Introduction

Models of the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) suggest abnormal tau spreads from the locus coeruleus (LC) to connected temporal lobe regions [1]. The LC is the brain’s primary source of norepinephrine, a neuromodulator that has been linked to a striking number of neuroprotective functions including reduction of inflammation [2] and promotion of neurotrophic factors [3]. Despite these neuroprotective functions, elevated norepinephrine metabolites are associated with greater tau pathology and cognitive impairment in AD [4,5,6,7], and may promote the cleavage of tau into aggregation and propagation-prone forms [8]. Given the therapeutic targeting of the LC-norepinephrine system in AD, basic research is needed to establish relationships between endogenous LC neurochemical function and tau pathology in vivo.

The central role of the LC in AD has made it a target for neuroimaging biomarker development. These efforts have concentrated on specialized magnetic resonance (MR) imaging sequences for which hyperintense MR signal is interpreted to reflect greater LC structural integrity (i.e., cell density) [9]. Greater LC structural integrity has been linked with indicators of successful aging including greater cortical thickness [10] and cognitive reserve [11, 12]. Research associating LC neuroimaging measures with the development of AD-related pathology in vivo is in its infancy. Initial studies find reduced LC integrity is associated with elevated cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) measures of β-amyloid in AD [13], and higher brain tau measured with positron emission tomography (PET) in aging [14] and autosomal dominant AD [15]. Together, these studies reveal LC to reflect neurobiological aging trajectories, which may be particularly valuable if applied to early detection of disease vulnerability.

The LC-norepinephrine system is critical for optimal cognitive performance [16, 17] including episodic memory [15, 18,19,20]. In mouse models of AD, LC lesions exacerbate memory impairments [21, 22]. This suggests the negative effect of AD-related pathology on episodic memory in humans may be heightened by accompanying disruption of the LC-norepinephrine system. Conversely, the successful maintenance of LC function despite AD pathology may support the relative preservation of memory, representing a neurobiological mechanism of cognitive resilience [23,24,25,26].

While methods development for structural LC imaging is accelerating, these methods do not provide direct insight into LC neurochemical function. We pursued LC imaging with the irreversible PET tracer [18F]Fluoro-m-tyrosine ([18F]FMT). [18F]FMT is a substrate for aromatic amino acid decarboxylase (AADC) and is a measure of monoamine synthesis capacity. [18F]FMT PET can be used to measure synthesis capacity of catecholamines norepinephrine and dopamine in the LC [27], catecholamine dopamine in the midbrain (ventral tegmental area/substantia nigra; VTA/SN) and striatum, and monoamine serotonin in the raphe nuclei. This study in cognitively normal older adults takes the first steps towards defining fundamental relationships among LC neurochemical function, AD-related tau pathology and episodic memory in aging. Parallel analyses in raphe, midbrain and striatum illustrate the specificity of these relationships.

Materials and Methods

Study design

Forty-nine older adults (mean age = 77.18, standard deviation (SD) = 5.80, range = 62–85, 29 female; mean years of education = 16.61, SD = 2.04, range = 12–20) are included and underwent [18F]FMT PET, [11C]Pittsburgh compound B PET ([11C]PiB) and memory testing. Of these participants, 42 underwent [18F]Flortaucipir PET. Participants were part of the Berkeley Aging Cohort Study, scored at least 25 on the Mini-Mental State Exam (mean = 28.69, SD = 1.26), and were characterized as cognitively normal [28]. Participants did not have a neurological, psychological‚ or psychiatric disorder, and did not take medication affecting cognition. The University of California, Berkeley, and Lawrence Berkeley National Laboratory Institutional Review Boards approved the study. Informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Structural MRI

Whole-brain images were acquired using a Siemens 3 T Trio Tim and 32-channel coil. T1-weighted volumetric magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo image sequence was acquired (TR = 2,3000 ms, TE = 2.98 ms, matrix = 256 × 256; FOV = 256; sagittal plane; 1 x 1 x 1 mm; 160 slices). Scans were segmented using FreeSurfer 5.3.0 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). Structural templates were generated in MNI space using Statistical Parametric Mapping software’s DARTEL (SPM12; Wellcome Trust Center for Neuroimaging, London, UK).

[18F]FMT PET

Participants underwent [18F]FMT PET scanning using a Siemens Biograph Truepoint 6 PET/CT and previously described methods [29, 30]. Supplemental Methods describe the strengths and limitations of [18F]FMT. Dynamic acquisition frames were obtained over 90 min in 3D mode. Frames were realigned, coregistered, and resliced to structural T1s [30]. Graphical analysis using a Patlak plot [31] was performed with a reference region approach rather than arterial plasma input function. Whole-brain images were generated using primary visual cortex (lingual gyrus and cuneus) reference region and PET frames corresponding to 25 to 90 min [32]. Whole-brain Kivis images were normalized to the DARTEL template. Kivis represents the amount of tracer accumulated in the brain relative to the visual cortex reference region. These images are comparable to Ki images obtained using an arterial plasma input function, but are scaled to the volume of tracer distribution in the reference region. However, this approach assumes minimal specific binding in visual cortex, and specific binding that does not vary substantially across individuals. It is also important to note that assuming the system is in equilibrium, Kivis is affected by K1, [18F]FMT’s rate constant from blood to tissue (product of perfusion and extraction). Therefore, it is possible that reduced delivery to the LC would result in lower LC Kivis. Future research measuring blood flow to LC will be necessary to determine the extent to which [18F]FMT Kivis is confounded by individual differences in K1.

Primary [18F]FMT analyses relied on LC regions of interest (ROIs) derived from an LC meta-ROI developed by Dahl and colleagues [15] based on previously published MR templates [18, 33,34,35,36]. Accurate coregistration (nearest-neighbor interpolation) was confirmed in Mango (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/mango/). To accommodate PET resolution, we smoothed the MR-derived LC ROI (4 mm FWHM) and thresholded ( > 0.147) to generate a mask of 479 1 x 1 x 1 mm voxels, that is approximately 6x the size of the original ROI (ROI available: https://neurovault.org/images/442231/). Given concerns about LC’s proximity to the raphe, we report parallel [18F]FMT Kivis analyses using dorsal and median raphe ROIs [37]. For completeness, we report results for VTA/SN, and striatum ROIs. Statistical analyses appear in main text, though see the Supplement for supporting Methods and Figures.

Though previous studies report PET signal in LC [38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47], for transparency, we display whole-brain voxel-wise analyses to support the use of [18F]FMT Kivis to measure brainstem signal. In the Supplement, we report voxel-wise regression analyses with LC [18F]FMT Kivis as the independent variable to illustrate the degree to which [18F]FMT Kivis measured in the LC ROI covaried with [18F]FMT Kivis throughout the brain, giving insight into the regional specificity of signal patterns in the brainstem (Figure S1).

[18F]Flortaucipir and [11C]PiB PET

All participants underwent [11C]PiB scanning to assess β-amyloid status and a subset underwent [18F]Flortaucipir scanning (n = 42) to measure tau pathology within 3 years of their [18F]FMT scan. The median time difference between [18F]FMT and [11C]PiB scans was 328 days (SD = 245; range = 14–966). The median time difference between [18F]FMT and [18F]Flortaucipir scanning was 176 days (SD = 236; range = 9–945). A subset of participants (n = 30; 10 [11C]PiB positive, 20 [11C]PiB negative) had multiple [18F]Flortaucipir scans (average number of timepoints per participant = 2.5; total number of [18F]Flortaucipir scans = 74).

A detailed description of [11C]PiB and [18F]Flortaucipir acquisition has been published previously [48]. [11C]PiB distribution volume ratio (DVR) images were generated with Logan graphical analysis on frames corresponding to 35–90 min post-injection using cerebellar gray matter reference region [49]. β-amyloid-positivity was based on global cortical [11C]PiB distribution volume ratio 1.065 or greater [50, 51].

[18F]Flortaucipir SUVR images were generated from frames corresponding to 80–100 min post-injection using inferior cerebellar gray matter reference. SUVR images were partial volume corrected as previously described [52]. [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR was measured within a previously established PET meta-ROI comprised of tau-vulnerable regions [53]. The tau PET meta-ROI was formed from a voxel-weighted average of the mean uptake in entorhinal, amygdala, parahippocampal, fusiform, inferior temporal, and middle temporal ROIs. Due to the relatively small sample (n = 30), longitudinal [18F]Flortaucipir analyses are considered preliminary. Longitudinal [18F]Flortaucipir scans were processed with a longitudinal pipeline and white matter reference region as previously described [54]. To measure change in [18F]Flortaucipir, the partial volume corrected SUVR at each time point was entered into linear mixed effects models with random effects for participant intercept and slope.

Neuropsychological testing

Participants underwent neuropsychological testing (median time difference from [18F]FMT = 144 days; SD = 101; range = 6–420). A composite memory measure was calculated using each participant’s California Verbal Learning Test-II [55] performance. Based on previous research demonstrating relationships between the LC-norepinephrine system and verbal learning and memory measures [7, 15, 18, 20], the composite was comprised of free recall performance during learning trials 1-5 and free recall following the long delay (Supplemental Methods).

To investigate the role of higher LC catecholamine synthesis capacity as a mechanism of cognitive resilience and reserve, we explored its relationship with a proxy measure of cognitive reserve: cognitive engagement across the lifespan. We measured Lifetime Experiences Questionnaire (LEQ) [56] total score, which assesses complex mental activity and physical activity across the lifetime. We excluded LEQ data from two participants due to incomplete responding, and misinterpretation of instructions (some additive rather than discrete estimations across years).

Statistical testing

Analyses were performed using IBM SPSS v27. Multiple linear regression models included age, sex, and years of education as covariates. We report regression models with and without [18F]FMT Kivis and [11C]PiB interaction terms given ambiguity as to whether tau-pathologic processes involving the LC are AD-related or age-related [57, 58]. For interaction models, independent variables were mean-centered. Years of education was excluded for regression models involving LEQ, as education contributes to this measure. Bootstrap 95% confidence intervals were calculated using the Robust Correlation Toolbox in MATLAB 2020b [59]. Post hoc testing of the moderation analysis testing a role of LC [18F]FMT Kivis in cognitive resilience was performed using PROCESS v4.0 [60].

Results

Visualization of LC with [18F]FMT PET

Whole-brain voxel-wise analysis of [18F]FMT Kivis maps revealed a highly significant contiguous midline cluster in the midbrain and brainstem with peaks consistent with the VTA/SN, LC, and raphe nuclei (Fig. 1A; Table S1A). While brainstem signal was greatest in midline regions consistent with the raphe, [18F]FMT Kivis signal in lateral regions consistent with LC is clear in individual participant scans at thresholds typically used to visualize [18F]FMT binding in the striatum (Fig. 1B).

Fig. 1: Locus coeruleus-related [18F]Fluoro-m-tyrosine signal ([18F]FMT).
figure 1

A Whole-brain analyses of [18F]FMT data for all subjects (n = 49) reveals significant PET signal in regions consistent with the substantia nigra/ventral tegmental area (SN/VTA), raphe nuclei, and locus coeruleus (LC) (one-sample t-test; voxel-wise threshold p < 1 × 1016). Inset displays overlaid regions of interest (ROIs). Results are overlaid on the older adults’ study-specific DARTEL template. B [18F]FMT Kivis scans overlaid on structural T1 scans in native space for 3 participants.

LC catecholamine synthesis capacity is associated with tau burden in Aβ + participants

[18F]FMT Kivis within the LC ROI did not differ between [11C]PiB positive (n = 14) and negative participants (n = 35; t(47) = 0.73, p = 0.47). [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR in tau vulnerable regions (Fig. 2A), was higher in [11C]PiB positive older adults (Levene’s test for equality of variances p = 0.001; t(12) = 2.65, p = 0.02, d = 1.29, 95% CI [0.56, 2.01]; n = 12 [11C]PiB positive, n = 30 [11C]PiB negative; Fig. 2B), replicating previous reports [53]. LC [18F]FMT Kivis was not associated with [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR in a model adjusting for age, sex and years of education (Table 1A). A model including [11C]PiB status and LC [18F]FMT by [11C]PiB interaction revealed a significant interaction (Table 1A; Fig. 2C). Higher LC [18F]FMT Kivis was related to lower [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR in [11C]PiB positive (r = −0.60 [−0.92 −0.03], p = 0.04) but not [11C]PiB negative participants (r = 0.12 [−0.39 0.40], p = 0.54). Adjusting for days between [18F]Flortaucipir and [18F]FMT scan did not affect the interaction (beta = −0.48, p = 0.002). [11C]PiB negative and positive groups did not differ in the time between [18F]Flortaucipir and [18F]FMT scans (t(41) = 0.39, p = 0.35).

Fig. 2: Relationship between [18F]Fluoro-m-tyrosine ([18F]FMT) Ki and [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR.
figure 2

A [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR was measured in a voxel-weighted meta-ROI. B [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR was related to β-amyloid status as defined by [11C]Pittsburgh compound B (PiB). [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR was significantly higher in [11C]PiB positive subjects. C A regression model with [18F]Flortaucipir as the dependent variable revealed an interaction between locus coeruleus (LC) [18F]FMT Kivis and [11C]PiB status. D This schematic displays the temporal relationship between [18F]FMT PET and longitudinal [18F]Flortaucipir PET scans. E There was not a difference in [18F]Flortaucipir slope between [11C]PiB positive and negative individuals. F A regression model with [18F]Flortaucipir slope derived from the linear mixed effect model as the dependent variable revealed an interaction between LC [18F]FMT Kivis and [11C]PiB status.

Table 1 Relationship between locus coeruleus [18F]FMT Kivis and [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR.

Suggesting our findings are specific to LC [18F]FMT Kivis, analyses of raphe [18F]FMT Kivis found no relationship with [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR (dorsal: beta = 0.22, p = 0.20; median: beta = 0.25, p = 0.15; Fig. S2) and no interactions with [11C]PiB status (dorsal: beta = 0.07, p = 0.67; median: beta = −0.01, p = 0.93). Similarly, analyses of midbrain (VTA/SN) and striatal ROIs found no relationship with [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR (midbrain: beta = 0.001, p = 0.99; striatum: beta = 0.11, p = 0.49; Fig. S2) and no interactions with [11C]PiB status (midbrain: beta = 0.11, p = 0.47; striatum: beta = 0.06, p = 0.70).

Summary Fig. 2D depicts longitudinal [18F]Flortaucipir acquisition timepoints relative to each participant’s [18F]FMT scan. There was not a significant difference in [18F]Flortaucipir slopes for [11C]PiB positive (n = 10) relative to negative participants (n = 20; Levene’s test for equality of variances p = 0.02; t(10) = 1.60, p = 0.14; Fig. 2E). Higher [18F]Flortaucipir slope was associated with lower LC [18F]FMT Kivis for a model adjusting for age, sex, years of education and [11C]PiB status (Table 1B). Consistent with cross-sectional analyses, there was a significant interaction between [18F]FMT Kivis and [11C]PiB status suggesting that relationships between LC [18F]FMT Kivis and increasing [18F]Flortaucipir are stronger in [11C]PiB positive participants, though the correlation was only marginal for [11C]PiB positive participants alone (Table 1B; Fig. 2F; [11C]PiB positive: r = −0.59 [−0.78, 0.35], p = 0.07; [11C]PiB negative: r = −0.35 [−0.75 0.30], p = 0.13). We do not interpret the lack of significance of this relationship strongly given the limited sample size (10 [11C]PiB positive). The significant [18F]FMT Kivis * [11C]PiB status interaction was not affected by the addition of a covariate adjusting for relative timing of [18F]Flortaucipir scans before or after the [18F]FMT Kivis scan (interaction beta = −0.52, p = 0.04; Supplementary Methods).

These relationships were not evident for analyses of raphe [18F]FMT Kivis, midbrain or striatum. In contrast to LC [18F]FMT Kivis findings, no other ROI predicted [18F]Flortaucipir slope for models including [11C]PiB status (dorsal raphe: beta = 0.15, p = 0.55 median raphe: beta = 0.19, p = 0.42; midbrain: beta = 0.06, p = 0.77; striatum: beta = −0.04, p = 0.84; Fig. S2). Further, there were no interactions with [11C]PiB status (dorsal: beta = 0.06, p = 0.77; median: beta = 0.11, p = 0.59; midbrain: beta = −0.14, p = 0.49; striatum: beta = −0.09, p = 0.65).

Tau, LC catecholamine synthesis capacity, and memory

LC [18F]FMT Kivis did not predict CVLT memory composite (beta = 0.07, p = 0.64; Fig. 3A). A model including [11C]PiB status revealed a relationship between [11C]PiB status and memory (beta = −0.31, p = 0.03; n = 12 [11C]PiB positive, n = 30 [11C]PiB negative), but no interaction between LC [18F]FMT Kivis and [11C]PiB (beta = 0.10, p = 0.53). Consistent with previous reports [61], higher [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR predicted lower memory (beta = −0.33, p = 0.04; Fig. 3B). This relationship was strongest for [11C]PiB positive participants ([18F]Flortaucipir * [11C]PiB; beta = −0.57, p = 0.02).

Fig. 3: Associations among locus coeruleus [18F]Fluoro-m-tyrosine (LC [18F]FMT Kivis), memory, and tau.
figure 3

A There was no relationship between LC [18F]FMT Kivis and memory as measured by the California Verbal Learning Test-II (CVLT) composite. B There was a negative relationship between tau burden measured with [18F]Flortaucipir and memory. This relationship was driven by β-amyloid positive participants as defined by [11C]Pittsburgh compound B (PiB). Correlations for [11C]PiB positive and negative participants are displayed separately. C There was a significant interaction between LC [18F]FMT Kivis and [18F]Flortaucipir such that the effect of tau on memory was absent in conditions of high LC [18F]FMT Kivis. We present discrete groupings of the continuous LC [18F]FMT Kivis variable for display purposes.

While LC [18F]FMT Kivis was not associated with better memory overall, we examined whether higher LC [18F]FMT Kivis was associated with better-than-expected memory given an individual’s tau burden. In line with the framework of resilience, we tested for the critical interaction between LC [18F]FMT Kivis and [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR [23, 24]:

Memory ~ [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR + LC [18F]FMT Kivis + [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR * LC [18F]FMT Kivis + age + sex + years of education.

This model revealed a significant interaction between [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR and LC [18F]FMT Kivis (beta = 0.37, p = 0.04). Addition of [11C]PiB status to the model did not change these results (beta = 0.37, p = 0.04). To visualize the [18F]Flortaucipir * [18F]FMT Kivis interaction, the unstandardized simple slopes were tested for low (−1 SD), moderate, and high (+1 SD) levels of LC [18F]FMT Kivis [60]. Variation in the slopes of the regression lines with varying levels of [18F]FMT Kivis show results consistent with a role of [18F]FMT Kivis in cognitive resilience. The negative impact of tau pathology on memory is only evident for individuals with low and medium levels of LC [18F]FMT Kivis. At high levels of LC [18F]FMT Kivis, this relationship was not apparent (Fig. 3C).

Analyses substituting raphe, midbrain, and striatal ROIs found no relationships with memory (dorsal: beta = 0.02, p = 0.91; median: beta = 0.01, p = 0.94; midbrain: beta = 0.10, p = 0.51; striatum: beta = -0.06, p = 0.68; Figure S3) or interactions with [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR (dorsal: beta = -0.21, p = 0.30; median: beta = −0.19, p = 0.31; midbrain: beta = 0.11, p = 0.52; striatum: beta = −0.08, p = 0.62).

LC catecholamine synthesis capacity and lifespan cognitive engagement

Evidence of a moderating effect of LC [18F]FMT Kivis on tau’s negative relationship with memory is consistent with the interpretation that higher LC catecholamine synthesis represents a mechanism of cognitive resilience [24]. However, there were relatively few participants with high levels of [18F]Flortaucipir SUVR, leading us to interpret this interaction with caution. Next, we tested LC [18F]FMT Kivis’s relationship with a proxy measure of cognitive reserve, intellectual engagement and physical activity across the lifespan, using the LEQ [56]. Higher LEQ was associated with higher LC [18F]FMT Kivis (beta = 0.33, p = 0.03; n = 46; Fig. 4). There was no interaction between [11C]PiB and LEQ (beta = −0.26, p = 0.11). Parallel analyses substituting raphe [18F]FMT Kivis found marginal positive relationships with LEQ (dorsal raphe: beta = 0.27, p = 0.07; median raphe: beta = 0.28, p = 0.053; Fig. S4), and no relationship for midbrain and striatal ROIs (midbrain: beta = 0.12, p = 0.43; striatum: beta = −0.06, p = 0.70; Figure S4). There were no interactions between [11C]PiB and LEQ for raphe and midbrain ROIs (beta = −0.09, p = 0.57), but a significant [11C]PiB * LEQ interaction in predicting striatal dopamine synthesis capacity (beta = −0.34, p = 0.04). This interaction was driven by [11C]PiB positive individuals who showed higher striatal dopamine synthesis capacity associated with lower LEQ ([11C]PiB positive: r = −0.59 [−0.85, −0.08], p = 0.03; [11C]PiB negative: r = 0.10 [−0.25, 0.43], p = 0.57; Fig. S4). The direction of this relationship is opposite to what is observed for LC and raphe ROIs, but is broadly consistent with our previous research suggesting striatal dopamine synthesis is upregulated in aging in the context of brain structural decline [62].

Fig. 4: Lifetime cognitive engagement and [18F]Fluoro-m-tyrosine ([18F]FMT) Kivis.
figure 4

There was a positive relationship between locus coeruleus (LC) [18F]FMT Ki and self-reported cognitive and physical activity across the lifespan as measured by the Lifetime Experiences Questionnaire (LEQ). β-amyloid status as defined by [11C]Pittsburgh compound B (PiB) is identified for individual subjects.

Discussion

This study establishes, for the first time, relationships among PET measures of LC neurochemical function and AD-related pathology, and provides insight into their combined effect on episodic memory performance. These findings suggest [18F]FMT Kivis is a valuable biomarker of LC neurochemical function and opens up new lines of research examining a protective role of neuromodulator systems in AD and, because the causal directions of these associations are not established, the role of AD pathology in altering LC neurochemistry.

We provide the first in vivo evidence that LC neurochemical function is related to the degree of tau burden and rates of tau accumulation in humans. In older adults with β-amyloid, higher LC catecholamine synthesis capacity was associated with lower temporal lobe tau burden. Further, higher LC catecholamine synthesis capacity was associated with lower rates of tau accumulation over time in a model adjusting for β-amyloid status (n = 30). Specifically, lower longitudinal tau slopes were associated with higher LC catecholamine synthesis capacity. These relationships were selective for LC, and were not observed for [18F]FMT signal in the raphe, midbrain, and striatum. The negative association is consistent with two interpretations, which are not mutually exclusive. First, advanced progression of tau pathology may be associated with greater LC tau burden and degeneration, which may reduce neurotransmitter synthesis. Second, higher LC catecholamine synthesis may actively stave off tau’s progression via neuroprotective functions [25, 63]. Measurement of tau burden in LC is not possible with available tracers, though longitudinal studies may disambiguate the directionality of these associations.

