INTRODUCTION

Bulimia nervosa (BN) is defined by the presence of recurrent binge-eating episodes that are accompanied by a sense of loss of control and followed by self-induced vomiting or another compensatory behavior to avoid weight gain. BN primarily affects females and typically begins during adolescence, a period of brain maturation during which structural and functional brain changes occur that may relate to adolescent-specific behaviors and characteristics such as risk taking and impulsivity (Romer, 2010), as well as the adolescent emergence of psychiatric illnesses (Giedd and Rapoport, 2010). In healthy individuals, white matter (WM) microstructure continues to change over adolescence and into adulthood (Lebel and Beaulieu, 2011; Lebel et al, 2008), specifically in tracts connecting the frontal cortex with other cortical and subcortical regions (Asato et al, 2010), with frontotemporal connections demonstrating the most prolonged development. Thus, uncovering WM abnormalities in individuals with BN may help to explain its typical emergence during adolescence and persistence into adulthood.

Our previous functional neuroimaging findings from adult and adolescent females with BN suggest that a failure to engage frontal regions may contribute to an impaired capacity for self-regulation (Marsh et al, 2009), early in the course of the illness (Marsh et al, 2011). Our previous structural MRI data revealed that local volumes of bilateral frontal and temporoparietal cortices were reduced in BN compared with healthy participants (Marsh et al, 2015). These data also suggested that reductions in inferior frontal cortices are associated with, and may underlie, these regulatory deficits in individuals with BN, likely contributing to their impaired control over eating and other behaviors (Marsh et al, 2015). Because these reductions became more prominent with age in BN, we suggested that they might represent an abnormal developmental trajectory such as accelerated gray matter loss, axonal pruning, or reduced myelination in frontal cortices. Data from the only extant diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) study of individuals with BN suggest reduced WM integrity in adult patients compared with healthy women in bilateral corona radiata extending to the posterior limb of the internal capsule, corpus callosum, right subinsular WM, and right fornix (Mettler et al, 2013). These data were interpreted in relation to altered taste, body perception, and reward processing in BN. Unknown; however, is whether WM microstructure is abnormal earlier in the course of the disorder or whether such abnormalities might contribute to self-regulatory deficits in BN.

Thus, the objective of this study was to assess WM microstructure in adolescents and adults with BN compared with age-matched healthy participants. Based on previous DTI findings (Mettler et al, 2013) and our previous findings from women with BN, we hypothesized that compared with control participants, those with BN would have reduced fractional anisotropy (FA) in multiple WM tracts, specifically those extending to and from frontal and temporoparietal areas. FA describes the distribution of water diffusion within a voxel and is thought to reflect properties of WM indices that can also be derived to add more information about WM architecture, such as mean, axial, and radial diffusivity (ie, MD, AD, and RD, respectively). To extend our previous study of surface morphology in these same individuals (Marsh et al, 2015), we also explored group differences in the age correlates of FA and whether abnormalities in the WM microstructure of individuals with BN were associated with measures of BN symptom severity or with deficits in self-regulatory control, as measured by cognitive interference on a Stroop task (Stroop, 1935) performed outside of the MRI scanner.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

Diffusion data were acquired from 35 adolescent and adult females with BN and 37 age- and BMI-matched healthy control (HC) females. A total of 33 of the BN and 28 of the HC participants had participated in our previous structural (Marsh et al, 2015) and functional (Marsh et al, 2009, 2011) MRI studies. Those with BN were recruited through flyers posted in the local community and advertisements posted on Internet sites (such as craigslist.com and eating disorder-specific websites) and through the New York State Psychiatric Institute (NYSPI) Eating Disorders Clinic where some were receiving treatment. Control participants were recruited through flyers and Internet advertisements. Those with a history of neurological illness, past seizures, head trauma with loss of consciousness, mental retardation, pervasive developmental disorder, or current Axis I disorders (other than major depression for the BN participants) were excluded. Controls also had no lifetime Axis I disorders. Formal diagnoses of BN and comorbid neuropsychiatric diagnoses were established using standard adult and child measures (Supplementary Material). All participants received monetary compensation for their participation. The institutional review board of the NYSPI approved this study, and all participants gave informed consent.

DTI Data Acquisition

MRI data were acquired on a GE Signa 3.0 Tesla whole body scanner (GE Medical Systems, Waukesha, Wisconsin). Diffusion data were measured along 25 noncollinear spatial directions using a single-shot spin echo planar imaging sequence (TR/TE=17 000 ms /90 ms, flip angle=90, field of view=24 cm, matrix size=132 × 128 (machine-interpolated to 256 × 256 for postprocessing), b value=1000 s/mm2, and three baseline images at b=0 s/mm2, slice thickness=2.5 mm without gap, and an in-plane resolution of 0.94 mm). Scan time for each excitation was 7 min and 56 s and we averaged 2 separate excitations (NEX=2) for each participant.

DTI Data Processing

DTI data were processed using the FMRIB Software Library (FSL) 5.0.6 (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/) (Smith et al, 2004). FMRIB diffusion toolbox (FDT) was used to assess FA, MD, AD, and RD. First, eddy currents that induce stretches and shears in the diffusion weighted images (DWIs) were corrected using FDT toolbox, and then brain was extracted using the Brain Extraction Tool (Smith, 2002) in order to delete nonbrain tissue from an image of the whole head. Second, a diffusion tensor model was fitted at each voxel using DTIFIT and FA, MD, AD, and RD images were derived from the fitted diffusion tensors. Data quality was assessed by visually checking the raw DW images, eddy current corrected DW images, and the color-encoded FA images (He et al, 2014) to exclude participants who had motion-corrupted DTI data.

Voxelwise statistical analysis of the FA images was then carried out using tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS) (Smith et al, 2006). First, all subjects’ FA images were aligned to a 1 × 1 × 1 mm standard space by running the nonlinear registration. The FMRIB58_FA standard-space image was used as the target image for registration (Smith et al, 2006) and the aligned FA images from each participant were then averaged to create a mean FA image. A mean FA skeleton image was generated by using a thinned group mean FA images, representing the centers of all WM tracts common to the group. The FA threshold for the skeleton was 0.20 to exclude gray matter regions from the analyses. Each participant’s aligned FA images were then projected onto the skeleton that was then used for statistical analyses. Nonlinear warps and skeleton projection were then also applied to MD, AD, and RD images for statistical analyses.

Stroop Interference

Stroop interference, measured outside the scanner, was used for correlation analyses with FA because the resolution of interference on this task requires the engagement of self-regulatory control (Marsh et al, 2006). This task version consisted of three subtasks on which participants were asked to name the color of dots (task A), read words printed in black ink (task B), and name the color of printed words as quickly as possible (task C). The time to completion of each task was recorded and Stroop interference was calculated (Supplementary Material).

Statistical Analyses

To assess potential alterations in WM microstructure, permutation-based nonparametric inference (n=10 000) was used to perform statistical analyses on FA as well as MD, AD, and RD using FSL Randomize version 2.9 (Nichols and Holmes, 2002). Our a priori test of group differences in FA and our exploratory tests of group differences in MD, AD, and RD were corrected for multiple comparisons with threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE, FWE-corrected p<0.05) (Smith and Nichols, 2009). The Johns Hopkins University White-Matter Tractography Atlas (Oishi et al, 2011) was used to label WM tracts. Using linear regression models adjusting for age and/or illness duration (p<0.05, uncorrected), we also explored associations of FA with BN symptom severity in the BN group and compared the groups in their patterns of correlations of FA with age and Stroop interference in tracts where group differences were detected.

RESULTS

Participants

After removing the DTI data from 7 BN and 9 control participants because of excessive motion, data from 28 female adolescents and adults with BN and 28 age-matched healthy females remained for analysis including 15 BN and 15 healthy adolescents (⩽19 years). The majority (26 BN and 25 HC) of these individuals also participated in our previous MRI studies (Marsh et al, 2009, 2011, 2015). All participants were right-handed. The BN group included 5 participants who were receiving inpatient treatment (scanned within 1 month of admission), 8 who were receiving outpatient psychotherapy, and 15 who were symptomatic but nontreatment seeking at the time of participation. None met criteria for major depressive disorder. Seven had a prior diagnosis of anorexia nervosa (AN). Ten were taking selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) at the time of scanning. Table 1 shows additional demographic and clinical characteristics of both groups.

Table 1 Demographic, Clinical, and Neuropsychological Characteristics of Participants

A Priori Hypothesis Testing

Compared with HC participants, those with BN had significantly smaller FA values in one large cluster (TFCE, FWE-corrected p<0.05) encompassing forceps minor and major, superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF), SLF-temporal, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFO), anterior thalamic radiation (ATR), corticospinal tract (CST), uncinate fasciculus (UNC), and cingulum (cingulate gyrus) of both hemispheres (Figure 1 and Table 2). In no instance did participants with BN exhibit significantly more FA than HC participants.

Figure 1
figure 1

Group differences in white matter microstructure. Cluster in which less FA was detected in the BN compared with HC participants (threshold-free cluster enhancement, FWE-corrected p<0.05). Regions showing smaller FA values are overlaid on the mean FA skeleton (green) and MNI152 T1 map. The results are ‘thickened’ using tbss_fill script in FSL TBSS toolbox and displayed in neurological orientation for better visualization. ATR, anterior thalamic radiation; CNG, cingulum (cingulate gyrus); CST, corticospinal tract; IFO, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus; SLF, superior longitudinal fasciculus; SLF-temporal, superior longitudinal fasciculus (temporal part); UNC, uncinate fasciculus.

PowerPoint slide

Table 2 Cluster in Which FA Was Significantly Less in the BN Compared with HC Participants

Exploratory Analyses

Group differences in RD

Compared with HC participants, those with BN had larger RD values in many of the same tracts where FA values were smaller: forceps minor and major, left SLF, SLF-temporal, IFO, ATR, CST, and cingulum (cingulate gyrus) (TFCE, FWE-corrected p<0.05, Supplementary Figure S1). No significant group differences were detected in any tracts using AD and MD.

Correlations with symptom severity

In the BN group, significant inverse associations of FA with log-transformed objective bulimic episodes (Walsh et al, 1997) were detected in forceps minor and major, left ATR, IFO, and SLF. Scatterplots of these associations revealed that the BN participants who engaged in most episodes in the 28 days before scanning had the least FA in these tracts (p<0.05, Figure 2). Significant inverse associations of FA with log-transformed vomiting episodes were detected in the same tracts (p<0.05, Supplementary Figure S2) and scatterplots indicated the least FA in the BN participants who engaged in those most vomiting episodes. Finally, significant inverse associations of FA with ratings of preoccupation with shape and weight were also detected in forceps minor, left ATR, IFO, and SLF, with scatterplots suggesting the least FA in those who were the most preoccupied with their shape and weight (p<0.05, Supplementary Figure S3).

Figure 2
figure 2

Correlations of FA with objective bulimic episodes in the BN group. Significant correlations of FA with objective bulimic episodes within the past 28 days before the MRI scan were detected in forceps minor and major, left ATR, IFO, and SLF. Logarithmic transformation was used to reduce excessive skewness in this frequency variable. FA values, adjusted for age and duration of illness, are plotted on the y axis, and log-transformed objective bulimic episodes are plotted on the x axis. The scatter shows less FA in those with more severe symptoms. These results are ‘thickened’ using tbss_fill script in FSL TBSS toolbox and displayed in neurological orientation.

PowerPoint slide

Age effects

Significant diagnosis-by-age interactions on FA were detected in forceps minor and major, bilateral ATR, CST, and IFO, and left SLF (p<0.05, Figure 3). Scatterplots of these interactions indicated that FA in these tracts correlated positively with age in the BN group only. In addition, significant diagnosis-by-age interactions on RD were detected in forceps minor and major, left ATR, CST, and SLF (p<0.05, Supplementary Figure S4), with scatterplots indicating inverse correlations of RD with age in these tracts in the BN group. Removal of the oldest BN participant (46 years) revealed that these diagnosis-by-age interactions on FA and RD were not outlier driven (p<0.05). In addition, these group differences in age effects remained when we controlled for illness duration in the BN sample (Supplementary Material and Supplementary Figure S5). Finally, analyses comparing FA and RD in the adolescent (15 BN, 15 HC, ⩽19 years) and adult (13 BN, 13 HC) groups separately revealed significant group differences in the adolescents only; compared with HC adolescents, those with BN had smaller FA and larger RD values in multiple tracts bilaterally, including forceps minor and major, ATR, SLF, SLF-temporal, IFO, inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), CST, UNC, and cingulum (cingulate gyrus) (TFCE, FWE-corrected p’s <0.05, Supplementary Figures S6a and b).

Figure 3
figure 3

Correlations of FA with age in the BN versus control groups. FA in forceps minor and major, bilateral ATR, CST, IFO, and left SLF correlated positively with age in the BN participants but not the healthy participants, producing significant diagnosis × age interactions. FA is plotted on the y axis and age is plotted on the x axis. The results are ‘thickened’ using tbss_fill script in FSL TBSS toolbox and displayed in neurological orientation.

PowerPoint slide

Interactions of diagnosis × Stroop interference

The BN and HC groups performed similarly on the Stroop task (Table 1). However, significant interactions of diagnosis-by-Stroop interference scores on FA were detected in forceps minor, left CST, SLF, and SLF-temporal, and right ATR and IFO (p<0.05, Supplementary Figure S7). FA in these tracts correlated inversely with Stroop scores in the BN and positively with Stroop scores in the healthy participants. Significant diagnosis-by-Stroop interactions on RD were also detected in left CST and IFO, as well as in forceps major (p<0.05). RD in these tracts correlated positively with Stroop scores in the BN and inversely with Stroop scores in the healthy participants.

Medication and comorbidity effects

After including current SSRI treatment as an independent variable in a separate linear regression, significantly decreased FA values in the forceps minor and major, SLF, SLF-temporal, IFO, ATR, and CST in the BN group remained unchanged from those depicted in Figure 1. We additionally conducted a between-group analysis including only the BN participants who were not taking medications (n=18). Both analyses indicated that SSRI treatment did not have an appreciable effect on our findings. Similarly, neither a history of anorexia nor current depressive symptoms contributed to our findings (p’s<0.05).

DISCUSSION

Significantly less FA was detected in BN compared with healthy individuals in WM tracts extending through bilateral frontal and temporoparietal areas, including forceps minor and major, SLF, SLF-temporal, IFO, ATR, CST, UNC, and cingulum. In the BN group, greater FA reductions in forceps, left ATR, IFO, and SLF were detected in those with the most severe symptoms, and increased RD was detected in the same left hemisphere tracts where FA was reduced compared with healthy participants. Significant diagnosis-by-age interactions on FA and RD in forceps and left ATR, CST, and SLF were driven by the BN group in which FA increased and RD decreased with advancing age in these tracts. Moreover, group differences in FA and RD were more widespread in adolescent participants, suggesting that abnormalities in WM microstructure arise early in the course of BN. Finally, significant diagnosis-by-Stroop interactions in forceps minor, right ATR and IFO, left CST, and SLF indicated that greater Stroop interference accompanied greater FA reductions in the BN group, suggesting that microstructural abnormalities in these tracts may be associated with functional deficits in self-regulation in BN.

Our previous fMRI findings from adult (Marsh et al, 2009) and adolescent (Marsh et al, 2011) females with BN indicated deficient activation of frontal areas compared with age-matched control participants during performance of a self-regulatory task. Our structural findings suggested smaller local volumes of left inferior frontal and bilateral temporoparietal regions in the same BN compared with healthy participants (Marsh et al, 2015). In addition, reductions in inferior frontal cortices were associated with more severe symptoms, advancing age, and greater Stroop interference in the BN group. The current findings of reduced FA in SLF, IFO, ATR, CST, UNC, and cingulum suggest that alterations in the microstructure of WM pathways extending to and from frontal and temporoparietal cortices are also present in adolescents with BN.

Forceps minor is the frontal portion of the corpus callosum. Thus, our finding of reduced FA in forceps minor is consistent with previous findings of reduced FA in the corpus callosum in adults with BN compared with healthy women (Mettler et al, 2013). These data were interpreted in light of human and nonhuman data implicating the corpus callosum in taste processing (Fabri and Polonara, 2013). Reduced FA in the corpus callosum has also been reported in adolescent binge drinkers (McQueeny et al, 2009) and is associated with increased impulsivity in cocaine dependence (Moeller et al, 2005), suggesting a common potential mechanism for binge-type and impulsive behaviors across eating and substance disorders. In contrast, reduced FA in the CST may be specific to BN and interpreted in terms of the physiological effects of binge eating and purging. The CST is a descending pathway that connects the cerebral cortex with the spinal cord and is involved in voluntary or skilled movements. Nerves within the CST support movement in the muscles of the head that are involved in swallowing, phonation, and movements of the tongue (Young et al, 2008). As such, microstructural abnormalities within the CST may perhaps be a product of the illness, given the detrimental effects of binge eating and purging on oropharyngeal swallow patterns in BN (Roberts et al, 1989).

We also detected less FA in the IFO and SLF in BN compared with healthy participants. These are major association pathways that connect frontal cortices with temporal and occipital lobes (IFO) (Kier et al, 2004) and with parietal areas (SLF I, II, III) and superior temporal gyrus (Makris et al, 2005). Both regional volume (Takeuchi et al, 2013) and integrity (Peters et al, 2014) of the IFO are associated with performance on tasks of executive functions in healthy individuals, regardless of age. These findings particularly implicate the IFO in visual processing, consistent with its frontal connections to occipital and temporal visual association cortices (Martino et al, 2010). Less FA in this tract has also been reported in women with AN compared with healthy women (Kazlouski et al, 2011), data suggested to relate to body image disturbances that may be due, in part, to poor communication across occipital, temporal, and frontal cortices. Findings from both animals and humans suggest that body image perceptions may be represented within the parietal cortex (Gaudio et al, 2014; Iriki et al, 2001). Given that three of the SLF components connect parietal and frontal regions, microstructural alterations in these WM fibers might also contribute to the impaired regulation of body representations in eating disorders. Although we did not have a direct measure of body image, FA reductions in IFO and SLF were greatest in the BN participants who were most preoccupied with their shape and weight. Thus, although speculative, reduced FA in these tracts may be associated with the impaired processing and regulation of body image in individuals with eating disorders, consistent with findings of reduced FA in individuals with AN (Kazlouski et al, 2011; Via et al, 2014) and body dysmorphic disorder (Buchanan et al, 2013) compared with healthy participants. Reduced FA in IFO and SLF is also consistent with our previous findings of smaller local volumes of bilateral temporoparietal areas in BN compared with control participants (Marsh et al, 2015), suggesting abnormalities in both gray and WM in these brain areas (Joos et al, 2010).

Our findings of less FA in the UNC, cingulum, and ATR may relate to the previously documented fMRI findings of reward dysfunction in individuals both ill and recovered from BN (Frank et al, 2013). The UNC and cingulum are major limbic tracts (Catani and Thiebaut de Schotten, 2008), whereas the ATR connects thalamus to medial prefrontal cortex, with further connections to medial temporal regions (ie, hippocampus and amygdala) that are involved in reward processing (Haber and Knutson, 2010). The integrity of these tracts may influence reward processing functions, as FA in these tracts is associated with reward-related BOLD response in the nucleus accumbens in healthy individuals (Camara et al, 2010; Koch et al, 2014). As the ATR is hypothesized to be part of an emotional/motivational system and relevant for the regulation of mood (Coenen et al, 2012), less FA in this tract may also relate to mood dysregulation in individuals with BN.

