Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

A quantitative model of ensemble perception as summed activation in feature space

Subjects

Abstract

Ensemble perception is a process by which we summarize complex scenes. Despite the importance of ensemble perception to everyday cognition, there are few computational models that provide a formal account of this process. Here we develop and test a model in which ensemble representations reflect the global sum of activation signals across all individual items. We leverage this set of minimal assumptions to formally connect a model of memory for individual items to ensembles. We compare our ensemble model against a set of alternative models in five experiments. Our approach uses performance on a visual memory task for individual items to generate zero-free-parameter predictions of interindividual and intraindividual differences in performance on an ensemble continuous-report task. Our top-down modelling approach formally unifies models of memory for individual items and ensembles and opens a venue for building and comparing models of distinct memory processes and representations.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Laboratory ensemble tasks.
Fig. 2: TCC framework for memory of individual items and ensembles.
Fig. 3: TCC ensemble models.
Fig. 4: Non-TCC ensemble models.
Fig. 5: Colour, shape and sequential memory tasks.
Fig. 6: Comparison in predictive accuracy between the Perceptual Summation model and competing models of ensemble memory for colour with set size manipulation.
Fig. 7: The Perceptual Summation model predicts ensemble memory for colour with set size manipulation.
Fig. 8: Comparison in predictive accuracy between the Perceptual Summation model and competing models of ensemble memory for colour with colour range manipulation.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The data are publicly available at the following OSF link: https://osf.io/mt29p/.

Code availability

The code is publicly available on OSF (https://osf.io/mt29p/).

References

  1. Baddeley, A. Working memory. Science 255, 556–559 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Miller, G. A. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol. Rev. 63, 81–97 (1956).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Pashler, H. Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 10, 358–377 (1984).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Simon, H. A. Invariants of human behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 41, 1–20 (1990).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Kahneman, D. A psychological perspective on economics. Am. Econ. Rev. 93, 162–168 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Ariely, D. Seeing sets: representation by statistical properties. Psychol. Sci. 12, 157–162 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Brady, T. F. & Oliva, A. Statistical learning using real-world scenes. Psychol. Sci. 19, 678–685 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Goldstein, M. H. et al. General cognitive principles for learning structure in time and space. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 249–258 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Whitney, D. & Yamanashi Leib, A. Ensemble perception. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 69, 105–129 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Alvarez, G. A. & Oliva, A. The representation of simple ensemble visual features outside the focus of attention. Psychol. Sci. 19, 392–398 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Brady, T. F., Shafer-Skelton, A. & Alvarez, G. A. Global ensemble texture representations are critical to rapid scene perception. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 43, 1160–1176 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Utochkin, I. Ensemble summary statistics as a basis for visual categorization. J. Vis. 15, 8 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Balas, B., Nakano, L. & Rosenholtz, R. A summary-statistic representation in peripheral vision explains visual crowding. J. Vis. 9, 13 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Block, N. Perceptual consciousness overflows cognitive access. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 567–575 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Cohen, M. A., Dennett, D. C. & Kanwisher, N. What is the bandwidth of perceptual experience? Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 324–335 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Grahek, I., Schaller, M. & Tackett, J. L. Anatomy of a psychological theory: integrating construct-validation and computational-modeling methods to advance theorizing. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 803–815 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Guest, O. & Martin, A. E. How computational modeling can force theory building in psychological science. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 789–802 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Navarro, D. J. If mathematical psychology did not exist we might need to invent it: a comment on theory building in psychology. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 707–716 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Oberauer, K. & Lewandowsky, S. Addressing the theory crisis in psychology. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 26, 1596–1618 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Busemeyer, J. R. & Wang, Y. M. Model comparisons and model selections based on generalization criterion methodology. J. Math. Psychol. 44, 171–189 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Lee, M. D. How cognitive modeling can benefit from hierarchical Bayesian models. J. Math. Psychol. 55, 1–7 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Yarkoni, T. The generalizability crisis. Behav. Brain Sci. 45, e1 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Rust, N. C. in The Cognitive Neurosciences 5th edn (eds Gazzaniga, M. S. & Mangun, G. R.) 337–348 (MIT Press, 2014).

