melby-Lervåg, M. & Hulme, C. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev. Psychol. 49, 270–291 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Melby-Lervag, M., Redick, T. S. & Hulme, C. Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of ‘far transfer’: evidence from a meta-analytic review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 11, 512–534 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Owen, A. M. et al. Putting brain training to the test. Nature 465, 775–778 (2010).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S. & Engle, R. W. Is working memory training effective? Psychol. Bull. 138, 628–654 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Simons, D. J. et al. Do ‘brain-training’ programs work? Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 17, 103–186 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. von Bastian, C. C., Guye, S. & De Simoni, C. in Cognitive and Working Memory Training: Perspectives from Psychology, Neuroscience, and Human Development (eds Novick J. M. et al.) pp 1–23 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2019).

  33. Miles, E. et al. Does self-control improve with practice? Evidence from a six-week training program. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 145, 1075–1091 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Friese, M., Frankenbach, J., Job, V. & Loschelder, D. D. Does self-control training improve self-control? A meta-analysis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 1077–1099 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Lee, B. M. & Kemmelmeier, M. How reliable are the effects of self-control training? A re-examination using self-report and physical measures. PLoS ONE 12, e0178814 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Alan, S., Boneva, T. & Ertac, S. Ever failed, try again, succeed better: results from a randomized educational intervention on grit. Q. J. Econ. 134, 1–130 (2019).

  37. Lazowski, R. A. & Hulleman, C. S. Motivation interventions in education: a meta-analytic review. Rev. Educ. Res. 86, 602–640 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Yeager, D. S. et al. Using design thinking to improve psychological interventions: the case of the growth mindset during the transition to high school. J. Educ. Psychol. 108, 374–391 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Yeager, D. S. et al. Teaching a lay theory before college narrows achievement gaps at scale. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, E3341–E3348 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Foliano, F., Rolfe, H., Buzzeo, J., Runge, J. & Wilkinson, D. Changing Mindsets: Effectiveness Trial (National Institute of Economic and Social Research, 2019).

  41. Li, Y. & Bates, T. C. You can’t change your basic ability, but you work at things, and that’s how we get hard things done: Testing the role of growth mindset on response to setbacks, educational attainment, and cognitive ability. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 148, 1640–1655 (2019).

  42. Sisk, V. F., Burgoyne, A. P., Sun, J., Butler, J. L. & Macnamara, B. N. To what extent and under which circumstances are growth mind-sets important to academic achievement? Two meta-analyses. Psychol. Sci. 29, 549–571 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Yeager, D. S. et al. A national experiment reveals where a growth mindset improves achievement. Nature 573, 364–369 (2019).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Crespi, L. P. Quantitative variation of incentive and performance in the white rat. Am. J. Psychol. 55, 467–517 (1942).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Eisenberger, R. Learned industriousness. Psychol. Rev. 99, 248–267 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. O’Doherty, J. P., Cockburn, J. & Pauli, W. M. Learning, reward, and decision making. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 68, 73–100 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Leonard, J. A., Lee, Y. & Schulz, L. E. Infants make more attempts to achieve a goal when they see adults persist. Science 357, 1290–1294 (2017).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Gunderson, E. A. et al. Parent praise to 1- to 3-year-olds predicts children’s motivational frameworks 5 years later. Child Dev. 84, 1526–1541 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Lucca, K., Horton, R. & Sommerville, J. A. Infants rationally decide when and how to deploy effort. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 372–379 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Boksem, M. A. & Tops, M. Mental fatigue: costs and benefits. Brain Res. Rev. 59, 125–139 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Botvinick, M. & Braver, T. Motivation and cognitive control: from behavior to neural mechanism. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 83–113 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Kurniawan, I. T., Guitart-Masip, M., Dayan, P. & Dolan, R. J. Effort and valuation in the brain: the effects of anticipation and execution. J. Neurosci. 33, 6160–6169 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Massar, S. A., Lim, J., Sasmita, K. S. & Chee, M. W. Rewards boost sustained attention through higher effort: a value-based decision making approach. Biol. Psychol. 120, 21–27 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Padmala, S. & Pessoa, L. Reward reduces conflict by enhancing attentional control and biasing visual cortical processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3419–3432 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Umemoto, A. & Holroyd, C. B. Task-specific effects of reward on task switching. Psychol. Res. 79, 698–707 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Varazzani, C., San-Galli, A., Gilardeau, S. & Bouret, S. Noradrenaline and dopamine neurons in the reward/effort trade-off: a direct electrophysiological comparison in behaving monkeys. J. Neurosci. 35, 7866–7877 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Deci, E. L., Koestner, R. & Ryan, R. M. A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychol. Bull. 125, 627–668 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Kohn, A. Punished by Rewards: the Trouble with Gold Stars, Incentive Plans, A’s, Praise, and Other Bribes (Houghton Mifflin Harcourt, 1999).