The neuroprotective functions of norepinephrine [2, 3, 64,65,66,67,68] have inspired proposals that the maintenance of LC neurochemical function confers AD resistance [25, 63], which are supported by demonstrations that the loss of LC-catecholamine signaling increases accumulation of β-amyloid and tau pathology in AD mouse models [21, 22]. However, there is substantial complexity to this line of research, as higher norepinephrine metabolites may promote tau propagation [8], and higher levels of norepinephrine metabolites in AD are associated with greater tau pathology [4, 5]. Norepinephrine metabolites are elevated across the AD spectrum [4, 6, 69], suggesting that there is upregulation of norepinephrine activity in response to disease [69]. It is possible that elevation of norepinephrine activity is desirable, even compensatory, in aging and preclinical AD, but, in later stages, beneficial effects are no longer evident and excessive metabolite generation signals greater disease severity. Additional research is needed to establish how CSF metabolite measures relate to PET measures, and to test the longitudinal trajectories of these measures in preclinical AD and MCI.

Higher LC catecholamine synthesis capacity appeared to mitigate tau’s negative effect on memory. The mechanisms by which LC catecholamines enhance memory are well established. The LC projects to the medial temporal lobe [70], and catecholamine release in hippocampus modulates long-term potentiation and depression [71]. In AD mouse models, treatment with a norepinephrine precursor partially restores memory performance despite pathology [72]. Complementing these findings, we found the effect of tau on memory to be reduced in conditions of high catecholamine synthesis. Conferral of better-than-expected cognitive performance given one’s pathology burden has been interpreted to reflect resilience [24], and is consistent with proposals that the norepinephrine system is central to mechanisms of reserve in AD [25, 26].

While the norepinephrine system affects cognition [16], cognition likely also affects the norepinephrine system [73, 74]. Specifically, cognitive activity may play a causal role in modifying the LC-norepinephrine system, allowing active older adults to maintain healthy LC-norepinephrine function. In rodent models, experiments manipulating the complexity and novelty of the environment find these interventions increase norepinephrine concentration in the brainstem and LC targets [73] and release in hippocampus [74]. As these studies assessed acute effects, the extent to which such enrichment enduringly impacts norepinephrine function, potentially bestowing brain reserve, is unknown. It is important to note that in addition to higher self-reported cognitive and physical activities (LEQ) being associated with higher LC catecholamine synthesis capacity, we found a marginal relationship between higher LEQ and serotonin synthesis capacity in the raphe nuclei, suggesting that the integrity of multiple neuromodulator systems in older age may be associated with lifestyle factors [75].

There are important limitations of this study. It will be critical for future [18F]FMT studies to measure LC-related signal using an arterial plasma input function and pair these measurements with MR perfusion measures to determine the extent to which LC [18F]FMT Ki is affected by blood flow. Additionally, there were relatively few β-amyloid-positive participants. It will be important to replicate our findings in a larger sample. This study was limited by long temporal delays between the acquisition of PET scans. We believe these time differences between scans did not introduce systematic bias as covarying for time did not change our results. Finally, the limited spatial resolution of PET and AADC activity in the raphe nuclei may have reduced our sensitivity for detecting relationships among LC catecholamine function, cognition, and AD-related pathology. Future test-retest reliability assessments for these small structures are needed.

The LC represents a principal site of interactions between neuromodulator systems and AD-related pathology, which are rarely studied in conjunction in humans. Advancing norepinephrine imaging methods promises to inform treatment approaches directly targeting the LC-norepinephrine system [63]. For example, vagal stimulation research developing interventions targeting the LC-norepinephrine system [76,77,78] would benefit from understanding an individual’s baseline LC neurochemical function. The addition of neurochemical imaging tools to complement the rapidly accelerating field of LC biomarker development is a valuable investment given LC’s role in basic cognition and the etiology of neurological and psychiatric disorders.