  24. Alvarez, G. A. Representing multiple objects as an ensemble enhances visual cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 122–131 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Ward, E. J., Bear, A. & Scholl, B. J. Can you perceive ensembles without perceiving individuals? The role of statistical perception in determining whether awareness overflows access. Cognition 152, 78–86 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Oriet, C., Giesinger, C. & Stewart, K. M. Can change detection succeed when change localization fails? J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 46, 1127–1147 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Haberman, J. & Whitney, D. Efficient summary statistical representation when change localization fails. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 855–859 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Marchant, A. P., Simons, D. J. & de Fockert, J. W. Ensemble representations: effects of set size and item heterogeneity on average size perception. Acta Psychol. 142, 245–250 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Šetić, M., Švegar, D. & Domijan, D. Modelling the statistical processing of visual information. Neurocomputing 70, 1808–1812 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Baek, J. & Chong, S. C. Ensemble perception and focused attention: two different modes of visual processing to cope with limited capacity. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 27, 602–606 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Solomon, J. A. Five dichotomies in the psychophysics of ensemble perception. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 83, 904–910 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Chetverikov, A., Campana, G. & Kristjánsson, R. Building ensemble representations: how the shape of preceding distractor distributions affects visual search. Cognition 153, 196–210 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Hansmann-Roth, S., Thorsteinsdóttir, S., Geng, J. & Kristjánsson, R. Temporal integration of feature probability distributions in visual working memory. J. Vis. 21, 1969 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. van Rooij, I. & Baggio, G. Theory before the test: how to build high-verisimilitude explanatory theories in psychological science. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 682–697 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Schurgin, M. W., Wixted, J. T. & Brady, T. F. Psychophysical scaling reveals a unified theory of visual memory strength. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 1156–1172 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Thurstone, L. L. A law of comparative judgment. Psychol. Rev. 34, 273–286 (1927).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Swets, J. A. Form of empirical ROCs in discrimination and diagnostic tasks: implications for theory and measurement of performance. Psychol. Bull. 99, 181–198 (1986).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Luce, R. D. & Galanter, E. Psychophysical scaling. Handb. Math. Psychol. 1, 245–307 (1963).

    Google Scholar 

  39. Shepard, R. N. Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science 237, 1317–1323 (1987).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Stevens, S. S. A scale for the measurement of a psychological magnitude: loudness. Psychol. Rev. 43, 405–416 (1936).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Wickens, T. D. Elementary Signal Detection Theory (Oxford Univ. Press, 2001).

  42. Wixted, J. T. The forgotten history of signal detection theory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 46, 201–233 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Brady, T. F., Schacter, D. L. & Alvarez, G. The adaptive nature of false memories is revealed by gist-based distortion of true memories. Preprint at PsyArXiv https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/zeg95 (2018).

  44. Chater, N., Tenenbaum, J. B. & Yuille, A. Probabilistic models of cognition: where next. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 292–293 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Hemmer, P. & Steyvers, M. A Bayesian account of reconstructive memory. Top. Cogn. Sci. 1, 189–202 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. McCarley, J. S. & Benjamin, A. S. in The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Engineering (eds Lee, J. D. & Kirlik, A.) 465–475 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2013).

  47. Hintzman, D. L. ‘Schema abstraction’ in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychol. Rev. 93, 411–428 (1986).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Howard, M. W. & Kahana, M. J. A distributed representation of temporal context. J. Math. Psychol. 46, 269–299 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Murdock, B. B. A theory for the storage and retrieval of item and associative information. Psychol. Rev. 89, 609–626 (1982).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Reder, L. M. et al. A mechanistic account of the mirror effect for word frequency: a computational model of remember–know judgments in a continuous recognition paradigm. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 26, 294–320 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Shiffrin, R. M. & Steyvers, M. A model for recognition memory: REM—retrieving effectively from memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 4, 145–166 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Kriegeskorte, N. & Wei, X. X. Neural tuning and representational geometry. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 22, 703–718 (2021).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Xiong, H. D. & Wei, X. X. Optimal encoding of prior information in noisy working memory systems. In Conference on Computational Cognitive Neuroscience (CCN, 2022).

  54. Nosofsky, R. M. Attention and learning processes in the identification and categorization of integral stimuli. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 13, 87–108 (1987).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Tenenbaum, J. B. Bayesian modeling of human concept learning. Adv. Neural Inf. Process. Syst. 11, 59–68 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  56. Shamir, M. Emerging principles of population coding: in search for the neural code. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 25, 140–148 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Averbeck, B. B., Latham, P. E. & Pouget, A. Neural correlations, population coding and computation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 358–366 (2006).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Bartolo, R., Saunders, R. C., Mitz, A. R. & Averbeck, B. B. Information-limiting correlations in large neural populations. J. Neurosci. 40, 1668–1678 (2020).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Kohn, A., Coen-Cagli, R., Kanitscheider, I. & Pouget, A. Correlations and neuronal population information. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 39, 237–256 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Williams, J. R., Robinson, M. M., Schurgin, M. W., Wixted, J. T. & Brady, T. F. You can’t ‘count’ how many items people remember in working memory: the importance of signal detection-based measures for understanding change detection performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 48, 1390–1409 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Robinson, M. M., Benjamin, A. S. & Irwin, D. E. Is there a K in capacity? Assessing the structure of visual short-term memory. Cogn. Psychol. 121, 101305 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Tong, K., Dubé, C. & Sekuler, R. What makes a prototype a prototype? Averaging visual features in a sequence. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 81, 1962–1978 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. VanderWeele, T. J. & Mathur, M. B. Some desirable properties of the Bonferroni correction: is the Bonferroni correction really so bad? Am. J. Epidemiol. 188, 617–618 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Rahnev, D., Block, N., Denison, R. N. & Jehee, J. Is perception probabilistic? Clarifying the definitions. Preprint at PsyArXiv https://psyarxiv.com/f8v5r/ (2021).