    Google Scholar 

  59. Tang, S. & Hall, V. C. The overjustification effect: a meta‐analysis. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 365–404 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Henderlong Corpus, J. & Lepper, M. R. The effects of person versus performance praise on children’s motivation: gender and age as moderating factors. Educ. Psychol. 27, 487–508 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Mueller, C. M. & Dweck, C. S. Praise for intelligence can undermine children’s motivation and performance. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 75, 33–52 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  62. Amsel, A. Frustrative nonreward in partial reinforcement and discrimination learning: some recent history and a theoretical extension. Psychol. Rev. 69, 306–328 (1962).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. McCuller, T., Wong, P. T. P. & Amsel, A. Transfer of persistence from fixed-ratio barpress training to runway extinction. Anim. Learn. Behav. 4, 53–57 (1976).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Eisenberger, R., Carlson, J. & Frank, M. Transfer of persistence to the acquisition of a new behaviour. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 31, 691–700 (1979).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Eisenberger, R., Carlson, J., Guile, M. & Shapiro, N. Transfer of effort across behaviors. Learn. Motiv. 10, 178–197 (1979).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Eisenberger, R., Mitchell, M. & Masterson, F. A. Effort training increases generalized self-control. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 49, 1294–1301 (1985).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Göllner, R. et al. Is doing your homework associated with becoming more conscientious. J. Res. Personal. 71, 1–12 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Claro, S., Paunesku, D. & Dweck, C. S. Growth mindset tempers the effects of poverty on academic achievement. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 8664–8668 (2016).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Tough, P. How Children Succeed: Grit, Curiosity, and the Hidden Power of Character (Houghton Mifflin Harcourt, 2012).

  70. Lin, H., Werner, K. M. & Inzlicht, M. Promises and perils of experimentation: the mutual-internal-validity problem. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 854–863 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Athey, S., Tibshirani, J. & Wager, S. Generalized random forests. Ann. Stat. 47, 1148–1178 (2019).

  72. Athey, S. & Wager, S. Estimating treatment effects with causal forests: an application. Obs. Stud. 5, 37–51 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Wager, S. & Athey, S. Estimation and inference of heterogeneous treatment effects using random forests. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 113, 1228–1242 (2018).

    Article  MathSciNet  CAS  Google Scholar 

  74. Basu, S., Kumbier, K., Brown, J. B. & Yu, B. Iterative random forests to discover predictive and stable high-order interactions. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115, 1943–1948 (2018).

    Article  ADS  MathSciNet  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Cacioppo, J. T. & Petty, R. E. The need for cognition. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 42, 116 (1982).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Simons, J. S. & Gaher, R. M. The Distress Tolerance Scale: development and validation of a self-report measure. Motiv. Emot. 29, 83–102 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. John, O. P. & Srivastava, S. in Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research (eds Pervin, L. A. & John, O. P.) pp 102–138 (Guildford, 1999).

  78. Duckworth, A. L. & Quinn, P. D. Development and validation of the Short Grit Scale (GRIT–S). J. Personal. Assess. 91, 166–174 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. De Castella, K. & Byrne, D. My intelligence may be more malleable than yours: the revised implicit theories of intelligence (self-theory) scale is a better predictor of achievement, motivation, and student disengagement. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 30, 245–267 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Campbell, A. V., Chung, J. M. H. & Inzlicht, M. Meaningfulness of effort: deriving purpose from really trying. Preprint at PsyArxiv https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/sg3aw (2022).

  81. Clay, G., Mlynski, C., Korb, F. M., Goschke, T. & Job, V. Rewarding cognitive effort increases the intrinsic value of mental labor. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 119, e2111785119 (2022).

  82. Dora, J., van Hooff, M. L. M., Geurts, S. A. E., Kompier, M. A. J. & Bijleveld, E. The effect of opportunity costs on mental fatigue in labor/leisure trade-offs. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 151, 695–710 (2022).

  83. Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W. & Myers, J. An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behav. Brain Sci. 36, 661–679 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Ritz, H. & Shenhav, A. Humans reconfigure target and distractor processing to address distinct task demands. Psychol. Rev. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000442 (2023).

  85. Ritz, H., Leng, X. & Shenhav, A. Cognitive control as a multivariate optimization problem. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 34, 569–591 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  86. Shenhav, A., Prater Fahey, M. & Grahek, I. Decomposing the motivation to exert mental effort. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 30, 307–314 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. Westbrook, A. et al. Dopamine promotes cognitive effort by biasing the benefits versus costs of cognitive work. Science 367, 1362–1366 (2020).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Westbrook, A., Frank, M. J. & Cools, R. A mosaic of cost-benefit control over cortico-striatal circuitry. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 710–721 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  89. Westbrook, A. et al. Economic choice and heart rate fractal scaling indicate that cognitive effort is reduced by depression and boosted by sad mood. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 8, 687–694 (2022).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Lin, H., Saunders, B., Friese, M., Evans, N. J. & Inzlicht, M. Strong effort manipulations reduce response caution: a preregistered reinvention of the ego-depletion paradigm. Psychol. Sci. 31, 531–547 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Wu, R., Ferguson, A. M. & Inzlicht, M. Do humans prefer cognitive effort over doing nothing? J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 152, 1069–1079 (2023).

  92. Miyake, A. & Friedman, N. P. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 8–14 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  93. Judd, N. & Klingberg, T. Training spatial cognition enhances mathematical learning in a randomized study of 17,000 children. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5, 1548–1554 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Scherer, R., Siddiq, F. & Sánchez Viveros, B. The cognitive benefits of learning computer programming: a meta-analysis of transfer effects. J. Educ. Psychol. 111, 764–792 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  95. Frömer, R., Lin, H., Wolf, C. K. D., Inzlicht, M. & Shenhav, A. Expectations of reward and efficacy guide cognitive control allocation. Nat. Commun. 121030 (2021).

    Article  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  96. Lin, H., Ristic, J., Inzlicht, M. & Otto, A. R. The average reward rate modulates behavioral and neural indices of effortful control allocation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 34, 2113–2126 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Inzlicht, M. & Campbell, A. V. Effort feels meaningful. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 26, 1035–1037 (2022).

    Google Scholar 

  98. Dweck, C. S. & Yeager, D. S. Mindsets: a view from two eras. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 14, 481–496 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  99. Danielmeier, C., Eichele, T., Forstmann, B. U., Tittgemeyer, M. & Ullsperger, M. Posterior medial frontal cortex activity predicts post-error adaptations in task-related visual and motor areas. J. Neurosci. 31, 1780–1789 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  100. Ritz, H. & Shenhav, A. Parametric control of distractor-oriented attention. In Proc. 41st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (eds Dale, R. & Bender, A.) 967–973 (Wiley-Blackwell, 2019).

  101. Kahneman, D., Tursky, B., Shapiro, D. & Crider, A. Pupillary, heart rate, and skin resistance changes during a mental task. J. Exp. Psychol. 79, 164–167 (1969).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Leys, C., Ley, C., Klein, O., Bernard, P. & Licata, L. Detecting outliers: do not use standard deviation around the mean, use absolute deviation around the median. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 764–766 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  103. Schönbrodt, F. D. & Wagenmakers, E. J. Bayes factor design analysis: planning for compelling evidence. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 128–142 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Morey, R. D. & Rouder, J. N. BayesFactor: computation of Bayes factors for common designs. R package version 0.9.12-4.2 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2018).

  105. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2019).

  106. Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., Morey, R. D. & Iverson, G. Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 16, 225–237 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Pearl, J. Lord’s paradox revisited – (Oh Lord! Kumbaya!). J. Causal Inference 4, 1–13 (2016).

  108. Bürkner, P. C. brms: an R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. J. Stat. Softw. 80, 1–28 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge H. Ritz for providing feedback on the task design and N. Lin for designing the task stimuli. This work was supported by a grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (RGPIN-2019-05280) awarded to M.I. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

H.L., A.W. and M.I. contributed to the conception and design of the work. A.W. and M.I. provided critical oversight of and feedback on the work. H.L. and F.F. programmed the experiment and collected the data. H.L. wrote the manuscript. A.W. and M.I. provided critical feedback.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hause Lin.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Human Behaviour thanks Masud Husain, and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Figs. 1–6 and Table 1.

Reporting Summary

Peer Review File

Supplementary Data 1

Supplementary Fig 1. Source data (Table 1): pilot results. Supplementary Fig. 2. Source data (Table 2): Bayesian posterior estimates for models predicting effort preference and controlling for pre-training effort preference and pre-training task accuracy. Supplementary Fig. 3. Source data (Table 3): exploratory analyses and Bayesian posterior densities for the effect of condition on task reaction time. Supplementary Fig. 4. Source data (Table 4): task accuracy for reaction time quartiles. Supplementary Fig. 5. Source data (Table 5): exploratory analyses and Bayesian posterior densities for the interaction effect between condition and pre-training effort preference. Supplementary Fig. 6. Source data (Table 6): casual forests examining heterogeneous treatment effects.

Source data

Source Data Fig. 1

Data underlying value functions.

Source Data Fig. 2

Bayesian posterior samples.

Source Data Fig. 3

Effort preference difference scores.

Source Data Fig. 4

Bayesian posterior samples.

Source Data Fig. 5

Data for computing correlations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lin, H., Westbrook, A., Fan, F. et al. An experimental manipulation of the value of effort. Nat Hum Behav (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-024-01842-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-024-01842-7

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing