Introduction

Methylphenidate (MPH) is a psychostimulant medication widely used to treat attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), a mental health disorder characterised by behavioural symptoms including impulsiveness, hyperactivity and inattention [1, 2]. In a recent network meta-analysis, MPH, as compared to placebo, was shown to decrease the core ADHD symptoms in children, adolescents and adults with a moderate effect size between 0.49–0.78 [3]. Though MPH has been used for more than half a century, we still lack a clear understanding of the exact neurochemical mechanisms through which it exerts its clinical effects [2, 4, 5].

MPH has a dual pharmacological profile inhibiting the reuptake of both dopamine (DA) and noradrenaline (NE) by blocking their respective transporters (DAT and NET, respectively) [6,7,8]. This consequently increases the bioavailability of synaptic DA and NE [9, 10]. MPH has also a weak affinity for the 5-HT1A receptor, where in vitro studies have suggested that it acts as a partial agonist [8]; however, to date there has been no clear evidence of target engagement in vivo. On the other hand, effects on DAT and NET have been well documented in a rich diversity of in vitro, animal and human studies [6]. Despite this, it remains unclear whether the functional effects of MPH are best understood through DAT or NET binding (for a detailed review please see [6]). Studies evaluating in vitro activity for both these neurochemical transporters seem to agree that the racemic mixture dl-MPH typically used in clinical formulations has higher affinity and uptake inhibition activity on the DAT than the NET [6]. In vivo human positron emission tomography (PET) studies have also shown a clear accumulation of MPH in the basal ganglia and binding to DAT (ED50 = 0.25 mg/kg) [11, 12]. However, in the living human brain MPH has also been shown to bind to NET (ED50 = 0.14 mg/kg) with a higher affinity than to DAT [13, 14].

A similar issue relates to effects of MPH on human cognition and brain function. Again, it is unclear whether effects mostly relate to binding to DAT, NET or both systems, and whether effects on different regional circuits are related to the engagement of these two targets, given the known differences in their distribution densities [15,16,17,18,19]. Several studies have combined acute administration of MPH with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in both healthy and clinical populations to try and unravel potential effects on brain function and gain a clearer understanding of its effects on behaviour [2, 4]. These studies have employed both resting state designs and task-based approaches that tap into specific cognitive constructs. Pharmacological resting state fMRI (rs-fMRI) allows for the evaluation of basic pharmacodynamical effects unconstrained by the nature of any task and have been widely used [20, 21]. Studies in healthy individuals have shown that acute administration of clinically relevant MPH doses induce measurable and meaningful changes in functional connectivity (FC). For example, one study found an MPH-associated increase in intrinsic connectivity between brain areas involved in sustained attention [22]. Another reported that MPH enhanced resting state FC of the striatum/thalamus with primary motor cortex and increased negative FC with frontal executive regions [23]. In another, MPH was shown to reduce the coupling within visual and somatomotor networks and increase the competitive decoupling between the default mode and task positive networks [24]. However, given that the blood-oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal underpinning fMRI studies has no intrinsic selectivity to any particular neurochemical target [25, 26], it is still unclear which MPH pharmacological targets underpin observed changes in brain FC.

To bridge this gap, we have previously developed a novel multimodal method (Receptor-Enriched Analysis of functional Connectivity by Targets—REACT) which enriches rs-fMRI analyses with information about the distribution density of molecular targets derived from PET imaging. Further, we have shown that the functional effects of 3,4-Methyl-enedioxy-methamphetamine can be understood through the distribution of its main serotonergic targets [27]. Here, we applied REACT to multi-echo rs-fMRI data acquired in a cohort of healthy participants after an acute challenge of MPH to investigate how drug-related changes in resting state FC relate to the distribution of DAT and NET. Given the considerable affinity of MPH to both transporters [2], we hypothesised that FC informed by these two targets would be sensitive to MPH effects.

We also performed an exploratory analysis to test whether MPH-induced changes in the DAT- and NET-enriched FC can be linked to inter-individual differences in behavioural responses on a reinforcement-learning task with known sensitivity to MPH (results for the main effects of this task are reported elsewhere [28]). Multiple fMRI studies have shown that inter-individual differences in behavioural responses to a task can be related to individual differences in spontaneous cortical activity at rest [29, 30]. Our hypothesis is that by using the functional maps related to the MPH targets, we can link inter-individual variability in behavioural response to the task with the resting state FC and tease apart the variability associated with DAT and NET.

Methods

Participants and study design

In this work, we used the resting state fMRI dataset from a larger study on ADHD, of which the task-based fMRI data have already been published elsewhere [28]. We included data from 30 healthy controls (HC, 33 ± 9.5 years, M/F: 19/11). Participants were recruited using on-line classified advertising websites and university mailing lists. Local and national ethical approvals were obtained from Brighton and Sussex Medical School (14/014/HAR; 12/131/HAR) and the East of England (Hertfordshire) National Research Ethics Committee (reference: 12/EE/0256). All participants provided written informed consent. We excluded all potential participants if they had any self-reported psychiatric disorders, substance abuse, history of serious cardiovascular condition, current or recent use of monoamine oxidase inhibitors, coumarin anticoagulants, anticonvulsants or antipsychotics, if they had a diagnosis of glaucoma or were pregnant. Upon enrolment to the study, the Beck's Depression Inventory (BDI) [31], the State and Trait Anxiety Inventory [32] and the Conners’ Adult ADHD Rating scale [33] were used to assess depression and anxiety scores and ADHD symptoms, respectively. Additionally, all participants were within a healthy range for blood pressure.

We followed a randomised, within-subjects, double-blind, placebo-controlled design where participants received 20 mg of oral MPH or placebo in two separate sessions, spaced by a minimum of 1 week. This dose of MPH is at the lower end of the clinical dose and allows us to evaluate functional effects in the brain within a clinically relevant dose while keeping potential side effects at minimum. All participants were tested at the same time of day for both the MPH and placebo session, to minimise potential circadian variability in resting brain activity [34].

We excluded one subject because of excessive head movement during the placebo session (mean relative displacement > 0.2 mm).

Image acquisition

Ninety minutes after drug dosing, participants completed an MRI session, to coincide with peak effects of MPH on DAT occupancy [12]. The session included a 9-min multi-echo rs-fMRI scan. The rs-fMRI scan was used for the analysis presented here. Prior to the resting state scan were three runs of a reinforcement-learning task encompassing a novelty manipulation [35, 36], which is reported in detail elsewhere [28]. For the purpose of this work we use the behavioural outcome of the task independent of the fMRI data concurrently acquired.

Data were acquired using a 1.5 T Siemens Avanto MRI scanner (Siemens AG Medical Solutions, Erlangen, Germany) equipped with a 32-channel head-coil. Rs-fMRI data were obtained using a T2*-weighted multi-echo EPI sequence [37] (TR = 2570 ms; TEs = 15, 34, 54 ms; flip angle = 90°; resolution = 3.7 × 3.75, slice thickness = 4.49 mm; matrix size = 64 × 64; 31 axial slices; 200 volumes). A 3D T1-weighted anatomical scan was obtained for each participant in one session using an MP-RAGE acquisition (TR = 2730 ms, TE = 3.57 ms, TI = 1000 ms, flip angle = 7°, matrix = 256 × 240, number of partitions = 192, GRAPPA factor = 2, resolution = 1 mm3).

Image pre-processing

The rs-fMRI dataset was pre-processed using AFNI [38] and FMRIB Software Library (FSL). Pre-processing steps included volume re-alignment, time-series de-spiking and slice time correction. After the pre-processing, functional data were optimally combined (OC) by taking a weighted summation of the three echoes using an exponential T2* weighting approach [39]. The OC data were then de-noised with the Multi-Echo ICA approach implemented by the tool meica.py (Version v2.5) [40, 41], to remove motion artefacts and other non-BOLD sources of noise. This de-noising method has proved its greater effectiveness in reducing non-BOLD sources of noise and increasing the temporal signal-to-noise ratio when compared to other standard regression approaches [41,42,43]. White matter and cerebrospinal fluid signals were regressed out and a high-pass temporal filter with a cut-off frequency of 0.005 Hz was applied.

A study-specific template representing the average T1-weighted anatomical image across subjects was built using the Advanced Normalisation Tools (ANTs) [44]. Each participant’s dataset was co-registered to its corresponding structural scan, then normalised to the study-specific template before warping to standard MNI152 space. Images were finally resampled at 2 × 2 × 2 mm3 resolution.

Population-based molecular templates

For the analysis with REACT, we used molecular templates of the DAT and NET systems. The DAT map is a publicly available template of 123I-Ioflupane SPECT images (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/spmtemplates) from 30 HC without evidence of nigrostriatal degeneration [45]. The NET atlas was obtained by averaging the [11C]MRB PET brain parametric maps from an independent dataset of ten HC (33.3 ± 10 years, four women). More details on the processing of the PET data and regional distribution of [11C]MRB binding in the human brain can be found in [46]. These two atlases were already standardised to MNI space with 2 mm3 resolution. They were further normalised to scale the image values between 0 and 1, while preserving the original intensity distribution of the images, and masked using a standard grey matter (GM) mask. This mask comes from a probabilistic GM map available in FSL, which we thresholded in order to retain only the voxels with >30% probability of being GM, and binarized. Of note, occipital areas (defined using the Harvard Oxford Atlas) were masked out of both molecular atlases as they were used as reference regions for quantification of the molecular data in the kinetic models for the radioligands.

Functional connectivity analysis with the REACT method

The functional circuits related to the DAT and the NET systems were estimated with REACT using a two-step multivariate regression analysis [47, 48] implemented in FSL (fsl_glm command). This analysis is conceptually comparable to a dual-regression, often used in rs-fMRI studies to investigate the FC of the resting state networks. The main difference with this standard approach is that, with REACT, molecular templates are used in place of the resting state networks as a set of spatial regressors in the first multivariate regression analysis. The DAT and NET templates are entered into the first step of this analysis to calculate the dominant BOLD fluctuation within these maps [49]. Both rs-fMRI data and the design matrix were demeaned (–demean option). The rs-fMRI volumes were masked using a binarized atlas derived from the molecular data to restrict the analysis to the voxels for which the transporter density information was available in the template. The subject-specific time series estimated in this first step were then used as temporal regressors in a second multivariate regression analysis to estimate the subject-specific spatial maps of the BOLD response after MPH and placebo. At this stage, the analysis was conducted on the whole grey matter volume. Both data and the design matrix were demeaned (–demean option); the design matrix columns were also normalised to unit standard deviation with the –des_norm option [47]. The general framework of this analysis has been reported elsewhere [27].

Statistical analysis

In order to test our main hypothesis, we ran a paired-sample t-test to compare the subject-specific target-enriched spatial maps of the two drug conditions (MPH and placebo). We applied cluster-based inference within Randomise [50], using 5000 permutations per test and contrast. A cluster was considered significant if pFWE < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons using the threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) option [51], Bonferroni-corrected for multiple comparisons across maps (DAT and NET) and contrasts (MPH > placebo, placebo > MPH).

We also conducted some further post-hoc analyses to explore whether there were any areas of the brain where the DAT and NET-enriched FC maps were related to some of the key-behavioural responses elicited during the task the subjects performed immediately before the resting state scan (a detailed description of these behavioural responses can be found in [28]; here we summarise only those we used in our analyses). For this, we conducted a set of linear regression analyses between the target-enriched FC maps (MPH minus placebo) and four behavioural scores: (1) overall task performance (total £ won during the task); (2) reward-learning rate (a mathematical quantity resulting from fitting of a reinforcement learning model [36] to participants’ behavioural responses, which captures the extent to which choice is updated by recently gathered evidence; a higher score reflects faster updating of reward values with recent experience); (3) persistence in selecting novel options after their first appearance; (4) persistence in selecting novel rather than familiar stimuli after their first appearance (these last two behavioural measures capture novelty preference). For each regression analysis, we used the difference between scores on active drug and placebo (MPH minus Placebo). These tests were also performed with Randomise using 5000 permutations per test and contrast. For the significant correlations, we then extracted the mean FC value (MPH minus Placebo) from the clusters we found to be significantly associated with the behavioural responses in order to estimate the Pearson correlation coefficient using SPSS. Please note, these values are simply presented as a measure of effect size. We did not conduct statistical inference as such analysis would be circular.

Results

In Fig. 1, we show the templates of the molecular density distribution of the DAT (left panel, top row) and NET maps (right panel, top row) that we used in the REACT analysis. We also show the weighted-maps of their respective functional-associated circuits for each drug condition, averaged across participants.

Fig. 1: Maps of the molecular templates of the dopamine and noradrenaline transporters (DAT and NET) and their respective target-enriched fMRI maps.
figure 1

The fMRI maps are averaged across subjects for the placebo and methylphenidate (MPH) conditions. The occipital areas were masked out for both molecular atlases as they were used as reference regions for the quantification of the molecular data in the kinetic models for the radioligands. For more details on the molecular templates, please refer to [45, 46].

We found a significant increase in FC (pFWE < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons at the cluster level using the TFCE option, Bonferroni-corrected for multiple comparisons across maps and contrasts) after MPH in the maps enriched by the DAT distribution (Fig. 2). Specifically, this effect mainly involved sensorimotor areas including the precentral and postcentral gyri and the anterior division of the supramarginal gyrus. No changes between MPH and placebo were found in the NET-enriched FC maps.

Fig. 2: Functional connectivity (FC) changes after MPH compared to placebo in the DAT-enriched maps.
figure 2

The MPH-induced FC increase is localised in the precentral and postcentral gyri and the anterior division of the supramarginal gyrus.

Relationship between drug-induced functional changes at rest and behavioural responses during the novelty reinforcement learning task

We found significant negative correlations between persistence in selecting novel options after their first appearance (MPH minus placebo) and the MPH-induced functional changes in the DAT and the NET-enriched FC maps (MPH minus placebo). Specifically, we found a negative correlation between DAT-enriched FC and this behavioural score in the cerebellum, i.e. the right crus I and II, the right VI and VIIb and the vermis crus II (r = −0.739, 95% CI = −0.865 to −0.542; Fig. 3a). For the NET-enriched FC, this correlation mainly involved the precentral gyrus, the posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus (r = −0.687, 95% CI = −0.831 to −0.488; Fig. 3a).

Fig. 3: Correlations between behavioural response and functional connectivity (FC).
figure 3

a Persistence in selecting novel rather than familiar stimuli after their first appearance (MPH minus placebo) is inversely correlated with FC changes (MPH minus placebo) in the DAT-enriched and NET-enriched FC maps. b Task performance (i.e. £ won) (MPH minus placebo) is positively correlated with functional increases in the NET-enriched FC maps.

We also found a positive association between FC changes induced by MPH in the NET-enriched maps and task performance (£ won) in the lateral occipital cortex, precuneus and superior parietal lobule (r = 0.753, 95% CI = 0.590–0.857; Fig. 3b).

We did not find any other significant correlation for any of the remaining behavioural measures tested.

Discussion

In this study, we applied our recently developed multimodal method for FC analysis informed by molecular targets (REACT) to investigate how drug-related changes in resting FC after a single administration of MPH in healthy individuals relate to the distribution of its main targets, i.e. the DAT and NET. In line with our main hypothesis, we found that MPH changed connectivity within the functional network related to the DAT. However, we did not find any significant drug-effects related to the NET distribution at the robust statistical thresholds reported here. Furthermore, we found that different regional changes in the DAT and NET-enriched FC maps can significantly explain some of the inter-individual variance in participants’ behaviour during a novelty reinforcement learning task engaging both systems [52]. Moreover, we have extended our method validation pipeline by showing that REACT is able to capture meaningful changes in the target-enriched functional circuits beyond those related to the serotonin system, which we reported in our first proof-of-concept work [27].

In the main analysis, we were able to show that MPH effects on resting FC co-vary with the known distribution of one of its main targets, the DAT. We found FC increases within the DAT-related network mapping somatomotor areas, such as the precentral and postcentral gyri and the anterior division of the supramarginal gyrus. This overlaps with regions where regional cerebral blood flow measured at rest is modulated by a single dose of MPH as part of a pattern which also includes the caudate nucleus, thalamus and mid-brain, as reported in [53], all areas enriched in DAT. Our findings are also consistent with previous observations in healthy individuals that MPH increases the FC between the striatum (a brain region highly enriched in DAT) and the somatomotor cortex [23]. Our results also mirror previous studies where levodopa and haloperidol (pro- and anti-dopaminergic drugs, respectively) were found to modulate the resting state and task-related FC between the motor cortex and the striatum in healthy participants [54, 55], and another study where acute phenylalanine and tyrosine depletion method used to decrease DA synthesis decreased FC within the somatomotor network [56]. While our data was acquired at rest, and therefore making inferences about the potential behavioural implications of such findings is inherently speculative, we note that the FC increase in the somatomotor cortex is in line with some reported effects of MPH and other catecholaminergic agents on motor performance [57]. For instance, MPH has been shown to improve motor functions in clinical conditions encompassing motor deficits. In fact, a single dose of MPH has been shown to improve motor coordination in children with developmental coordination disorder and ADHD [58], and low doses of this drug have been reported to improve gait and voluntary movement in patients with Parkinson disease [59]. While the implications of these findings for the improvements in motor coordination observed in patients with ADHD after MPH are relatively intuitive to extrapolate, their contribution to better understanding the effects of MPH on other symptoms-domains, such as impulsivity or inattention, is less straightforward.

Recent computational modelling approaches attempting to establish a single parsimonious model to explain all of the core symptomatology of ADHD have suggested that signal loss due to low neural gain in the corticostriatal loops might be at the heart of the behavioural manifestations of this disorder [60]. Here, neural gain is a process through which the brain amplifies incoming signals according to their momentary importance [61]. In high neural gain states, neural populations amplify strong signals and attenuate weaker ones, leading to neural representations that are less susceptible to noise and serve to stabilise behaviour. In contrast, low neural gain states are associated with behavioural instability [60]. Conceptually, inattention can be conceptualised as a frequent shift between different competing goals and an inability to focus and stay with the currently most valuable option. Likewise, frequent switches between cognitive goals might propagate through the motor system and lead to frequent changes in motor programs, potentially explaining hyperactivity.

Therefore, impaired neural gain in corticostriatal loops during action selection has the potential to explain the instable behavioural patterns underlying both inattention and hyperactivity in patients with ADHD [60]. A key modulator of neural gain within these corticostriatal loops is DA, as it modulates the strength of information conveyed from the cortex (including the sensorimotor cortex), via the striatum, through the internal pathways of the basal ganglia [61]. Under low DA, signal differentiation between the direct and indirect pathways of the loops are comparable in strength, resulting in signal interference and behavioural instability. Following this concept, decreases in DA could account for the behavioural instability observed in ADHD [60]. Indeed, this DA decrease has been shown in PET studies [62].

Conversely, increases in DA strengthen signal differentiation in the direct pathway, suppressing noise, stabilising neural systems and behavioural patterns. Therefore, it is possible that the increased DAT-related connectivity in the somatomotor cortex that we report here reflects MPH-induced strengthening of neural gain in the direct pathway through a tonic increase in DA. If that is true, our findings in healthy individuals might reflect a basic pharmacological mechanism through which MPH can improve motor coordination, inattention and hyperactivity in patients with ADHD by modulating DAergic transmission. However, we must acknowledge that for now this hypothesis remains speculative and will need to be addressed in future empirical studies.

The lack of findings for the NET-enriched functional circuit is surprising for a number of reasons. First, human PET studies have shown that at clinical doses (such as those used here), MPH binds to the NET with a higher affinity than to the DAT [13, 14]. Second, there is further evidence from studies in experimental animal showing that MPH can directly affect the discharge properties of the locus coeruleus (LC) [63], which is the main source of noradrenergic innervation in the brain. These findings are also supported by a rs-fMRI study in humans showing that MPH exerts effects on the FC of the LC [64]. Third, there is a general consensus that the MPH pro-cognitive effects in ADHD patients and HC involve modulation of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) [65]. While both DA and NA have critical influence on PFC cognitive functioning [65, 66], there are relatively low levels of DAT in the PFC [67], supporting the hypothesis that MPH and other psychomotor stimulants effects in this area may involve NET inhibition [68]. However, a number of reasons can be advanced to explain this lack of findings in the NET-related FC. First, there may be a limitation in our technique. We use the available binding sites for NET, but PET occupancy studies suggest a regional variation in the MPH occupancy of NET [13] which would weigh more towards the effects at the LC which may be poorly represented in the MRI maps acquired at 1.5 or 3 T [69]. A general limitation that we should also consider is that our subjects performed a reinforcement learning task—which is known to elicit a dopaminergic response [70]—immediately before the rs-fMRI scan, and that this may have induced some carry-over effects biasing FC towards the DAT-related network. Furthermore, it is also important to point out that the reuptake of dopamine and noradrenaline is complex, as both transporters can participate in the reuptake of both DA and NA [71, 72]. This implies that MPH could still affect noradrenergic transmission through DAT inhibition. Moreover, we cannot exclude that some NET-related FC changes that do not reach statistical significance at rest may emerge during a paradigm preferentially engaging attentional networks [73, 74]. Finally, we acquired data on a single time-interval post-dosing with a single low dose. PET occupancy of 10 mg and 40 mg MPH at NET assessed in vivo using [11C]MRB indicates that this dose is sufficient to produce a significant NET occupancy with robust peak effects between 75 min and at least 3 h post-dosing [14].

Although MPH did not elicit robust behavioural responses during the novelty-reinforcement learning task in HC (data published elsewhere [28]), our exploratory analysis highlighted that regional differences between MPH and placebo on DAT- and NET-enriched FC are significantly correlated with inter-individual differences in the MPH-induced changes in key-behavioural responses associated with this task. Indeed, our findings show that despite the relatively low density of DAT in the cerebellum, the DAT-enriched connectivity in this same area was related to performance. This may be related to the strong anatomical connectivity between the cerebellum and the DAT-enriched basal ganglia [75], or it may relate to direct effects within the cerebellum. Despite the low detection of DAT availability in vivo in humans, DAT is present in the cerebellum [76] and our findings match current models suggesting a role of the cerebellum in error/novelty detection [77,78,79].

While we did not find a treatment effect of MPH in the NET-related FC, we did find a positive association between FC changes induced by MPH in the NET-enriched maps and overall task performance (£ won) in the lateral occipital cortex, precuneus and superior parietal lobule. We also found negative correlations between persistence in selecting novel rather than familiar options and NET-enriched FC changes induced by MPH mainly in the precentral gyrus, the posterior cingulate cortex and the precuneus. Consistent with our findings, these areas have previously been shown to be implicated in novelty processing [80,81,82]. Animal studies have shown that the occipital and parietal cortices receive a dense noradrenergic innervation and that pharmacological NET blockade increases extracellular NE (but not DA) in these areas [83, 84]. Furthermore, a resting state fMRI study in healthy subjects reported a FC decrease induced by atomoxetine, i.e. a relatively selective NET blocker, predominantly in the posterior brain regions including the visual system [85]. Another fMRI study using a n-back task in healthy volunteers reported atomoxetine-induced FC changes in the frontoparietal network, including areas such as the precentral gyrus and the precuneus, during working-memory processing [86].

Although preliminary, our findings suggest that REACT may hold the potential to tease apart regional functional effects related to different molecular targets underlying the same cognitive/behavioural downstream effect when compounds like MPH binding with considerable affinity to more than one molecular target are used. However, since we relied on correlations between resting state FC and behavioural measures acquired at a different time point, this hypothesis would benefit from further development of REACT to accommodate task-based designs.

Our study has some limitations to acknowledge. First, we did not collect blood samples and assay plasma levels of MPH to control for individual differences in drug exposure or to explore pharmacokinetic/pharmacodynamic relationships. Second, while the original study also included patients with ADHD [28], we only report data from the healthy control group, where all participants received the same drug at the same dose; therefore, the implications of our findings should not be generalised to clinical populations. However, we decided to not include the ADHD dataset because all patients were on chronic stimulant treatment, which is known to produce long-lasting changes in baseline DAT bioavailability [87, 88]. Furthermore, ADHD patients were tested under their routine treatment, which was heterogeneous (i.e. either MPH or dexamphetamine). These two factors would have made interpretation of potential findings challenging. Third, stimulants can potentially influence fMRI BOLD signal, which depends on the haemodynamic coupling of neuronal activities and local changes in blood flow and oxygenation [25]. However, previous work has found that while stimulants can decrease cortical blood flow, they do not obscure BOLD signals or disrupt neurovascular coupling during resting brain activity [89]. Finally, we relied on group-based molecular templates estimated in two independent cohorts of healthy individuals. Therefore, further specification from intra-regional variation across subjects is not possible using the current dataset as it would require PET data for each ligand and participant. Nevertheless, it would be interesting to test whether subject-specific receptor density maps would provide additional information. This last aspect may be critical in moving forward to examine MPH-induced FC changes in clinical populations (i.e. ADHD), who may have alterations in the distribution of DAT [88] and NET [90].

Conclusion

Using our recently developed multimodal method for FC analysis informed by molecular targets (REACT), we show that meaningful MPH effects on human brain resting FC can be understood through the distribution of, at least, one of its main targets, i.e. DAT. We also provide evidence to support the idea that this method may be able to capture concomitant differential regional functional effects related to different targets underlying the same cognitive/behavioural effect, when compounds have mixed pharmacological profiles. By defining the target-specific topography of the functional effects of pharmacological compounds in the human brain, our method holds the potential to advance our understanding of the mechanisms of action of many drugs for which target affinity is relatively well characterised but system-level brain pharmacodynamic models related to their targets are missing.