  65. Eckstein, M. P. Probabilistic computations for attention, eye movements, and search. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 3, 319–342 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Ma, W. J. Organizing probabilistic models of perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 511–518 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Zeng, T., Tompary, A., Schapiro, A. C. & Thompson-Schill, S. L. Tracking the relation between gist and item memory over the course of long-term memory consolidation. eLife https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.65588 (2021).

  68. Rosenbaum, D. & Bowman, H. Extraction of gist without encoding of individual items in RSVP of numerical sequences. Preprint at OSF https://osf.io/n2rcj (2021).

  69. Hommel, B. et al. No one knows what attention is. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 81, 2288–2303 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Greene, N. R. & Naveh-Benjamin, M. The effects of divided attention at encoding on specific and gist-based associative episodic memory. Mem. Cogn. 50, 59–76 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Chen, Z., Zhuang, R., Wang, X., Ren, Y. & Abrams, R. A. Ensemble perception without attention depends upon attentional control settings. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 83, 1240–1250 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Zepp, J., Dubé, C. & Melcher, D. A direct comparison of central tendency recall and temporal integration in the successive field iconic memory task. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 83, 1337–1356 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Gershman, S. J. in The Oxford Handbook of Human Memory (eds Kahana, M. & Wagner, A.) (Oxford Univ. Press, 2021).

  74. Li, A. Y., Liang, J. C., Lee, A. C. & Barense, M. D. The validated circular shape space: quantifying the visual similarity of shape. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 149, 949–966 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Zhang, W. & Luck, S. J. Discrete fixed-resolution representations in visual working memory. Nature 453, 233–235 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Smith, J. D. & Minda, J. P. Prototypes in the mist: the early epochs of category learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 24, 1411–1436 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Nadarajah, S., Afuecheta, E. & Chan, S. On the distribution of maximum of multivariate normal random vectors. Commun. Stat. Theory Methods 48, 2425–2445 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge funding from the National Institutes of Health (National Research Service Award Fellowship No. 1F32MH127823-01 to M.M.R.) and the National Science Foundation (grant nos. BCS-1653457 and BCS-2146988 to T.F.B.). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

M.M.R. and T.F.B. conceived and designed the experiments, developed the material and analytic tools and models, and wrote the paper. M.M.R. implemented the main experiments and modelling analyses.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Maria M. Robinson or Timothy F. Brady.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Human Behaviour thanks Bernhard Spitzer, Eddie Ester and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data

Extended Data Fig. 1 The Perceptual Summation model predicts ensemble memory for color with a range manipulation.

Graphical representation of TCC models’ fit and prediction of data in Experiment 2. In this experiment participants had to remember colors of simultaneously presented circles, and the range of colors was manipulated in the ensemble task. The top row of panel A shows the fits of the TCC model for individual items to aggregate data from the visual working memory task for six items. The bottom row of panel A shows results from the predictive analysis in which d’ estimates from the visual working memory task were substituted into the TCC Perceptual Summation (blue), Post-perceptual (red) and Automatic Averaging (green) models to predict the ensemble data. The bottom panel (B) shows model predictions for a few example participants. Schurgin et al.35).

Extended Data Fig. 2 Comparison in predictive accuracy between Perceptual Summation model and competing models of ensemble memory for shape with the set size manipulation.

The top panel shows violin plots for the difference in predicted negative log likelihood scores between each of the six alternative competing models (PNLLAlt) and the main Perceptual Summation model (PNLLPerSum) for Experiment 3 (n = 50 participants). Lower values of PNLL indicate higher predictive accuracy, therefore, PNLL difference scores higher (or lower) than zero indicate support for the Perceptual Summation (or a competing) model. In both experiments, the vast majority of participants are better predicted by the Perceptual Summation model than any of the alternatives. The bottom panel shows a table with a summary of descriptive and inferential statistics from all comparisons in Experiment 3, including the mean and standard error of the mean across participants. PNLL values were compared with a paired two-tailed t-test, corrected for multiple comparisons and all p-values were statistically significant (p < 0.001).

