Introduction

Ulotaront is a trace amine-associated receptor 1 (TAAR1) agonist with serotonin 5-HT1A agonist activity, but without affinity for D2 or serotonin 5-HT2A receptors, the targets of current antipsychotics1. TAARs are a family of G-protein-coupled receptors; the TAAR1 subtype, expressed in multiple brain regions including key dopaminergic nuclei (ventral tegmental area [VTA]) and serotonergic (dorsal raphe nuclei [DRN]), modulates monoaminergic and glutamatergic neurotransmission2. In mice, ulotaront exerts inhibitory effects on VTA neuronal firing1 and attenuates the ketamine-induced increase in striatal dopamine synthesis capacity3. In addition, ulotaront prominently suppresses rapid eye movement (REM) sleep in rodents and humans, similar to many antidepressants4. Ulotaront has demonstrated broad efficacy in preclinical models of psychosis1 and is currently in phase 3 clinical trials evaluating its safety and efficacy in the treatment of schizophrenia, with positive results available from both an initial acute treatment study5 and a 6-month continuation study6. Here we review the phase 1 studies that led to phase 2 clinical development.

Schizotypy is a dimensional personality trait that shares genetic, neuroanatomical, neurobiological, and cognitive characteristics with schizophrenia and schizophrenia spectrum disorders7,8,9. Subjects with personality trait scores demonstrating high schizotypy (HS) versus low schizotypy (LS) were recruited to widen the magnitude of the changes in reward processing and executive function analogous to those seen in schizophrenia, but in people who are entirely healthy and without the confounds of drug treatment and psychotic symptoms.

In light of the preclinical results, we undertook phase I translational studies, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in healthy volunteers with high or low schizotypy, that were designed to evaluate the effects of ulotaront compared to the D2 receptor antagonist amisulpride10 on neurocognitive processes implicated in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia, including brain circuits of reward, working memory and emotional processing, resting state connectivity (RSC), and cerebral blood flow (CBF).

The primary outcome was blood oxygen level‒dependent (BOLD) responses in ventral striatum (VS) during the anticipation phase of the monetary incentive delay (MID) task. This task has been widely used in fMRI studies to investigate reward circuitry in response to anticipation of wins or losses and their occurrence11,12,13. Cues that predict monetary wins elicit increased BOLD responses in the VS that are mediated by increased dopamine release and thus attenuated by postsynaptic dopamine receptor-blocking antipsychotics. It is thought that high-tonic dopamine release obscures contingent release and results in random learning of incentive salience and thus the positive symptoms of schizophrenia14. Increased VS activity appears to be a shared neuronal correlate of positive symptoms in schizotypal people and unmedicated first episode patients15. We hypothesized that ulotaront would attenuate VS BOLD responses in the anticipatory phase of the MID task in view of its preclinical actions in decreasing dopamine neuronal firing and synthesis capacity1,3.

Secondary outcomes included ulotaront and amisulpride effects on fMRI responses during N-Back working memory task and neurocognitive processes of identification and recall of emotionally-valenced faces or words in the emotional test battery (ETB). The N-Back signal detection task has been used to assess impairments of working memory, associated with poor social and occupational functioning in schizophrenia16. We previously showed that amisulpride (400 mg) reversed impaired performance in high schizotypes performing this task17 and hypothesized ulotaront would have the same effect. We used the ETB to assess possible antidepressant efficacy given the antidepressant-like preclinical findings. Using the ETB, acute or 7-day administration of antidepressants, SSRIs or SNRIs, was shown to increase the recall of positive self-referent words and the perception of ambiguous faces as happy18,19,20,21 in healthy volunteers and patients with major depressive disorders.

Finally, we conducted exploratory analyses of RSC and arterial spin labeling. There is evidence of abnormal RSC22,23 within the default mode network (DMN, self-referential processing), executive control network (ECN, goal-directed activity) and anterior salience network (ASN, detecting and orienting towards salient stimuli)24,25,26 in schizophrenia. In addition, arterial spin labeling (ASL) provides absolute measures of CBF, reflecting regional neuronal activity. A recent study found a specific association between increased CBF in striatum and the negative symptom, apathy27. Another study found alterations in striatal and prefrontal CBF may precede onset of psychosis28.

Materials and methods

Participants

The study was designed and conducted in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki (1964) and registered with ClinicalTrials.gov, identifier: NCT01972711. This was a double-blind, placebo-controlled, randomized study performed at the Universities of Manchester and Oxford, UK between March 27, 2014 and July 7, 2015. The study was approved by the National Research Service (Northwest). Three amendments to the protocol were approved to clarify the population to be studied, amend timing of the interim analysis, and permit greater flexibility in scheduling. All participants gave informed consent. Baseline and final visit health checks included physical and neurological examinations; electrocardiography; renal, liver and thyroid function blood tests; and suicide risk assessment. Adverse drug effects were monitored. One hundred thirty-three individuals were screened online using the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ)29 to recruit those with low (<10) and high (≥40) schizotypy scores who were aged 18–45 years. Current psychiatric disorder was excluded with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV30. Mental state assessments included: Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale31, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS)32, Launay-Slade Hallucination Scale33 and Columbia-Suicide Severity Rating Scale (C-SSRS)34. Intelligence quotient was assessed with the National Reading Test35. One hundred five participants were randomized, 2 did not complete the study, and 96 had acceptable fMRI/cognitive task data and were included in the analysis. Participants with LS (n = 51) and HS (n = 45) received placebo (n = 32), ulotaront (n = 30; single dose, 50 mg) or amisulpride (n = 34; single dose, 400 mg) (dosages chosen as the highest that did not cause side effects) (Fig. 1A). Randomization was stratified by site and schizotypy according to a 1:1:1 code accessible only to pharmacy staff, producing groups with similar demographics (Fig. 1B). Participants underwent fMRI scans 2 h posttreatment. Full clinical assessments were carried out each visit and patients were asked about side effects.

Fig. 1: Study design and task procedures.
figure 1

A Experimental workflow diagram showing screening for schizotypy and allocation to treatment groups for selected volunteers. B Demographic data, including treatment group, age, gender, and level of schizotypy in the MID task. C Design of the MID task. Participants were presented with a cue predicting either monetary gain, loss, or no change, and asked to press a button in response to a following target as fast as possible. There were three types of trial: possible reward, possible punishment and neutral, depending on whether participants responded on time to the target. D ROI used in the MID task. Masks were created in bilateral striatum, bilateral insula and mOFC. All masks were 10-mm radius spheres based on previously reported coordinates from the meta-analysis of Liu et al.41. Specific coordinates of ROI used for the anticipatory phase were 38 20 –8, and -32 18 –6 for right and left insula, 12 10 –4 and –12 10 –6 for right and left striatum, and 2 50 –16 for mOFC. For the outcome phase, coordinates were 36 22 –8 and –28 24 –8 for right and left insula, 12 10 –6 and 10 8 –4 for right and left striatum and –2 56 –6 and 2 48 –14 for mOFC. ASL arterial spin labeling, ETB emotional test battery, MID monetary incentive delay, mOFC medial orbital frontal cortex, RSC resting state connectivity, SPQ Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire, VS ventral striatum.

Monetary incentive delay task

Each trial consisted of a cue indicating trial type (win, loss, or neutral) followed by a target cue requiring a rapid button press, which triggered win, loss, or neutral outcomes (Fig. 1C)36. There were three trial types: possible reward, possible punishment, and neutral, spread across 120 trials and with a winning rate of 63%. In reward trials, fast response to the target resulted in monetary gain (win) and slow response had no consequence. In loss trials, fast response had no consequence, while slow response led to loss of money. In neutral trials, responses had no consequence. Winning and losing cues also indicated whether participants could win/lose either £1 or £5. The amount won or lost was displayed. fMRI responses were recorded in the: (1) anticipatory phase, from the initial cue to the response to the target; and (2) outcome phase, from the win/loss display.

N-Back

Subjects viewed a series of letters and were asked to indicate if the letter presented was an “x” (0-Back) or if it matched letters shown in one (1-Back), two (2-Back) and three (3-Back) previous trials, with increasing difficulty level17 (see Supplementary material for full details).

Emotional test battery

ETB tasks presented stimuli of differing emotional valence (positive/negative words or face stimuli) and tested ability to remember words and identify face emotions. Participants completed standard emotional word-based memory recall (EREC) and face-based emotional categorization (ECAT) tasks37,38. Instructions were presented on screen for participants to read before task completion. The psychometric properties of these tasks have been assessed previously39, with intraclass correlation coefficients of 0.4–0.8. Previous studies showed that antidepressant treatment led to improved identification and recall for positive relative to negative stimuli40.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)

Scanner details and sequences, acquisition of field maps, data production and anatomical reference image acquisition, time-correction, realignment, normalization, and smoothing were carried out as described in Supplementary material. For primary region of interest (ROI) analyses, ROIs were predefined using 10-mm radius spheres centered on coordinates from a meta-analysis of MID studies41 (Fig. 1C) and for dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) in the N-Back task16. Anatomical masks were used to pre-define other ROIs in the N-Back task. The methodologies for RSC and ASL are described in Supplementary material.

Statistics

A formal sample size calculation was not attempted for this 2-center fMRI MID paradigm. Based on a statistical investigation of our previous fMRI studies42 and the MID literature14 a target of 36 subjects per treatment arm was selected, requiring a sample of 108 to complete. The primary endpoints were BOLD fMRI responses in key ROIs while performing the MID, N-Back, and SD tasks after a single-dose of study medication. The ROIs are specified in the text and in Supplementary methods. The secondary endpoints were the performance measures in each task.

In the MID task, reaction time was measured in milliseconds and analyzed with repeated measures ANOVA with trial type (win, loss, neutral) as a within-subject factor, schizotypy and treatment as between-subject factors, and site and gender as covariates. After checks for equivalence of group standard deviations, analysis of variance (ANOVA) of BOLD signal task responses in ROIs (Fig. 1D) was carried out with treatment and schizotypy as fixed effects, without factors or covariates for site, task, phase or gender. Primary comparisons were placebo versus ulotaront, followed by placebo versus amisulpride and ulotaront versus amisulpride. The same design was used to analyze N-Back ROI BOLD responses. There was no correction for repeated group comparisons in several ROIs. However, in small volume family-wise error (FWE) correction, ROIs were combined into a single set of voxels to threshold statistical significance for activated voxels. Whole-brain voxels were used to threshold significant activations outside predicted ROIs.

For the N-Back task, difficulty (0, 1, 2 or 3-Back) was a within-subject factor in repeated measures ANOVAs of accuracy and reaction time. Two primary effects tested were treatment and treatment × task difficulty. Equivalent effects were tested in analyses of schizotypy and schizotypy × treatment interactions. Significant effects were followed-up by post hoc tests. BOLD signal responses in predefined ROIs contrasted all levels of task difficulty with the 0-back condition using the same ANOVA design as for the MID task.

ETB data were tested using repeated measures ANOVA (within-subject factor = positive/negative valence of stimuli and between-subject factor coded for treatment). For each task, the primary contrasts were interaction between treatment group and within-subject factors to test for emotion-specific treatment effects on performance. The primary prediction was that ulotaront would induce positive changes in ETB endpoints relative to placebo. ANOVA was also performed to determine the effects of schizotypy across treatments. Significant effects were followed-up by post hoc tests.

Results

MID task

ANOVA of MID task behavioral data showed participants were significantly faster on win and loss compared with neutral trials with no effect of schizotypy (Supplementary Fig. 1). Repeated measures ANCOVA of trial type × schizotypy × treatment using site and sex covariates revealed an effect of trial type [F (1.70,149.48) = 5.69; p = 0.006] with participants significantly faster on win (p < 0.001; CI: –16.34, –7.72) and loss (p < 0.001; CI: –15.51, –6.70), compared to neutral trials (Supplementary Fig. 1). There was a main effect of treatment [F (2,88) = 3.98, p = 0.02], with participants in the ulotaront group showing a significant slowing of reaction time compared to placebo (p = 0.049; CI: 0.9, 42.96) and amisulpride (p = 0.048, CI: 0.14, 42.31) groups. Nonsignificant differences were observed between placebo and amisulpride.

The MID task changed BOLD signals bilaterally in prespecified ROIs (phase × valence × ROI interaction; p < 0.001) (Fig. 2A). During the anticipation phase, VS was activated and medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC) deactivated in win and loss trials (Fig. 2A). In the outcome phase, BOLD responses in insula and mOFC were substantially greater than in the anticipation phase, with mOFC responding more to win and insula to loss outcomes (Fig. 2A). There were no main effects of schizotypy in win or loss versus neutral trials.

Fig. 2: Main effects of MID task and treatment on BOLD response.
figure 2

A Main effects of MID task on ROI BOLD signals in anticipation and outcome phases in VS, insula and mOFC. B Effects of treatment with ulotaront, amisulpride or placebo on BOLD signals in VS, insula and mOFC ROIs during anticipation of loss, and on left insula responses in win trials in anticipation and outcome phases. C High schizotypes are sensitive to loss anticipation and this is reversed by ulotaront but responses to loss outcomes are similar in HS and LS and ulotaront enhanced these responses. Whole-brain SVC analysis with FWE p < 0.05 correction. BOLD blood oxygen level‒dependent, FWE family-wise error, HS high schizotypy, LS low schizotypy, MID monetary incentive delay, mOFC medial orbital frontal cortex, ROI region of interest, SVC small volume correction, VS ventral striatum. Error bars are standard errors of the mean.

The pattern of treatment effects on BOLD responses was similar for anticipation of wins and losses (see Supplementary Table 1 for all comparisons). Striatal BOLD responses were smaller in both treatment groups, and significantly so for the ulotaront group in anticipation of loss (striatum left, p = 0.03; right p = 0.06) (Fig. 2B), compared with placebo. The statistical effect of ulotaront on left or right striatal responses to loss was unaffected by including reaction time in the model as a covariate that did not approach significance. Reaction time did not correlate with left or right striatal BOLD responses (r < –0.11; n = 96). Insula responses were not significantly affected by treatment. In mOFC, win and loss anticipation evoked small negative BOLD responses that became positive in the ulotaront group in win trials compared to placebo (left and right mOFC, p < 0.01) (Supplementary Table 1) and in loss trials (i.e., loss-avoidance trials) compared to amisulpride (left, p = 0.029) (Fig. 2B); (right, p = 0.048) (Supplementary Table 1). In the outcome phase, there were no treatment effects in striatum or mOFC. In left insula, ulotaront increased responses to wins (p = 0.004) and losses (p = 0.035), compared to amisulpride (Fig. 2B). In right insula, HS, but not LS subjects, responded to anticipation of loss under placebo and this was prevented by ulotaront treatment; treatment × schizotypy was significant both in ROI (p = 0.021; not shown) and small volume corrected (SVC) (Fig. 2C) analyses (p = 0.021; FWE-corrected, t = 3.54). In contrast to HS responses in anticipation of potential losses, both LS and HS groups responded to loss outcomes in right insula, which was enhanced by ulotaront treatment (ROI, p = 0.043; SVC, p = 0.017) (Fig. 2C).

N-Back

Participants receiving ulotaront were less accurate compared to placebo (p < 0.001) and amisulpride (p = 0.011) (Fig. 3A). Schizotypy did not affect accuracy. N-Back performance activated the task-positive ROIs (DLPFC, ACC and precuneus; Fig. 3B) but this was not affected by treatment. HS had smaller DLPFC BOLD responses to the task than LS participants in ROI and SVC analyses, but these were not modified by treatment (Fig. 3Ci). The expected hippocampal deactivation at all 3 levels of task difficulty was prevented on the right by ulotaront but unaffected by amisulpride (Fig. 3Cii). This was corroborated by SVC analysis (Supplementary Fig. 2). Similarly in the whole-brain analysis, de-activations in postcentral gyrus and right hippocampus were attenuated by ulotaront. Both drugs induced deactivation in left frontal pole, compared to placebo (Fig. 3D). A main effect of schizotypy showed greater activation in frontal pole in HS compared to LS groups. In 2 regions, HS versus LS differences seen under placebo were reversed in ulotaront treated participants. (Supplementary Fig. 3A–C).

Fig. 3: Main effects of N-Back task and treatment on BOLD response.
figure 3

A Performance at different levels of N-Back difficulty. B Whole-brain analysis cluster corrected p < 0.05 showing the main effect of task for all participants. Task-positive clusters are shown in red and task-negative clusters are in blue. B Main effect of treatment in hippocampus and schizotypy in DLPFC, comparing all 3 levels of N-Back with 0-Back. C ROI analysis showing effect of schizotypy in left DLPFC and loss of hippocampal deactivation after ulotaront treatment. D Whole-brain analysis showing effect of ulotaront versus placebo (–12 42 26, voxels 419) and amisulpride versus placebo (–26 38 36, voxels 729) in left frontal pole. E ETB analysis of emotional processing in LS and HS subjects outside the scanner and effects after treatment with placebo, ulotaront or amisulpride. The analysis assessed reaction time in the emotional categorization task and the number of both positive and negative emotional words recalled. BOLD blood oxygen level-dependent, DLPFC dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, ETB emotional test battery, HS high schizotypy, LS low schizotypy, ROI region of interest. Error bars are standard errors of the mean.

Emotional test battery

There was a main effect of treatment on reaction times in the ECAT (p = 0.015) (Fig. 3E). Participants receiving ulotaront (average = 882.02 ms) were slower than those receiving placebo (average = 785.21 ms). Although the effect of amisulpride versus placebo was similar, this was not significant ( > 0.10). There were no other significant treatment effects or interactions with schizotypy. In the EREC, there was an effect of treatment for total number of correct words recalled [F (1,55) = 4.867, p = 0.032]. Participants receiving ulotaront recalled fewer correct words compared to placebo (average 3.78 versus 4.83), regardless of valence (Fig. 3E). Comparing amisulpride and placebo showed an interaction between treatment and valence (p = 0.039; data not shown). Post hoc tests revealed participants receiving amisulpride recalled fewer correct positive words (p = 0.014). No differences were seen for negative words. There was an effect of schizotypy (p = 0.002) with HS recalling fewer words than LS participants, regardless of valence (Supplementary Tables 2 and 3).

Resting state connectivity

Treatment × schizotypy × site ANOVA for pair-wise treatment comparisons of RSC showed a significant schizotypy effect (p = 0.002) with HS having significantly reduced DMN, ASN, and right ECN connectivity compared to LS participants. Both drugs tended to reduce schizotypy effects (Fig. 4). In DMN, there was a significant treatment ×schizotypy interaction, with connectivity in HS subjects under placebo increased by amisulpride to equal LS connectivity (p = 0.017) (Fig. 4A). In contrast, in the ASN, the effect of schizotypy was reversed by ulotaront (treatment × schizotypy, p = 0.003) but not amisulpride, especially in insula (Fig. 4B). A similar pattern was seen in right ECN but there were no statistically significant treatment × schizotypy interactions in whole ECN (Fig. 4C). However, the right inferior frontal cortical component of the ECN showed markedly reduced connectivity in HS participants that was abolished by ulotaront (p = 0.005) and by amisulpride treatment at trend level (p = 0.063) (Supplementary Fig. 4). In left ECN, the effect of schizotypy was not modified by ulotaront or amisulpride.

Fig. 4: Resting state connectivity; treatment by schizotypy interactions.
figure 4

AC HS group have reduced connectivity compared with LS in all networks, A lessened by amisulpride in DMN, B lessened by ulotaront in ASN, and C unaffected by treatment in right ECN. The histograms show group mean intensities (z-scores) of individual resting state networks identified from the independent component analysis. Significance values for treatment by schizotypy shown in A and B are from ANOVA with factors for treatment and schizotypy group. ASN anterior salience network, DMN default mode network, ECN executive control network. Error bars are standard errors of the mean.

Arterial spin labeling

Ulotaront significantly reduced CBF compared to placebo in posterior cingulate (p = 0.003) (Fig. 5A), right thalamus (Supplementary Fig. 5A) and right DLPFC (p = 0.003) (Supplementary Fig. 5B). In DLPFC, greater reductions in CBF occurred in HS compared to LS participants in the ulotaront versus placebo (Fig. 5B) and ulotaront versus amisulpride (Fig. 5C) comparisons. Amisulpride-induced reductions in CBF were significant in right DLPFC and bilateral superior temporal cortex and did not differ in HS and LS participants in any region. Compared with amisulpride, ulotaront produced greater reduction in CBF in HS than the LS group in anterior cingulate (Supplementary Fig. 5C), bilateral DLPFC and insula, in right thalamus and left supramarginal gyrus. Mean CBF under placebo did not differ between HS and LS groups.

Fig. 5: Arterial spin labeling; treatment by schizotypy interactions.
figure 5

Analysis of CBF for A placebo versus ulotaront treatment effect in the posterior cingulate, B Ulotaront/placebo by schizotypy interaction in right DLPFC, and C Ulotaront/amisulpride by schizotypy interaction in right DLPFC/insula cluster. CBF cerebral blood flow, DLPFC dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, LS low schizotypy, HS high schizotypy, pFWEc family-wise error-corrected p-value. Error bars are standard errors of the mean.

Safety

For participants receiving ulotaront, the most commonly reported (≥10%) treatment-emergent adverse events (TEAEs) were somnolence (60.0%), nausea (34.3%), dizziness (28.6%), dry mouth (14.3%), and headache (11.4%) (Supplementary Table 4). There were no deaths or TEAEs leading to discontinuation from the study and no clinically meaningful changes in the safety parameters.

Discussion

In summary, ulotaront partially met the primary endpoint in reducing anticipatory VS responses in the MID and reversed aberrant right insula responses to anticipated loss in HS participants. On the secondary endpoints, reductions in performance accuracy and speed were seen with ulotaront in the ETB and N-Back tasks. In HS participants, ulotaront improved reduced RSC and exerted regionally selective effects on cortical CBF.

In the MID task, the finding that participants were significantly faster on win or loss trials compared with neutral trials suggested that performance was controlled by appropriate motivational drives to win or avoid losing. The task engaged neural activity in prespecified ROIs during anticipation of monetary win or loss, consistent with previous studies43,44,45. The finding that VS responded more to anticipation of possible wins than win outcomes is consistent with the role of dopamine in prediction error learning, in which cues predicting rewards elicit anticipatory dopamine neural activity46. Cues allowing avoidance of aversive outcomes acquire incentive salience in the same way, albeit motivated by threat. This may account for striatal BOLD responses to cues predicting possible loss. We predicted ulotaront would attenuate striatal anticipatory BOLD responses through indirect actions in modulating dopamine neural activity. The effects of ulotaront occurred bilaterally in striatum but were significant only in anticipation of loss in left striatum. In contrast, amisulpride did not attenuate striatal responses, despite its D2 receptor affinity, as recently reported by Grimm et al.47, possibly due to competing presynaptic disinhibition of dopamine release at low doses10. In the MID task, ulotaront treatment achieved the primary outcome in modulating striatal responses to incentive stimuli. Psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia may be moderated by normalizing salience abnormalities in the reward system48. Thus, ulotaront may modulate dopaminergic incentive circuitry relevant to improvement of positive schizophrenia symptoms.

Compared with a quiescent, deactivated state under placebo, mOFC showed bilateral positive BOLD responses induced by ulotaront in win and loss anticipation. This contrasts with reduced anticipatory responses in VS following ulotaront pretreatment. These regionally contrasting effects between mOFC and VS could indicate that ulotaront lessens transfer of incentive to the cue. The action of ulotaront might normalize aberrant learning and delusion formation in hyper-dopaminergic psychotic states. No drug effects were seen in the outcome phase in mOFC, suggesting no modification of outcome value, thought to be encoded in this brain region.

We found effects of schizotypy only in insula, a key component of the salience network. Anticipation of loss activated right insula only in HS subjects and this was normalized by ulotaront. This treatment × schizotypy interaction in ROI analysis was robust to FWE correction in the SVC analysis. The differing responses may reflect aberrant salience processing in anticipation of potential loss, which was reversed by both drugs. Aberrant salience processing has been implicated in positive symptoms of schizophrenia49. These results in HS are consistent with evidence implicating aberrant right insula structure and function in schizophrenia pathogenesis50,51,52. In left insula, ulotaront enhanced activations to wins and losses compared with attenuations after amisulpride. In right insula, ulotaront significantly enhanced loss responses but not wins. Enhanced insula responses to outcomes could predict beneficial effects of ulotaront on affective blunting, a key component of the negative syndrome53.

Considering the secondary objectives, in the N-Back task, neither ulotaront nor amisulpride modulated BOLD signals in prespecified ROIs associated with cognitive function54,55. Ulotaront reduced hippocampal deactivation compared to placebo, and LS participants showed reduced activation in prefrontal cortex, confirming task sensitivity to schizotypy56. Several studies corroborate stronger negative connectivity between prefrontal cortex and right hippocampus during working memory as an endophenotype of schizophrenia, and thus reduced right hippocampal deactivation in our N-back task would be compatible with an antipsychotic action of ulotaront57. However, ulotaront affected task performance by reducing accuracy across several conditions, including 0-Back trials. These findings may be partially due to a high proportion of participants (60%) experiencing somnolence/sedation. The reduced hippocampal deactivation may indicate decreased ability to switch off this region of the DMN in engaging executive networks for successful task completion58.

In RSC analyses, HS subjects showed reduced network intensity in dorsal and ventral DMN, ASN and right ECN. Each network is defined by the functional anatomy of the voxels whose low frequency BOLD fluctuations correlate with a shared underlying time series; the correlations being taken to reflect connectivity59,60. Recent studies suggested that schizophrenia is characterized by reduced connectivity within these networks52,61,62,63. HS participants generally showed reduced connectivity compared to LS under placebo, and both ulotaront and amisulpride reduced or abolished LS-HS differences with some selectivity for different networks. Ulotaront was effective in reversing decreased connectivity in HS participants in ASN (principally bilateral insula) but not significantly in DMN or ECN, whereas amisulpride was effective selectively in DMN. The selective effect of ulotaront in HS subjects in insula was remarkably convergent with the MID task finding that ulotaront reduced aberrant responses to losses in HS participants in this ROI and in keeping with other studies indicating a key role of insula in schizophrenia dysconnectivity52,62,64. Notwithstanding the interest of these results, it should be noted that there is considerable uncertainty about the unitary nature of schizotypy in the general population, how it is best measured, and its relationship to psychosis risk9. We suggest that the utility of cognition in schizotypy as a surrogate for psychosis in drug-development remains experimental.

There was little evidence of a schizotypy effect on regional CBF. Both ulotaront and amisulpride tended to reduce CBF in similar ROIs in LS and HS subjects. However, ulotaront treatment reduced CBF in DLPFC, posterior cingulate, insula and other regions to a greater extent in HS than LS subjects, producing significant interactions with schizotypy compared to placebo or amisulpride. This suggested ulotaront differentially affects CBF in HS subjects. Combined with RSC data, it appears that ulotaront and amisulpride affect CBF in structures prominent in overlapping networks, such as anterior cingulate (DMN), insula (ASN) and DLPFC (ECN). In each region, ulotaront reduced connectivity and CBF more in HS than LS subjects, suggesting it may engage processes underlying HS, and potentially those in schizophrenia. In contrast, neither ulotaront nor amisulpride showed an antidepressant-like profile on the ETB.

There are limitations to the current study. The investigation was designed to profile effects of ulotaront across a range of cognitive and connectivity biomarkers relevant to schizophrenia and to the antipsychotic and antidepressant behavioral profiles detected in preclinical development1. There is a problem of multiple comparisons inherent in profiling and maximizing the information extracted. This is exacerbated by analyzing factors other than treatment that could influence performance, including site, sex, schizotypy, task, behavioral measures, and ROIs. We attempted to mitigate this by selecting a primary outcome measure (decreased VS response in reward/loss anticipation) and powering sample size to 36/treatment arm. The study was not designed or powered to detect efficacy on a limited number of exploratory endpoints but rather to profile the effects of the new drug, ulotaront, on a range of measures of potential relevance to clinical development for schizophrenia.

The interpretation of some results is complicated by drowsiness reported by 60% of those taking ulotaront and their reduced response speed and accuracy in some of the tasks. In the MID task, minimally slower responses of the ulotaront group did not prevent the speeding induced by the prospect of winning and not losing and this suggests a full engagement in the task. Furthermore, the reduced activation in striatum after ulotaront was not due to the 8% increase in reaction time since it was unaffected by covarying reaction time in the ANOVA. In the N-Back task, covarying for poorer accuracy did not abolish the drug effects on fMRI responses in a post hoc analysis (data not shown). These mitigations notwithstanding, drug-induced sedation remains a potential confound in the task-evoked responses. However, ulotaront reversed the effect of HS versus LS in the insula both on BOLD responses to loss anticipation in the MID task, and in insula connectivity within the salience network. Differential HS versus LS effects of ulotaront were also seen in non-ROI regions in the N-Back task and in RSC. These HS selective effects would seem relatively immune from the confound of drowsiness.

In conclusion, the results indicated that ulotaront’s ability to reduce presynaptic dopamine function in preclinical development translated to phase 1 effects in modifying dopamine-related fMRI responses in the MID task. Furthermore, ulotaront reduced the effects of HS in insula in the MID and RSC and in other regions in the N-Back task. This translational evidence of functional target engagement by ulotaront together with the absence of effects in the ETB, led to phase 2 studies prioritizing efficacy trials in schizophrenia. Antipsychotic efficacy was subsequently reported in a phase 2 clinical trial (4) and ulotaront is currently undergoing evaluation for treatment of schizophrenia in randomized controlled phase 3 clinical trials.