Introduction

Insomnia is an established risk factor for depression onset, maintenance, and recurrence which becomes more prevalent with age [1,2,3,4]. Indeed, over one-third of older adults suffer from insomnia, and late-life insomnia has been shown to prospectively predict incident depressive symptoms and major depressive disorder [2, 5]. In a large multi-site study, the continued presence of insomnia predicted the maintenance of depression in older adults over a 12-month period, and this was only partially mitigated by active antidepressant treatment [3]. Furthermore, sleep disturbance has been shown to predict depression recurrence in older adults independent of depressive symptom severity, chronic illness, or antidepressant use [6]. The mechanisms that contribute to the link between insomnia and depression are not known, and the absence of research is particularly striking in older adults given their prevalence of insomnia and the adverse effects of late-life depression on psychosocial and physical health [1, 7, 8].

Insomnia may increase risk for late-life depression through dysregulation in the reward system. Reward dysregulation is not only a common feature of depression [9], but is also evident in nondepressed at-risk populations [10, 11] and prospectively predicts onset of depression and increases in depressive symptoms [12, 13]. Anhedonia, a cardinal symptom of depression, is thought to be due to alterations in reward processing and can manifest as reduced motivation for reward (i.e., reduced “wanting”), reduced sensitivity for reward (i.e., reduced “liking”), and/or blunted reward learning. Older adults report difficulties with motivation in the context of depression [7] and are hypothesized to be vulnerable to reward dysregulation due to age-related decreases in frontostriatal dopaminergic function [14, 15].

In striking contrast to the prevalence of insomnia in older adults [1], as well as evidence of reward dysregulation in older adults [14, 15], no study to our knowledge has examined whether insomnia might contribute to changes in reward processing in older adults, although some research in adults supports this emerging hypothesis [16, 17]. One small study found that adults with sleep complaints had lower desire for (i.e., “wanting”) and enjoyment of (i.e., “liking”) rewarding experiences than comparison controls over 7 days of self-report experience sampling [18]. Other work found that diagnostic insomnia is associated with reduced gray matter volume [19, 20] and altered functional connectivity [21] in reward-related brain regions, although assessment of reward processes was not performed. It is not known whether insomnia as diagnosed by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM)-5 [22] is associated with behavioral deficits in reward processing.

Furthermore, no studies have evaluated individual difference factors, such as sex and inflammatory burden, which could modulate effects of late-life insomnia on the reward system. Inflammation is implicated in depression pathophysiology, associated with alterations in reward function [23, 24], and elevated following sleep disturbance [25]. Moreover, inflammation predicts treatment response to antidepressants [24] and electroconvulsive therapy [26] as well as susceptibility to stress in preclinical models [27]. It is not known if elevated inflammation confers enhanced vulnerability to reward dysregulation in the context of late-life insomnia. Sex-specific effects in relation to insomnia have received little empirical attention, although adult females have been shown to exhibit lower motivation for monetary reward as compared to males [28], as well as greater alterations in reward processing following an inflammatory challenge [29].

To address these gaps, community dwelling older adults with and without late-life insomnia underwent evaluation of reward processing by administration of a standard monetary reward task, which assessed both reward motivation (i.e., “wanting”) and reward sensitivity (i.e., “liking”). It is hypothesized that, in comparison to older adults without insomnia, those with late-life insomnia will exhibit (1) reduced reward motivation; and (2) reduced reward sensitivity. Secondary study objectives are to test for sex-specific effects and whether elevated inflammation potentiates these associations.

Materials and methods

Participants and procedure

Data for the current study were obtained from the baseline assessment of an ongoing randomized controlled trial investigating psychobiological effects of an acute inflammatory challenge (R01# AG051944; Sleep and Healthy Aging Research on Depression Study; NCT03256760). All study procedures were approved by the University of California (UCLA) Institutional Review Board. Baseline assessment included administration of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 (SCID-DSM-5) by trained research staff and completion of self-report questionnaires and behavioral tasks. Participants were 104 healthy older adults (aged 60–79) who either met (n = 31; 21 female) or did not meet (n = 73; 31 female) criteria for insomnia disorder (i.e., late-life insomnia) as assessed by the SCID-DSM-5. Exclusion criteria are described in detail in Supplementary Material and included among others current psychiatric illness as assessed by the DSM-5 except insomnia disorder; cognitive impairment as indicated by a score < 24 on the Mini-Mental Status Exam (MMSE)Footnote 1; sleep apnea as confirmed by polysomnography; presence of an acute infectious illness in the 2 weeks prior to an experimental session (via self-report and white blood cell count within normal range); BMI > 35 kg/m2; smoking; use of medications known to modulate the immune system; use of over the counter or prescription sleep aids (via self-report).

Participants were recruited from the Los Angeles community August 2016–July 2019 using the GENESYS Sampling Systems company, which provided contact information for households with at least one person aged 60 years or older within a 10 mile radius of UCLA. A total of 283 responded to recruitment letters and phone calls and completed phone and in-person eligibility screening. Out of 117 eligible participants who provided C-reactive protein (CRP) data, 115 provided complete self-report questionnaire data and 113 completed the Effort Expenditure for Rewards Task (EEfRT) (data lost by computer malfunction (n = 4)). Six participants were excluded for completing fewer than 18 trials on the EEfRT, and one participant was excluded due to levels of CRP at 36.3 mg/L. In total, 104 participants provided complete data across all measures; participants with missing data did not differ from participants with full data in terms of insomnia diagnosis or demographic or psychosocial characteristics (Table S2).

Assessments

Insomnia and related behavioral symptoms

Insomnia disorder was diagnosed by SCID-DSM-5 and confirmed in a diagnostic consensus meeting with a board-certified psychiatrist (MRI). Severity of insomnia symptoms was assessed with the Insomnia Severity Index. Severity of depressive symptoms was assessed by the Beck Depression Inventory-II; the scale item relating to sleep quality was removed prior to calculating the total score. Physical fatigue symptoms were assessed with the physical fatigue subscale from the Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory Scale. Depressive and fatigue symptoms were assessed and included as covariates in statistical analyses to ensure they were not contributing to possible group differences in reward processing; both have been associated with reward motivation [30, 31] and insomnia [32].

Behavioral reward task

The EEfRT [28] was used to evaluate reward processing. The EEfRT is a computerized task that assesses effort-based decision making in the context of monetary reward. During the task, participants are presented with a series of trials in which they choose between an easy, low-effort trial (worth a low reward amount of $1.00) and a hard, high-effort trial (worth higher reward amounts ranging between $1.24 and $4.30). Easy trials required 30 button presses using the index finger of the nondominant hand in 7 s, while hard trials required 100 button presses with the pinky finger of the dominant hand in 21 s. Participants were told that not all successfully completed trials would be rewarded, and the probability that a successful response would yield a reward (12, 50, and 88%) was presented for each trial. In the current study, the EEfRT was shortened from 20 to 10 min and hard trials used the pinky finger of the dominant hand rather than the nondominant hand to accommodate constraints in the laboratory environment (also used in [23]).

Motivation for reward on the EEfRT is operationalized by willingness to exert effort for monetary reward; i.e., the selection of high-effort/high-reward trials relative to the selection of low-effort/low-reward trials. Sensitivity to reward is operationalized by the association between changes in monetary reward magnitude (ranging from $1.24 to $4.30) and changes in likelihood of selecting high-effort/high-reward trials vs. low-effort/low-reward trials [23, 33].

Inflammation

Inflammation was assessed by measuring circulating levels of the systemic inflammatory marker CRP between the hours of 8 and 9 a.m. Participants provided blood samples prior to receiving breakfast as part of the larger experimental protocol, but were not required to fast. Blood samples were collected by venipuncture into EDTA tubes, placed on ice, centrifuged for acquisition of plasma, and stored at −80 for batch testing. CRP levels were determined utilizing the Human CRP Quantikine ELISA (R&D Systems) according to the manufacturer’s protocol with a lower limit of detection of 0.2 mg/L. Samples were assayed in duplicate. Intra- and inter-assay precision of all tests was <6.1%.

Sample size

No studies have evaluated group differences in effort-based decision making in association with insomnia disorder. Several studies have evaluated EEfRT performance in other patient populations relative to comparison control participants and yielded large effect sizes (d > 0.8) [30, 31]. Assuming an effect size of d = 0.8, 100 participants in the current study provide statistical power of >90% (α = 0.05) to detect differences in EEfRT performance as a function of insomnia, the primary objective of the current study. For secondary analyses, past studies have shown small to medium effects of sex [23, 28] and inflammation [23] on EEfRT performance. Given these estimates, we recognize that secondary analyses that examine moderating effects of sex or inflammation on reward processing as a function of insomnia are exploratory, as they are not adequately powered.

Analytic approach

Data reduction

Participants who chose fewer than 18 total trials were excluded (n = 6; 5 comparison controls). Individual EEfRT trials in which the participant did not choose between an easy or hard task were excluded (7.28% of all trials). Participants successfully completed 93.94% of the hard trials and 82.46% of the easy trials; trials that the participant selected but completed fewer than 10% of the required button presses were excluded (n = 39 trials; 0.96% of all trials).

Generalized estimating equations (GEE) models

GEE with a binary logistic model and independent working correlation structure were used to analyze decision-making behavior on the EEfRT; this is a typical approach for this task as GEE account for correlated data and are appropriate for a binary dependent variable [28]. The working correlation structure was determined with the STATA program QIC (quasilikelihood under the independence model criterion).

Consistent with previous studies [28], the following task-specific variables were included as continuous time-varying covariates: reward magnitude (range $1.24–$4.30), probability, and, to control for fatigue effects during the task, trial number (see Table S1 for GEE models with these task-specific variables only). Between-subject predictors were group (insomnia vs. control), severity of depressive symptoms, severity of physical fatigue, and sex. The dependent binary variable was coded as 0 (low-effort/low-reward choice) and 1 (high-effort/high-reward choice). Level of significance was set at p < 0.05 and hypothesis tests were two sided.

Assessment of reward motivation

Motivation for reward was operationalized as willingness to exert effort for a monetary reward. Hence, reduced reward motivation was characterized as being less willing to exert effort for a higher amount of monetary reward (i.e., choosing fewer high-effort/high-reward trials and more low-effort/low-reward trials). The predictor of interest for the primary analysis was the group (insomnia vs. control) variable. For sex-specific analyses, sex differences in reward motivation were tested as the main effect of sex in the prediction of willingness to exert effort for a high monetary reward. Sex differences in relation to insomnia were tested with a two-way interaction term (sex by group).

Assessment of reward sensitivity

Sensitivity to reward was operationalized by the association between changes in monetary reward magnitude and changes in likelihood of selecting high-effort/high-reward trials vs. low-effort/low-reward trials [23, 33]. Hence, reduced reward sensitivity was represented by an attenuated association between increases in potential monetary reward magnitude and increased likelihood of selecting high-effort/high-reward trials. Late-life insomnia vs. comparison control group differences in reward sensitivity were tested using an interaction term between group and reward magnitude. For sex-specific analyses, sex differences in reward sensitivity were tested with an interaction term between sex and reward magnitude; this allowed us to ascertain whether changes in monetary reward magnitude were associated with changes in likelihood of selecting high-effort/high-reward trials differently as a function of sex. Sex differences in relation to late-life insomnia for reward sensitivity were tested with a three-way interaction term (sex by reward magnitude by group).

Assessment of CRP in late-life insomnia and in association with EEfRT performance

Levels of CRP were evaluated for outliers and due to positive skew were log transformed prior to analyses; seven participants with levels below the limit of detection were imputed to 50% of the lower limit of detection; one participant was excluded as an outlier (CRP of 36.3 mg/L) and all other CRP values were within 3 standard deviations of the mean. To evaluate CRP as a moderator of the effect of late-life insomnia on reward motivation, a two-way interaction term between group (binary) and CRP (continuous) was tested in the GEE model predicting hard trial choice. Sex-specific analyses included a three-way interaction term (sex by group by CRP). To evaluate CRP as a moderator of the effect of late-life insomnia on reward sensitivity, a three-way interaction term (group by reward magnitude by CRP) was tested. Sex-specific analyses included a four-way interaction term (sex by group by reward magnitude by CRP). Models with CRP controlled for age, BMI, and ethnicity.

Results

Participant characteristics

Participants were community dwelling older adults (Mage = 65.86) with (n = 31) and without (n = 73) late-life insomnia who were predominantly white, educated, and of normal BMI (Table 1). There were more females than males in the insomnia group (n = 21 females) but not in the comparison control group (n = 31 females). The groups did not otherwise differ on sociodemographic characteristics (Table 1). Older adults with late-life insomnia had significantly higher severity of insomnia, depressive, and physical fatigue symptoms, as compared to comparison controls (Table 1), with no further differences as a function of sex (see Table S3 for differences by sex collapsing across groups).

Table 1 Demographic and psychosocial characteristics of community dwelling older adults with and without late-life insomnia.

Levels of CRP were on average low (M = 1.73 mg/L, SD = 2.38; range: 0.1–14.55 mg/L) and did not differ by sex (p = .761). CRP was significantly elevated in individuals with late-life insomnia relative to comparison controls, t(114) = 2.040, p = .044, d = .413, and CRP was correlated with severity of insomnia symptoms, r = 0.280, p = 0.002, for all participants with CRP data. Similar patterns were evident for the subset of participants with full data across all measures, t(102) = 1.600, p = 0.113, d = 0.343, r = 0.292, p = 0.002.

Reward motivation

Late-life insomnia was hypothesized to be associated with reduced reward motivation. Indeed, results showed that those with late-life insomnia were significantly less likely to select hard (high effort/high reward) trials than comparison controls (b = −0.580, SE = 0.078, p < 0.001; 95% CI [−0.732, −0.428]). This association remained significant controlling for sex, depressive and physical fatigue symptoms, and task-specific variables (i.e., reward magnitude, probability, and trial number) (95% CI [−0. 955, −0.569]; Table 2; Fig. 1). To further interrogate the role of insomnia on reward motivation, we examined whether severity of insomnia symptoms was associated with hard trial choice across the sample, and found that more severe insomnia symptoms predicted fewer hard trial choices in unadjusted (95% CI [−0. 026, −0.0004]) and adjusted (95% CI [−0. 048, −0. 013]) models (Table S4). Thus, both insomnia diagnosis and severity of insomnia in older adults were associated with a shift away from high-effort/high-reward trials and toward low-effort/low-reward trials, indicative of reduced reward motivation for monetary reward in the context of effort-based decision making.

Table 2 Reward motivation is reduced in older adults with late-life insomnia relative to comparison controls (Model 1) with sex-specific effects (Model 2) using Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE) and adjusting for participant characteristics and task-specific variables. Reward sensitivity is reduced in those with late-life insomnia relative to comparison controls (Model 3) with no sex-specific effects (Model 4).
Fig. 1: Reward motivation in relation to late-life insomnia and sex plotted with 95% confidence intervals and adjusted for participant and task characteristics.
figure 1

Reward motivation is significantly reduced in older adults with late-life insomnia (n = 31; 21 female) relative to comparison controls (n = 73; 31 female) (p < 0.001; (a)) with evidence of sex-specific effects (p = 0.001; (b)). Late-life insomnia is associated with reduced reward motivation in males (p < 0.001) and females (p < 0.001), and this reduction is greater for males than females (p = 0.002).

Sex differences and reward motivation

Female sex was associated with reduced reward motivation, i.e., lower likelihood of selecting hard trials, in comparison to male sex, as indicated by a main effect of sex in unadjusted (b = −0.421, SE = 0.078, p < 0.001; 95% CI [−0.564, −0.278]) and adjusted (95% CI [−0.448, −0.137]; Table 2) models. To examine whether this sex difference further varied as a function of insomnia status, a two-way interaction term between sex and insomnia group was examined. The interaction between sex and insomnia was significant, such that late-life insomnia was associated with a reduction in selection of hard trials to a greater degree in males as compared to females in unadjusted (b = 0.390, SE = 0.163, p = 0.017; 95% CI [0.070, 0.710]) and adjusted (95% CI [−0.225, −0.915]; Table 2) models (Fig. 1). Follow-up within-group analyses found that both males (b = −0.248, SE = 0.033, p < 0.001) and females (b = −0.127, SE = 0.026, p < 0.001) exhibited a reduction in monetary reward motivation in association with late-life insomnia, and this difference was significantly greater for males, X2(1) = 9.97, p = 0.002.

Inflammation and reward motivation

There was no evidence that elevated systemic inflammation accentuated the effect of late-life insomnia on reward motivation in adjusted or unadjusted models (p’s > 0.7; Fig. 2). However, sex-specific analyses revealed a significant three-way interaction (b = 1.091, SE = 0.214, p < 0.001; 95% CI [0.672, 1.551]), such that among males only, higher CRP was associated with lower monetary reward motivation for those with late-life insomnia relative to comparison controls. This association held in models adjusted for depressive and fatigue symptoms, task-specific variable, age, BMI, and ethnicity (b = 1.131, SE = 0.241, p < 0.001; 95% CI [0.658, 1.604]; Table S5). As shown in Fig. 2 and confirmed by follow-up within-group analyses, higher levels of CRP were associated with a reduction in monetary reward motivation for males with late-life insomnia (b = −0.150, SE = 0.041, p < 0.001) but not comparison control males (b = 0.024, SE = 0.017, p = 0.142), comparison control females (b = −0.027, SE = 0.018, p = 0.130), or females with late-life insomnia (b = 0.035, SE = 0.023, p = 0.118).

Fig. 2: Reward motivation in relation to late-life insomnia, sex, and systemic inflammation plotted with 95% confidence intervals and adjusted for participant and task characteristics.
figure 2

CRP is not related to reward motivation among older adults with late-life insomnia (n = 31; 21 female) or comparison controls across sexes (n = 73; 31 female) (p = 0.614; (a)) but there were sex-specific effects (p < 0.001). Among males with late-life insomnia, higher levels of CRP are associated with lower reward motivation (p < 0.001; (b)). CRP was not significantly associated with reward motivation for females with or without late-life insomnia (p’s > 0.1; (c)).

Reward sensitivity

Late-life insomnia was also hypothesized to be associated with reduced sensitivity to reward. Consistent with hypotheses, those with late-life insomnia showed an attenuated association between increases in potential monetary reward and increases in hard trial choice, as compared to comparison controls (b = −0.265, SE = 0.087, p = 0.002; 95% CI [−0.436, −0.094]). Results remained significant in an adjusted model (95% CI [−0.430, −0.075]; Table 2). Follow-up within-group analyses confirmed that while increases in reward magnitude were associated with increases in hard trial choice for both those with (b = 0.079, SE = 0.016, p < 0.001) and without (b = 0.128, SE = 0.010, p < 0.001) late-life insomnia, this association was significantly attenuated in those with late-life insomnia as compared to comparison controls, X2(1) = 6.72, p = 0.010 (Fig. 3). There was no evidence that higher insomnia symptom severity across the sample was associated with reduced reward sensitivity (p > 0.4).

Fig. 3: Reward sensitivity in relation to late-life insomnia and sex plotted with 95% confidence intervals and adjusted for participant and task characteristics.
figure 3

Older adults with late-life insomnia (n = 31; 21 female) show reduced reward sensitivity relative to comparison controls (n = 73; 31 female) (p = 0.005; (a)) with no evidence for sex-specific effects (p = 0.063; (b, c)).

Sex differences and reward sensitivity

Sex was associated with reward sensitivity, as represented by a significant interaction between sex and reward magnitude in the prediction of hard trial choice in unadjusted (b = −0.227, SE = 0.083, p = 0.006; 95% CI [−0.389, −0.064]) and adjusted (b = −0.226, SE = 0.086, p = 0.008; 95% CI [−0.394, −0.058]; Table S6) models. Follow-up within-group analyses found that while increases in reward magnitude were significantly associated with increases in hard trial choice for both male (b = 0.132; SE = 0.012, p < 0.001) and female (b = 0.094; SE = 0.013, p < 0.001) older adults, this effect was significantly stronger for males X2(1) = 5.07, p = 0.024. Thus, male sex in older adults is associated with higher sensitivity to monetary reward on the EEfRT. There was no evidence that this sex difference varied as a function of insomnia, as tested by a three-way interaction term (group by reward magnitude by sex) (p = 0.063; 95% CI [−0.743, 0.019]; Table 2, Fig. 3).

Inflammation and reward sensitivity

There was no evidence that elevated inflammation moderated the effect of late-life insomnia on reward sensitivity (b = 0.206, SE = 0.121, p = 0.089; 95% CI [−0.032, 0.444]), nor was there evidence for further moderation by sex (b = −0.456, SE = 0.263, p = 0.084; 95% CI [−0.972, 0.061]) in adjusted models.

Discussion

Late-life insomnia is associated with reduced motivation and reduced sensitivity for monetary reward. As compared to community dwelling older adults without insomnia, those with late-life insomnia were less willing to exert effort to attain a higher monetary reward. Those with late-life insomnia were also less behaviorally responsive to increases in monetary reward, indicating a reduction in sensitivity to increases in reward magnitude. These effects were not attributable to group differences in depressive symptoms or fatigue, which underscores the importance of the reward system as a potential clinical target in efforts to decrease risk for late-life depression, particularly in the context of other risk factors such as insomnia. Moreover, results provide preliminary evidence that males may be more vulnerable to insomnia and inflammation-associated reductions in motivation for monetary reward.

There is an emerging literature linking sleep disturbance to dysregulated reward processing. One study found that adults with sleep complaints exhibited lower “wanting” for and “liking” of positive daily experiences than comparison controls [18], which parallels the insomnia-associated reductions in motivation and sensitivity observed in the current study. Consistent with these behavioral effects, structural and functional alterations in reward-related brain regions have been observed in adult insomnia patients relative to comparison controls [20, 21]. In experimental studies, sleep deprivation alters reactivity in reward neurocircuitry in response to rewarding stimuli [34, 35], suggesting a causal role for sleep loss in reward dysregulation. Several studies have shown associations between sleep disturbance in adolescence and alterations in neural reactivity to reward or functional connectivity in corticolimbic reward circuitry [36, 37]. Thus, although small, the literature converges in demonstrating an interconnection of the sleep and reward systems. The present study extends these prior findings and demonstrates an association between late-life insomnia and reward processing, using a validated assessment of multiple reward processes on a behavioral level.

Several lines of evidence support a role for inflammation in the association between late-life insomnia and reward processing. Treatment of insomnia has been shown to reduce systemic, cellular, or genomic markers of inflammation [38] and elevated inflammation can interfere with dopamine synthesis and function [24] and has been correlated with alterations in behavioral reward tasks [23] and reward neurocircuitry [39]. In the current study CRP was elevated in older adults with late-life insomnia, and among males elevated CRP accentuated the effect of late-life insomnia on reward motivation. This finding is consistent with other work demonstrating sex-specific associations between inflammation and depression or depressive phenotypes [40, 41], but additional study is required to ascertain the determinants of the presence and direction of these associations.

Prior research has also shown sex differences in reward processing, with adult females exhibiting reduced motivation for monetary reward relative to adult males [28, 31]. The current study suggests this pattern remains evident in older adults, despite the array of neurobiological and socioemotional changes that accompany aging [7, 14, 15, 42, 43]. However, in the presence of late-life insomnia, this pattern was reversed; males showed a heightened vulnerability to dysregulation of monetary reward processes, in which late-life insomnia conferred greater reductions in reward motivation in males relative to females. The fact that insomnia reverses sex difference in monetary reward processing is striking, especially given evidence of the male sex advantage in motivational processing in the older adult comparison controls. This enhanced vulnerability for males with insomnia was most marked in the context of elevated inflammation, an unexpected finding that is nevertheless consistent with large scale epidemiological studies linking elevated inflammation to depressive symptoms in males [41]. If corroborated by future studies, this finding would have clinical implications for precision medicine in psychiatry; specifically, older males with insomnia and elevated inflammation may preferentially benefit from interventions targeted at remediating reductions in reward motivation.

While speculative, the heightened salience of monetary reward for males in the current study may also be linked to key developmental transitions in aging. Retirement can increase risk for depression [44], with older males at elevated risk [45], possibly due to new challenges to self-worth, self-efficacy, and productivity that can be intertwined with monetary reward and perceived masculinity. Indeed, financial and occupational stressors have been shown to predict depression in males, whereas interpersonal social stressors predict depression in females [46]. Addressing these contextual socioemotional and environmental factors will be important in developing treatments to address reward dysregulation. Furthermore, the extent to which insomnia alters response to social rewards, and how this may vary by sex, requires further examination. Most notably, there is evidence for greater sensitivity to social reward in females as compared to males [47].

These results should be interpreted in the context of several limitations. As a cross-sectional observational study, no causal conclusions can be made. While validated and widely used, the EEfRT does not assess affective response to reward and uses monetary reward only; a fine-grained interrogation of the multifaceted reward system using multiple tasks and social rewards will enrich future studies. The EEfRT is also a complex decision-making task, and aging is most consistently associated with impaired reward processing for tasks requiring integration of reward information [48] rather than disruption of more basic reward function [49]. Whether insomnia disrupts basic reward function in older adults requires examination with less cognitively burdensome tasks. While the study had adequate statistical power to answer the primary study aims, secondary analyses on sex differences and CRP in relation to insomnia and reward sensitivity were exploratory, as they were not adequately powered. Moreover, the sample size for males with insomnia was small, and moderation analyses focusing on sex differences and CRP in relation to reward motivation should be interpreted with caution and require replication. Finally, although plasma CRP is a common indicator of systemic inflammation that predicts health outcomes and has been shown to correlate with other markers of inflammation, including inflammatory cytokines in both plasma and cerebrospinal fluid [50], assessment of a broader array of inflammatory biomarkers would provide a more comprehensive picture of immune system status.

Insomnia is prevalent in older adults and a well-established risk factor for depression. Results from the current study suggest that dysregulation in the reward system may be one mechanism linking insomnia to late-life depression; if confirmed by future studies, this suggests that interventions that either target insomnia and/or deficits in reward processing may mitigate the risk of depression in nondepressed older adults, especially males with insomnia. Findings also indicate that late-life insomnia is associated with both reduced motivation and reduced reward sensitivity, which has further implications for treatment given that these constructs are phenomenologically and biologically distinguishable. An important topic moving forward is to identify whether males with elevated inflammation are especially vulnerable to insomnia-induced deficits in reward function, as our exploratory findings suggest. Ultimately, characterizing these mechanistic pathways is a critical step toward refined prevention and treatment of depression, particularly given that insomnia, elevated inflammation, and reward deficits are all modifiable risk factors that can be specifically targeted for intervention.

Funding and disclosure

The authors declare no competing interests. This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health, USA (R01AG051944, R01AG056424-01, R01AG026364, R01CA160245, R01AG057750, and R01CA207130), the Norman Cousins Center for Psychoneuroimmunology (MRI), UCLA Clinical and Translational Science Institute (UL1TR001881), and Max Kade Foundation.