Extended Data Fig. 3 The Perceptual Summation model predicts ensemble memory for shape with a set size manipulation.

Graphical representation of the TCC models’ fit and prediction of data in Experiment 3. In this experiment participants had to remember different shapes, and the number of shapes was manipulated in the working memory and ensemble task. The top row of panel A shows the fits of the TCC model for individual items to aggregate data from the visual working memory task for six items and the second row of panel A shows results from the predictive analysis in which d’ estimates from the visual working memory task were substituted into the TCC Perceptual Summation (blue), Post-perceptual (red) and Automatic Averaging (green) models to predict the ensemble data. Panel B shows data and model predictions for a few example participants.

Extended Data Fig. 4 Comparison in predictive accuracy between Perceptual Summation model and competing models of ensemble memory for shape with the range manipulation.

The top panel shows violin plots with the difference in predicted negative log likelihood scores between each of the six alternative competing models (PNLLAlt) and the main Perceptual Summation model (PNLLPerSum) for Experiment 4 (n = 50 participants). Lower values of PNLL indicate higher predictive accuracy, therefore, PNLL difference scores higher (or lower) than zero indicate support for the Perceptual Summation (or a competing) model. In both experiments, the vast majority of participants are better predicted by the Perceptual Summation model than any of the alternatives. The bottom panel shows a table with a summary of descriptive and inferential statistics from all comparisons in Experiment 4, including the mean and standard error of the mean across participants. PNLL values were compared with a paired two-tailed t-test, corrected for multiple comparisons and all p-values were statistically significant (p < 0.001).

Extended Data Fig. 5 The Perceptual Summation model predicts ensemble memory for shape with a range manipulation.

Graphical representation of TCC model’s fit and prediction of data in Experiment 4. In this experiment participants had to remember simultaneously presented shapes, and the range of shapes was manipulated in the ensemble task. The top row of panel A shows the fits of the TCC model for individual items to aggregate data from the visual working memory task for six items, and the second row of panel A shows results from the predictive analysis in which d’ estimates from the visual working memory task were substituted into the TCC Perceptual Summation (blue), Post-perceptual (red) and Automatic Averaging (green) models to predict the ensemble data. Panel B shows data and model predictions for a few example participants.

Extended Data Fig. 6 Comparison in predictive accuracy between Sequential Perceptual Summation model and competing models of ensemble memory for sequentially presented stimuli.

The top panel shows violin plots of the difference in predicted negative log likelihood scores between each of the eight alternative competing models (PNLLAlt) and the main Sequential Perceptual Summation model (PNLLPerSum) (n = 50 participants). Lower values of PNLL indicate higher predictive accuracy, therefore, PNLL difference scores higher (or lower) than zero indicate support for the Sequential Perceptual Summation (or a competing) model. The vast majority of participants are better predicted by the Sequential Perceptual Summation model than any of the alternatives. Note that the baseline here is the Sequential Perceptual Summation model that relies on fitting a decay rate. The independent d’ Perceptual Summation model, the last model above, is the same model but without this parametric assumption about how d’ changes across the items in the working memory task. This independent model is instead one in which we used separate d’ estimates to quantify familiarity of items as a function of serial position, rather than a single d’ and rate parameter. This model is marked with an * because it is also a version of the Sequential Perceptual Summation model and so shows comparable predictive accuracy to the main Sequential Perceptual Summation model we use, as expected. The bottom panel shows a table with a summary of descriptive and inferential statistics from all comparisons in Experiment 5, including the mean and standard error of the mean across participants. PNLL values were compared with a paired two-tailed t-test, corrected for multiple comparisons and for all comparisons between competing models p-values were statistically significant (p < 0.001).

Extended Data Fig. 7 The Perceptual Summation model predicts ensemble memory for sequentially presented stimuli.

Summary of results from Experiment 5, in which participants had to remember colors of sequentially presented real-world objects. The top row of panel A shows the fits of the Sequential TCC model to individual data and the second row of panel A shows the TCC Sequential Perceptual Summation (blue), Post-perceptual (red) and Automatic Averaging (green) models’ predictions of the ensemble data in two conditions. In the clockwise (counterclockwise) condition the most recently shown items were from the clockwise (counterclockwise) direction from the mean color, producing a clockwise (counterclockwise) bias. Panel B shows data and model predictions for a few example participants.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Figs. 1–3, Table 1, Discussion and references.

Reporting Summary

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Robinson, M.M., Brady, T.F. A quantitative model of ensemble perception as summed activation in feature space. Nat Hum Behav 7, 1638–1651 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-023-01602-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-023-01602-z

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing