Introduction

Addiction costs the US over $600 billion per year [1] and FDA-approved treatments are limited. Current treatments ignore the co-morbidity between different addictions [2, 3] and their shared neurobiology [4], and do not address the underlying causes of the disorder [5, 6]. Identifying the core neurochemical processes underlying these behaviors could advance the search for novel therapeutics to treat various addictions.

Dopamine (DA) neurotransmission is critical to the reinforcing effects of psychostimulant drugs [7], palatable, high caloric foods [8, 9], and non-drug reinforcers like sex [10, 11] and gambling [12]. Attempts to leverage DA drugs for treatment of addiction have yielded no medications that lack abuse liability [13, 14] and exhibit efficacy to reduce craving and prevent relapse.

The serotonin 2C receptor (5-HT2CR) is a promising therapeutic candidate for treatment of addiction [15, 16]. 5-HT2CRs are most highly expressed on GABAergic interneurons in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and striatal areas, including the ventral tegmental area (VTA), nucleus accumbens (NAcc), caudate and putamen [17, 18]. Activation of 5-HT2CRs should inhibit mesolimbic DA signaling, attenuating the abuse-related effects of reinforcers. Electrophysiological and microdialysis studies [16, 19,20,21], and receptor knockout studies [22], provide support for this hypothesis. Moreover, 5-HT2CR agonists decrease psychostimulant self-administration and reinstatement [21] in non-human primates (NHPs), food intake in humans [23, 24], and nicotine [25] and alcohol [26] self-administration in rats.

Perseverative behavior, which has been implicated in addiction, can be defined as a general inability to alter responding with changing reinforcement contingencies. It is measured using reversal learning tasks and often interpreted as a measure of compulsivity [27, 28]. A progression from recreational use of reinforcers to compulsive use is thought to underlie the development and maintenance of addiction [29,30,31]. Reinforcer-induced increases in DA signaling within the striatum could play a crucial role in decision-making and intake of reinforcers. For example, prenatal exposure to methamphetamine (METH) and methylphenidate in mice [32] and METH pretreatment in rats [33] increase perseverative behavior. The 5-HT system modulates perseverative behavior and the abuse-related effects of reinforcers, likely through its effects on DA neurotransmission, given that selective depletion of 5-HT in the orbital PFC (oPFC) increases perseverative responding in animal models [34, 35]. Studies addressing the effects of 5-HT2CRs on perseverative behavior in rodents have yielded mixed results. The 5-HT2CR agonists Ro 60-0175 [36] and CP 809.101 [37] decrease perseverative responding, whereas another study found that the 5-HT2C antagonist SB242084 has the same effect [38]. Thus far, there have been no studies on the effects of 5-HT2CR agonists or antagonists on perseverative behavior in NHPs, or on whether the 5-HT2CR is necessary for these effects.

The present study evaluated whether long-term high caloric diet (HCD) or methamphetamine (METH) intake increases perseverative behavior, and whether a 5-HT2CR agonist decreases response perseveration in female rhesus monkeys. The present study also determined whether 5-HT2CR activation decreases intake of a low caloric diet (LCD) and/or a choice diet, where both a LCD and a palatable HCD are available. To demonstrate that the 5-HT2CR is necessary for these effects, pretreatment with a selective 5-HT2C receptor antagonist was used.

Materials and methods

Subjects

Adult female rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) weighing 7–14 kg (N = 10) served as subjects. Menstrual cycle was not monitored. At the onset, subjects were experimentally naive, having never had any access to drugs of abuse or a palatable HCD. All procedures strictly followed the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (Eighth Edition, revised in 2010), and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Emory University.

Subjects in the METH group (N = 5) were pair-housed in indoor stainless-steel home cages at the Yerkes National Primate Research Center (YNPRC) Main Station. They were fed Purina monkey chow (Ralston Purina, St. Louis, MO, USA) supplemented with fruits and vegetables, and had water continually available in the colony, maintained at an ambient temperature of 22 ± 2 °C at 45–50% humidity and a 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at hour 0700; lights off at hour 1900). Environmental enrichment was provided on a regular rotating basis. Subjects were accessed for testing using a pole and collar (primate products) method and positioned into a primate chair (primate products) placed inside testing chambers.

Subjects in the HCD group (N = 5) were housed in indoor–outdoor 144 ft2 (12 × 12 ft.) pens at the YNPRC Field Station in groups of 5–6 females. Environmental enrichment was provided on a regular rotating basis. Socially housed female rhesus monkeys form a dominance hierarchy, with subordinates showing a distinct behavioral and physiological phenotype [39], thus only middle ranking animals were utilized. Subjects were accessed for testing using a transport cage (primate products) method and carried to testing chambers.

Drugs

The 5-HT2CR agonist WAY163909 hydrochloride [(7b– R,10a –R)-1,2,3,4,8,9,10,10a—octahydro-7bHcyclopenta[b][1,4] diazepino [6,7,1hi] indole] was a generous gift from Pfizer Inc. (New York, NY, USA). WAY163909 was dissolved in 10 mg/mL beta-cyclodextrin and administered intramuscularly 45 min prior to testing. WAY163909 is selective for the 5-HT2CR, having a much higher affinity for the 5-HT2CR (Ki = 10.5) than the 5-HT2AR (Ki = 212) or the 5-HT2BR (Ki = 2101) [40]. The selective 5-HT2C antagonist SB242084 [6-chloro-2,3-dihydro-5-methyl-N-[6-[(2-methyl-3-pyridinyl)oxy]-3-pyridinyl]-1H-indole-1-carboxyamide dihydrochloride hydrate] was purchased from Tocris Bioscience (Ellisville, MO, USA), dissolved in at a concentration of 1.0 mg/mL in a 20:20:60 mixture of 95% ethanol, Tween 80 (Sigma-Aldrich®, St. Louis, MO), and 0.9% saline, and further diluted to appropriate concentrations using 0.9% saline. SB242084 is also selective for the 5-HT2CR, having a much higher affinity for the 5-HT2CR (Ki = 7.1) than the 5-HT2AR (Ki = 160) or the 5-HT2BR (Ki = 100) [40]. SB242084 was administered intramuscularly 15 min prior to WAY163909. (±)METH hydrochloride was provided by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (Research Technology Branch, Research Triangle Park, NC, USA), dissolved in 0.9% saline, and administered intravenously.

Reversal learning

Subjects were trained on a discrimination reversal learning (DRL) task [41]. The Wisconsin General Test Apparatus (WGTA) was fitted with a stimulus tray containing three wells. DRL sessions were conducted once/day (Monday–Friday at the Yerkes Field Station and Monday–Sunday at the Yerkes Main Station). Each session consisted of 30 trials separated by 15 s intervals. Subjects had a 2-min limited hold to emit their response before the trial ended. The positions of objects during each trial were randomized.

During acquisition of the task, one of three distinct objects was paired with a hidden reward (M&Ms, Skittles, etc.). If the subject’s first response during a trial was the rewarded object, the trial would end, followed by a 15 s time-out, and the next trial. If the subject’s first response was one of the unrewarded objects, there would be a 15 s time-out, followed by a repetition of that trial with a forced correction in which the chosen incorrect object was moved to reveal the empty well underneath. Subjects could repeat each trial, making an unlimited number of erroneous responses until they chose the rewarded object, moving on to the next trial. Acquisition continued until the subject reached a performance criterion of 90% correct responses (the rewarded object was chosen first). After meeting these criteria, they underwent a reversal the following day.

During reversal, the reward was hidden under one of the previously unrewarded objects. Reversal lasted for one 30-trial session with no required performance criterion. Reversal trials were conducted identically to acquisition trials. Possible errors included choosing the previously rewarded object (perseverative error) or the third unrewarded (random error). The inclusion of forced correction trial repetitions resulted in a finite number of possible correct responses (30), but an unlimited number of possible perseverative errors and random errors.

Reversal learning was measured before (pre-exposure) and after intake of METH or the HCD for 6 months (post-HCD exposure or post-METH exposure). All pretreatments with vehicle, WAY163909, or SB242084 were given prior to reversal sessions. A new set of three objects was used for each subsequence round of acquisition and reversal.

