INTRODUCTION

A large body of literature suggests dopamine (DA) is involved in the genesis of schizophrenic symptoms, but the exact mechanism by which it exerts its effects remains unclear. Much current thinking suggests that schizophrenia may be associated with subcortical hyperdopaminergia—based in large part on a substantial neuroimaging literature indicating increased presynaptic release of DA in the striatum of schizophrenic patients (Howes et al, 2012)—and prefrontal cortex (PFC) hypodopaminergia (Guillin et al, 2007). Moreover, it has been suggested that although hyperdopaminergia may not be the sole cause of the illness, it may be involved in the exacerbation of symptoms or at least some subset of them. A clearer understanding of these questions would shed light on the neurobiological basis of the illness and, in turn, potentially point the way to more effective treatments.

Understandably, recent attention has focused not only on the specific DA abnormalities seen in schizophrenia, but also on how these may lead to particular clinical manifestations of the illness. DA is probably best regarded not simply as ‘excitatory’ or ‘inhibitory’. Rather, recent work has suggested that it may alter the processing of information in a region-specific manner (eg, Santesso et al, 2009). Dopaminergic innervation originates primarily in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and nearby substantia nigra, and from here projects widely. The primary DA pathways are the mesocortical, which projects from VTA to prefrontal cortex, the mesolimbic, which projects from VTA to the hippocampus, amygdala, and nucleus accumbens, and nigrostriatal, which projects from substantia nigra to the striatum. Much recent research has focused on the subcortical nigrostriatal system as well as the mesocortical projections to PFC and other frontal cortex areas. For example, work by Winterer (2006) has suggested that electrophysiologic activity in the PFC of schizophrenic patients may be characterized by higher levels of cortical response variability (or ‘noise’), and that increased DA levels may correlate with decreased PFC noise, an effect possibly mediated via DA’s action on excitatory NMDA synapses and inhibitory GABA synapses. Consistent with this view, computational models examining the importance of DA in PFC function in the control (Durstewitz et al, 2000) and schizophrenic (Komek et al, 2012) conditions have been described.

Relatively less attention has been paid to the mesolimbic DA projections terminating in the hippocampus (Miyake et al, 2011). However, the neuroanatomic literature has shown that there are abundant VTA-to-hippocampal projections (eg, Gasbarri et al, 1997, 1994), and that the human hippocampus is rich in DA receptors (De Keyser, 1993). In the current study, we focus on DA’s modulatory effects on the hippocampus, a brain area that has been implicated in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia, particularly the genesis of positive symptoms. The hippocampus is known to be important in the storage and recall of memories and the formation of associations. The mechanism by which this is achieved is not known with certainty, but it is likely that synchrony in the gamma frequency band (40 Hz) is of central importance to healthy hippocampal functioning (Colgin and Moser, 2010), and is disrupted in schizophrenia (Gandal et al, 2012; Lisman et al, 2008).

Attempts to understand DA’s effects on hippocampal functioning in a detailed, mechanistic way—and how this may go awry in schizophrenia—are made complicated by two considerations. First, it has become clear that DA affects neural processing not through a single electrophysiologic effect but through a broad range of activities on synaptic and ion-channel functioning (Beaulieu and Gainetdinov, 2011). Second, recent research has made abundantly clear that other neurotransmitters—eg, the glutamatergic and GABAergic systems—are abnormal in the hippocampus in schizophrenia. In addition, aberrant connectivity (Hoffman and McGlashan, 2003) has also been seen. A priori, it is not clear how DA deficiencies would interact with these abnormalities.

In this paper, we address these questions using a systems biology approach. We used a previously developed, biophysically detailed computational hippocampal model (Siekmeier and Vanmaanen, 2013) implemented on a 72-processor supercomputer, and took a specific quantitative deficit in the 40 Hz auditory steady state response (ASSR) as index of schizophrenic behavior. DA effects are applied via alterations in a constellation of synaptic and ion channel effects, in a receptor-mediated manner, both to an unaffected (control) and to a schizophrenic model. We show in the schizophrenic model that increasing DA levels lead to a decrement in gamma band response, and that in the control model similar DA levels show a modest enhancement of gamma band activity (similar to experimental work). This work may have implications for the development of novel agents to treat schizophrenia.

METHODS

Background and Model Construction

In the ASSR task, subjects receive auditory stimuli at various frequencies, and cortical response is measured via electroencephalography (EEG) or magnetoencephalography; typically, frequencies present in the response signal are determined via Fourier transform or wavelet analysis. Stimulation protocols include a range of frequencies (eg, 20, 30, and 40 Hz), and generally control subjects show a strong cortical response at the stimulated frequency. Schizophrenic patients, however, show a unique inability to attune to gamma band (40 Hz) stimulation—multiple studies have shown that schizophrenic patients’ 40 Hz response to the 40 Hz drive is markedly lower than that of controls, whereas response at other frequencies (eg, 20 and 30 Hz) is comparable to those of controls (Gandal et al, 2012). A recent review of the literature on oscillatory abnormalities in schizophrenia (Uhlhaas and Singer, 2010) found that across all experimental protocols—ie, those looking at spontaneous activity, as well as those using induced, evoked, and ASSR paradigms—the most robust effect was found in patients in the gamma band ASSR task. Thus, this may be tapping into a key neural deficit in schizophrenia and may represent a sensitive biomarker of the illness (Javitt et al, 2008).