The only extant DTI study of individuals with BN (Mettler et al, 2013) used a different methodology for data analysis (SPM vs TBSS) and a different atlas for anatomical labeling (Orrison, 1995 versus Oishi et al, 2011). Nevertheless, our findings converge in showing that FA is reduced in BN in multiple WM tracts. Unlike the prior study, we also assessed age effects on group differences in WM microstructure and observed diagnosis-by-age interactions on FA in tracts where group differences were detected (ie, forceps minor and major, ATR, CST, IFO, and left SLF), deriving from significant positive associations with age in the BN group. Together with our finding that FA reductions were more widespread in the BN compared with healthy adolescents, these data suggest that WM microstructural abnormalities may arise early in BN but may not contribute to illness persistence, as FA reductions were not associated with illness duration (Supplementary Material). Developmental DTI data from healthy individuals suggest that age-related increases in FA are primarily driven by reductions in RD (Giorgio et al, 2008), a measure that rodent post-mortem data suggest may reflect the degree of myelination (Song et al, 2002). The interpretation of changes in RD over development is less clear in humans. For example, RD reductions and FA increases in a given tract could also result from more coherent alignment of fibers or fewer crossing fibers (Jones et al, 2013). Nevertheless, our finding that RD was inversely associated with age in multiple left hemisphere tracts in the BN group may suggest an abnormal trajectory of WM development in BN. Future longitudinal studies should test this hypothesis directly, given that protracted myelination seems to underlie many different disease phenotypes (Haroutunian et al, 2014).

Although FA in multiple tracts increased with advancing age in the BN group, less FA in some of these tracts (forceps minor, right ATR and IFO, left CST, and SLF) was associated with greater Stroop interference in BN. As significant group differences in FA and RD were not detected in adults (when analyzed separately), the effects of Stroop performance and symptom severity could not be tested in the adult subsample because these exploratory analyses entailed using FA (or RD) values extracted from clusters on maps depicting significant group differences. Thus, the adolescents in our BN sample likely contributed to the association of less FA with greater Stroop interference in that group. Stroop interference typically decreases with advancing age in healthy individuals (Marsh et al, 2006), likely reflecting the functional maturation of frontostriatal and frontoparietal attentional control circuits. Integrity of the SLF is associated with the ability to orient attention on another cognitive task in healthy adolescents (Ge et al, 2013), and less FA in the IFO is reported in adults with ADHD (Shaw et al, 2015). Thus, less FA in these tracts in adolescents with BN may contribute to their difficulty in mobilizing the attentional resources necessary to avoid word reading on the Stroop task and to avoid the cues that trigger their binge-eating episodes in daily life.

This study is limited by its modest sample size, cross-sectional design, and the heterogeneity of our BN sample that included participants at differing stages of treatment and who varied in symptom severity. In addition, our BN and control groups differed in verbal IQ, but this difference was driven by the adults in our sample (Supplementary Table S1) and therefore unlikely driving the group differences in FA that remained significant in the adolescents when tested separately. Although we did not account for menstrual status, which could affect neural functioning in women (Dreher et al, 2007), we do not suspect that FA would be affected, nor do we suspect that menstrual status differed systematically enough across groups to confound our findings. Another limitation was our use of acquisition parameters that recent advances have shown to be suboptimal. For example, the acquisition of at least 30 gradient directions and isotropic voxels is now recommended (Jones et al, 2013), and newer parameters may reduce the crossing fiber issue (Jbabdi et al, 2010). Finally, our use of the Stroop task to assess self-regulatory control in this study was not ideal, given that Stroop interference was not associated with BN symptoms in our sample.

Future studies of individuals with BN should employ tractography to further probe the WM microstructural abnormalities uncovered herein and enhance our understanding of anatomical connectivity in BN. Such future studies should continue probing associations of anatomical connections with brain function and behavior in BN. Future longitudinal studies comparing brain development in BN and healthy adolescents, beginning at illness onset, are required to determine whether our cross-sectional findings of age effects on WM microstructure in the BN group reflect abnormal maturation or neurodevelopmental abnormalities in BN or, instead, the effects of chronic illness on WM microstructure.

FUNDING AND DISCLOSURE

The authors declare no conflict of interest.