5-HT2CR interventions

Following baseline reversal DRL task measures, during which no pretreatments were given, all subjects were evaluated after administration of WAY163909 or its vehicle. WAY163909 pretreatments (vehicle, 0.1, 0.3, and 1.0 mg/kg) were randomized across subjects in each group. Doses were chosen based on psychostimulant self-administration and reinstatement studies, and microdialysis studies on psychostimulant-induced DA overflow in the NAcc [21]. For one subject (Iv8) in the HCD group, the dose range was reduced because the highest dose of WAY163909 produced behavioral suppression during the task. She was evaluated under the following doses: 0.03, 0.1, and 0.3 mg/kg WAY163909. A second, final baseline measure was collected for all subjects following all other treatments. For all pre-exposure group DRL graphs presented, N = 10 for all experimental conditions, except the 1.0 mg/kg WAY163909 treatment condition, for which N = 9. For the HCD group, baseline food intake of a LCD only, during which no pretreatments were given, was measured. Then, LCD intake was evaluated after administration of WAY163909 or its vehicle.

After 6 months of METH SA or HCD consumption, subjects were re-tested on the DRL task after administration of WAY163909 (0.3 mg/kg for Iv8, 1.0 mg/kg for all others), its vehicle, or a combination of WAY163909 and SB242084 (0.1 mg/kg). For the HCD group, food intake was measured under no pretreatment conditions. Then, food intake was evaluated following administration of WAY163909 (0.3 mg/kg for Iv8, 1.0 mg/kg for all others), its vehicle, or a combination of WAY163909 and SB242084 (0.1 mg/kg). The dose of SB242084 used was chosen based on previous drug self-administration experiments in squirrel monkeys [19, 20]. All pretreatments were counterbalanced across subjects.

Intravenous drug self-administration

Following baseline reversal learning testing, subjects in the METH group self-administered METH (0.01 mg/kg/infusion i.v.) for a period of 6 months. The apparatus and self-administration (SA) procedure are described in detail by Howell and Wilcox [42]. Briefly, subjects responded under a fixed-ratio (FR) 20 schedule of drug delivery during 60-min daily sessions in the morning (starting between 0700 and 1000 h) Monday–Friday. The effects of WAY163909 on METH SA were not tested here because they have been previously described [21].

Food intake

Prior to and during pre-exposure reversal learning testing, subjects in the HCD group had ad libitum access to standard monkey chow (LCD; 3.45 kcal/g, Purina 5038) via previously validated automated feeders that allowed for constitutive quantification of caloric intake [43]. Briefly, activation of a radio-frequency (RF) antenna via RF identification chip within each subject’s wrist signaled a computer to dispense a single pellet of food. The caloric composition of the LCD was 12% fat, 18% protein, 4.14% sugar carbohydrate, and 65.9% fiber carbohydrate. Following pre-exposure reversal learning testing, subjects in the HCD group were given access to both the LCD and a HCD (4.47 kcal/g, D07091204S Research Diets, New Brunswick NJ), with a caloric composition of 36% fat, 18% protein, 16.4% sugar carbohydrate, and 29.9% fiber-starch carbohydrate, for 6 months. In monkeys, availability of diet choice sustains excess calorie intake and promotes obesity compared to availability of only a HCD [44, 45]. Measures of food intake were taken for 24 h following vehicle, WAY163909, or SB242084 + WAY163909 pretreatments.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted using GraphPad Prism (GraphPad Software, version 7). Paired t-tests and repeated measures analysis of variance (RM ANOVA) with Dunnett’s post hoc test were used when appropriate. Significance for all tests was set a priori at the 95% confidence level (p < 0.05).

Results

Reversal learning measures before METH or HCD exposure

Pre-exposure DRL task measures are reported as group mean ± SEM (Fig. 1). During reversal sessions, subjects (N = 10) made 13.5 ± 1.71 correct responses at baseline, when no pretreatments were given, and 11.2 ± 1.99 after vehicle pretreatment; 23.8 ± 3.09 perseverative errors at baseline and 26.6 ± 3.8 after vehicle pretreatment; 7.05 ± 1.27 random errors at baseline and 6.2 ± 1.24 after vehicle pretreatment. Subjects took 3.35 ± 0.35 days to acquire discriminations prior to baseline reversal, and 4.4 ± 1.01 days to acquire discriminations prior to vehicle pretreatment. RM ANOVA with Dunnett’s post hoc test revealed no significant difference between any baseline or vehicle DRL task measures (p < 0.05).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Effects of WAY163909 on reversal learning measures before METH or HCD exposure. Subjects participated in a DRL task in which several measures were taken during reversal sessions. Dose-effect curves for WAY163909 (0.1, 0.3, and 1.0 mg/kg) were established for the following measures: a correct responses, b perseverative errors, c random errors, and d acquisition latency (days). The data are presented as the group mean ± SEM, where N = 10 for each experimental condition, except for the 1.0 mg/kg WAY163909 dose condition, for which N = 9. **p < 0.01 for correct responses compared to vehicle; ****p < 0.0001 for correct responses and perseverative errors compared to vehicle

5-HT2CR effects on reversal learning measures before METH or HCD exposure

WAY163909 pretreatments dose-dependently increased pre-exposure correct responses [F(4,36) = 14.33, p < 0.0001] compared to vehicle (Fig. 1A). After pretreatment with the 0.1, 0.3, and 1.0 mg/kg WAY163909 doses, subjects made an average of 16.6 ± 1.48, 19.9 ± 2.23, and 20.9 ± 1.37 correct responses, respectively. WAY163909 pretreatments also significantly decreased pre-exposure perseverative errors [F(4,36) = 28.02, p < 0.0001] compared to vehicle (Fig. 1B). After pretreatment with the 0.1, 0.3, and 1.0 mg/kg WAY163909 doses, subjects made an average of 14.5 ± 2.23, 11.0 ± 2.49, and 8.3 ± 1.2 perseverative errors, respectively. Pre-exposure random errors (Fig. 1C) [F(4,36) = 1.295, p = 0.2903] and acquisition latencies (Fig. 1D) [F(4,36) = 1.161, p = 0.3443] were not affected by WAY163909 pretreatments.

5-HT2CR effects on food consumption before HCD exposure

At baseline, subjects in the HCD group only had access to a LCD. RM ANOVA revealed no difference in 24-h total food intake across any of the WAY163909 doses tested [F(4,16) = 0.3243, p = 0.8078] (data not shown).

Extended intake of the HCD or METH

Mean cumulative METH intake (Fig. 2A) over the 6-month period was 48.01 ± 9.335 mg/kg. Under pre-exposure conditions, subjects in the HCD group only had access to the LCD and they consumed an average of 1833.73 ± 223.90 kcal/week (Fig. 2B). During the 6-month choice diet condition (LCD + HCD), mean total food intake increased to 3060.65 ± 287.74 kcal/week [t(4) = 12.08, p = 0.0003] compared to the pre-exposure condition (Fig. 2B).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Reversal learning measures after 6-month METH or HCD exposure. Subjects in the METH group (N = 5) self-administered METH for 6 months. Data for total cumulative METH intake over this 6-month period a are presented as the 7-day mean ± SEM. Subjects in the HCD group (N = 5) consumed a HCD (under a dietary choice condition) for 6 months. The data for mean food intake b at baseline (LCD only) and during the 6-month choice diet condition (LCD + HCD), are presented as the mean ± SEM. DRL task measures were taken for the METH group. Data for correct responses c and perseverative errors e, under pre-exposure and post-METH exposure conditions, for the group as well as by individual, are presented as the mean ± SEM. DRL task measures were taken for the HCD group. Data for correct responses d and perseverative errors f, under pre-exposure and post-HCD exposure conditions, for the group as well as by individual, are presented as the mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05 for post-HCD and post-METH exposure perseverative errors compared with pre-exposure perseverative errors; ***p < 0.001 for post-HCD exposure mean food intake compared with pre-exposure mean food intake

Reversal learning measures after METH or HCD exposure

Subjects were re-tested on the DRL task after 6-months of either METH SA or HCD consumption. Compared to pre-exposure conditions, perseverative errors increased after either extended METH SA [t(4) = 3.493, p = 0.0125] (Fig. 2E) or HCD consumption [t(4) = 2.259, p = 0.0434] (Fig. 2F). Perseveration in the METH group doubled, from 23.3 ± 4.90 pre-exposure perseverative errors to 45.4 ± 4.80 perseverative errors following 6-months of METH SA. In the HCD group, perseveration increased from 24.3 ± 4.33 pre-exposure perseverative errors to 30 ± 5.82 perseverative errors after 6 months of HCD intake. Post-METH exposure (Fig. 2C), but not post-HCD exposure (Fig. 2D), correct responses trended toward a decrease compared to the pre-exposure condition, however this difference was not significant (p = 0.0611). Random errors and acquisition latency were not affected by extended intake of either the HCD or METH (data not shown).

5-HT2CR effects on reversal learning measures after METH or HCD exposure

Following 6 months of METH SA, the highest dose of WAY163909 (1.0 mg/kg) increased correct responses [F(2,8) = 30.46, p = 0.0002] (Fig. 3A) and decreased perseverative errors [F(2,8) = 50.22, p < 0.0001] (Fig. 2B) compared to vehicle. When a dose of the antagonist SB242084 (0.1 mg/kg) was given in combination with the high dose of WAY163909, these effects of WAY163909 were blocked. When normalized to vehicle performance (% of vehicle), there was no difference between the effects of 1.0 mg/kg WAY163909 on pre- vs post-METH exposure correct responses (Fig. 3C) or perseverative errors (Fig. 3D), but the combined 0.1 mg/kg SB242084 + 1.0 mg/kg WAY163909 pretreatment produced a much smaller increase in post-METH exposure correct responses [(F(2,8) = 21.89, p = 0.0006] and much small decrease in perseverative errors [(F(2,8) = 30.02, p = 0.0002], than the high WAY163909 dose alone. Random errors (data not shown) were not affected by the high dose of WAY163909 or the combined SB242084 + WAY163909 pretreatment [F(2,8) = 3.79, p = 0.0695] and there was no difference in acquisition latency (data not shown) for any of the pretreatments [F(2,8) = 2.646, p = 0.1312].

Fig. 3
figure 3

Effects of combined pretreatment with WAY163909 and SB242084 on reversal learning measures after METH exposure. Subjects in the METH group (N = 5) self-administered METH for 6 months. Afterward, post-METH exposure perseverative behavior was measured using a DRL task under the following experimental conditions: vehicle, 1.0 mg/kg WAY163909, and 1.0 mg/kg WAY163909 + 0.1 mg/kg SB242084. Effects of these pretreatments are shown for a correct responses and b perseverative errors. The effects of WAY163909 and SB242084 before and after METH exposure are shown for c correct responses (% of vehicle) and d perseverative errors (% of vehicle). The data are presented as the group mean ± SEM for each experimental condition. ***p < 0.001 for post-METH exposure correct responses compared with vehicle; ****p < 0.0001 for post-METH exposure perseverative errors compared with vehicle; bars with the same letter (a or b) are not statistically different, while bars with different letters denote statistical difference

Following 6-month access to the HCD, the high WAY163909 dose increased correct responses [F(2,8) = 43.89, p < 0.0001] (Fig. 4A) and decreased perseverative errors [F(2,8) = 15.15, p = 0.0019] (Fig. 4B) compared to vehicle. SB242084 (0.1 mg/kg) blocked these effects. When normalized to vehicle performance (% of vehicle), there was no difference between the effects of 1.0 mg/kg WAY163909 on pre- vs post-HCD exposure correct responses (Fig. 4C) or perseverative errors (Fig. 4D), but the combined 0.1 mg/kg SB242084 + 1.0 mg/kg WAY163909 pretreatment produced a much smaller increase in post-HCD exposure correct responses [(F(2,8) = 6.434, p = 0.0216] and much small decrease in perseverative errors [(F(2,8) = 10.15, p = 0.0064], than the high WAY163909 dose alone. Random errors (data not shown) were not affected by any of the pretreatments [F(2,8) = 3.253, p = 0.0925] and there was no difference in acquisition latency (data not shown) across these conditions [F(2,8) = 2.341, p = 0.1583].

Fig. 4
figure 4

Effects of combined pretreatment with WAY163909 and SB242084 on reversal learning measures after HCD exposure. Subjects in the HCD group (N = 5) were given access to both a LCD and a palatable HCD for 6 months. Post-HCD exposure perseverative behavior was measured under the following conditions: vehicle, high WAY163909, and high WAY163909 + 0.1 mg/kg SB242084. Effects of these pretreatments are shown for a correct responses and b perseverative errors. The effects of WAY163909 and SB242084 before and after HCD exposure are shown for c correct responses (% of vehicle) and d perseverative errors (% of vehicle). The data are presented as the group mean ± SEM for each experimental condition. **p < 0.01 for post-HCD exposure perseverative errors compared with vehicle; ****p < 0.0001 for post-HCD exposure correct responses compared with vehicle; bars with the same letter (a or b) are not statistically different, while bars with different letters denote statistical difference

5-HT2CR effects on food consumption after HCD exposure

RM ANOVA revealed no change in 24-h LCD calorie intake (Fig. 5A) across any of the pretreatments tested [F(4,16) = 0.8625, p = 0.5073]. However, WAY163909 did have an effect on HCD only intake (Fig. 5B), with the medium and high doses decreasing HCD only 24-h food intake [F(4,16) = 12.63, p < 0.0001] compared to vehicle. SB242084 (0.1 mg/kg) again blocked this effect. WAY163909 also had an effect on total food intake (Fig. 5C), with the medium and high WAY163909 doses decreasing total 24-h food intake [F(4,16) = 12.14, p < 0.0001] compared to vehicle.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Effects of combined pretreatment with WAY163909 and SB242084 on food intake after HCD exposure. Subjects in the HCD group (N = 5) were given access to both a LCD and a palatable HCD for 6 months. Immediately after the 6-month period, a dose-effect curve for WAY163909 (low, medium, and high doses) was established for the HCD group for 24-h a LCD only food intake, b HCD only food intake, and c total food intake. Intake was also measured when a high dose of WAY163909 + SB242084 dose (0.1 mg/kg) combination pretreatment was given. The data are presented as the group mean ± SEM for each experimental condition. *p < 0.05 for post-HCD exposure 24 h total food intake compared with vehicle; **p < 0.01 for post-HCD exposure 24 h HCD food intake compared with vehicle; ***p < 0.001 for post-HCD exposure 24 h HCD food intake compared to vehicle; ****p < 0.0001 for post-HCD exposure 24 h total food intake compared with vehicle

Discussion

The present study examined the effects of long-term HCD or METH intake and the 5-HT2CR on perseverative behavior and food intake. Extended METH or HCD intake increased perseveration in all subjects. WAY163909 decreased perseveration at baseline and following this extended period of HCD or METH intake. WAY163909 also reduced HCD, but not LCD, intake. SB242084 blocked the effects of WAY163909 on reversal learning and HCD intake following 6 months in the dietary choice condition.

Reversal learning after METH or HCD exposure

Long-term intake of METH or a HCD increased perseverative behavior and this increase appears to be specific, as no effect was seen in other DRL task measures, including correct responses and random errors. The increase in perseveration was likely not driven by a general disruption of learning, or increased training during acquisition, because discrimination acquisition was not affected by the HCD or METH intake period. Reversal learning performance at baseline in this study was poor compared to what others have noted in monkeys [41]. This may be due to the use of forced corrections, which are not commonly employed.

Effects of WAY163909 on reversal learning

WAY163909 decreased perseverative errors, but not random errors, at baseline and following long-term METH or HCD intake, suggesting the effects of WAY163909 on perseveration were specific. Acquisition latency was not affected, indicating that the decrease in perseveration produced by WAY163909 was not due to a decrease in object discrimination exposure or training. These results agree with previous findings that Ro 60-0175 [36] and CP 809.101 [37] decreased perseverative responding, but conflict with another study reporting that the SB242084 had the same effect [38]. Differences in the compounds used, the specifics of the task, and model organisms may account for these disparate results. WAY163909 improved reversal learning performance by increasing reversal correct responses at baseline and after prolonged HCD or METH intake. These results suggest that WAY163909 improves flexible decision-making within the context of changing reinforcement contingencies, regardless of food or drug intake history, by both increasing newly reinforced responses and decreasing responses that are no longer correct.

Effects of WAY163909 on food intake

WAY163909 had no effect on baseline consumption of a LCD. Under the dietary choice condition, WAY163909 reduced total caloric intake by attenuating HCD, but not LCD, intake. Thus, 5-HT2CR modulation of food intake appears to be diet-specific, suggesting that these effects are not due to general decreases in motor function or appetitive behavior, but are specific to highly reinforcing foods [8, 9]. In agreement with the findings reported here, previous studies using the less selective 5-HT2CR agonist lorcaserin found that this compound reduced intake of a high fat diet and body weight in rats [46] and humans [47].

Agonists at the 5-HT2CR exhibit efficacy at decreasing intake of drugs and foods that have known effects on reward circuitry. The 5-HT2CR agonist Ro 60-0175 reduced cocaine intake in squirrel monkeys [19], and lorcaserin reduced nicotine [25] and alcohol [26] intake in rats. In a previous study, the same doses of WAY163909 used here reduced drug intake and drug seeking of METH and cocaine [21] in rhesus monkeys. One study reported that 7-day treatment with lorcaserin, which is less selective for the 5-HT2CR and exhibits more 5-HT2AR agonist activity than WAY163909 [40], did not attenuate cocaine vs food choice [48]. This study evaluated only male rhesus monkeys; whereas, the study using WAY163909 [21] used males and females. Moreover, the drug self-administration paradigms and actual compounds used were different, which may also account for the opposing results. Overall, these studies suggest that the effects of 5-HT2C agonists may be generalizable to behaviors that show characteristics of addiction given their impact on the reward system.

The same doses of WAY163909 decreased HCD intake and perseverative responding, and the same dose of SB242084 blocked the effects of WAY163909 on both behaviors. Similarly, the same doses of WAY163909 that reduced perseverative behavior in the current study attenuated METH intake and drug seeking of METH [21]. These data suggest that the reduction in HCD or METH intake [21] induced by activation of 5-HT2CRs is accompanied by a parallel decrease in perseverative behavior as assessed by the DRL task. It is unknown at this time how activation of 5-HT2CRs affects other domains of decision-making. However, activation of 5-HT2CRs may be simultaneously modulating both perseverative behavior and HCD or METH intake through related neurochemical mechanisms.

Effects of SB242084 + WAY163909 on reversal learning after METH or HCD exposure, and food intake after HCD exposure

The present findings suggest that the 5-HT2CR is necessary for the effects of WAY163909, as SB242084 blocked them. WAY163909 increased correct responses, and decreased perseverative responding and HCD intake, and these effects were attenuated when SB242084 was given in combination with WAY163909. These results conflict with a previous study showing that SB242084 decreases perseveration [38] in rodents, but they align with other findings demonstrating that 5-HT2C receptor agonists, rather than antagonists, decrease perseverative responding [36, 37]. The present results also agree with studies in rhesus monkeys showing that SB242084 blocks the decreases in cocaine self-administration induced by 5-HT2CR agonists [19] and can itself recapitulate some of the abuse-related effects of psychostimulants [20].

Limitations and future directions

There were two significant limitations to this study. First, the effects of the 5-HT2CR antagonist alone on reversal performance were not evaluated. This would have provided important within-subjects information and clarified the previously discussed inconsistent effects of the 5-HT2CR on reversal learning. Additionally, it would have been useful to show that other receptor antagonists would not reverse the effects WAY163909. Future studies could greatly clarify and improve upon the findings reported here by addressing these limitations in a separate experiment.