The computational model used in this paper is based on the one detailed in our previous published work. Briefly, the model consists of 160 pyramidal cells and 80 interneurons of three subtypes (basket cells, chandelier cells, and calretinin-positive cells). The pyramidal cell model is based on the 64 compartment model of Traub, et al (1994); the interneuron model is derived from the 51 compartment model of Traub and Miles (1995). They both include realistic dendritic arbors and incorporate Na+, Ca2+, K+DR, K+AHP, K+C, and K+A channels distributed along the somato-dendritic axis; intracellular Ca2+ concentration is explicitly represented. The connectivity of the model was arrived at based on an exhaustive review of the neuroanatomic literature on projection patterns of hippocampal pyramidal neurons and interneuron subtypes, and was designed to emulate area CA1. It also includes simulated DA receptors.

In the study described here, we drive the model—that is, we deliver input stimulation to the constituent computational neurons—at 20, 30, and 40 Hz. This is meant to recreate the auditory stimuli used in the experimental human studies using the ASSR task, at the frequencies most commonly used. For each run, a simulated EEG, calculated based on Nunez (1981), is written to file. We use two distinct network models throughout our study, corresponding to the control and schizophrenic cases. The manner in which we created these models is described in Siekmeier and Vanmaanen (2013). Briefly, we tuned the control model to show similar 20, 30, and 40 Hz behaviors to the controls in experimental studies. We arrived at the schizophrenic model by (1) exhaustively reviewing the relevant literature on NMDA function, dendritic spine density, and GABA system integrity in schizophrenic hippocampus and, based on the ranges of abnormalities found, (2) searching the parameter space of putative schizophrenic neural abnormalities, using the response to the ASSR task as our outcome metric. Compared with the control model, the schizophrenic model has a 30% decrease in NMDA conductance and dendritic spine density.

The model is implemented using the General Neural Simulation System neural modeling software,version 2.3, with MPI programs written in C++ for parameter searches. The hardware platform used is a 72-processor dedicated Beowulf Cluster running under Linux (Red Hat Enterprise Linux 5.0).

Operationalization of Dopaminergic Effects

At baseline in humans, extracellular DA levels are said to be around 10–20 nM, and with activity intrasynaptic concentrations can reach the micromolar range (Grace, 2011). Direct measures of tissue-level increases of DA in schizophrenic patients are, of course, very difficult to obtain. Indirect measures involving (11C)raclopride binding (Abi-Dargham et al, 2000; Laruelle et al, 1999b) or CSF samples (Gattaz et al, 1983) have tended to show modest DA increases in patients. To ensure that any possible effect is captured, we modeled DA increases ranging over several orders of magnitude, from 0 to 1 mM.

DA receptors can broadly be divided into D1-like receptors (D1 and D5) and D2-like receptors (D2, D3, and D4). Human postmortem studies that have done direct comparisons have shown that D1 receptors outnumber D2 receptors in the hippocampus, and indicated that this difference is considerable. For example, Hall et al (1988) assessed D1 vs D2 binding in postmortem human brain using homogeneous (that is, not subfield specific) samples from the hippocampus, and found average D1 concentrations of 1.4 pmol/g vs negligible concentrations of D2 receptors. De Keyser (1993), in a detailed review of the literature, estimated that the ratio of D1 to D2 receptors in the human hippocampus is 11 to 1. Studies that have done subfield-specific analyses have found that in area CA1 this difference is even more marked. For example, Court et al (1998) showed that in CA1 D1 density was 30.03 fmol/mg tissue, whereas D2 density was 0.18 fmol/mg tissue, a 17-fold difference. Gangarossa et al (2012) looked at DA binding in a cell type-specific manner (ie, pyramidal cells vs interneurons). In CA1, they found no D2 binding in either cell type to speak of. Therefore, in the current study, we focus on D1-type receptor-mediated effects.

There is an extensive literature on the manner in which DA exerts neurophysiologic effects by altering ion channel and synaptic conductances. For example, this research has shown that DA significantly decreases conductance of the KAHP (Pedarzani and Storm, 1995) and KC (Satake et al, 2008) channels, increases conductance at AMPA synapses (Yang, 2000), and has small or negligible effects at the KDR and KA channels (Dong and White, 2003). The experimental literature used to arrive at all parameter values for DA implementation is described in detail Supplementary Methods. Our methodology for introducing DA effects into the model in a concentration-dependent manner is detailed below.

We assumed that the fraction of DA receptors bound is a function of DA concentration, and its dissociation constant at the DA receptor, Ki, is represented as follows (Kenakin, 1997):

Here, P is the percentage of DA receptors bound. We take Ki to be 4.3 (Tiberi and Caron, 1994) (expressing the dissociation constant as the negative exponent of the concentration, in molar terms, at which 50% of receptors are occupied).

We assumed that the ultimate neurophysiologic effect (eg, increase or decrease in ion-channel conductance, expressed as EA below) was a linear function of DA receptor occupancy. That is,

The proportionality constant Emax (Kenakin, 1997) is different for each distinct ion channel or synapse. These values were derived from experimental studies that quantified the change in channel conductance when tissue was exposed to particular concentrations of DA or DA agonist. Emax values and the experimental studies from which they were derived are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Emax Values

Given the purposes of our study, we have chosen to focus on the relatively acute effects of DA, as outlined above. We acknowledge that this may be dissimilar from the effects of chronic DA elevation (ie, months to years), which may be associated with changes in receptor density, etc.

Simulated Experimental Protocol

We drove the control (non-schizophrenic) hippocampal model at 20, 30, and 40 Hz, and the schizophrenic model in the baseline (no DA) condition, and at increasing simulated concentrations of DA in 10 gradations. At each simulated concentration, we calculated the percentage of DA receptors occupied, using Equation (1). Then, based on this, we calculated the percentage change in conductance in each of the constituent ion channels of the model, using Equation (2) and the values of Table 1. Thus, for example, at a DA concentration of 67 μM, 57% of DA receptors are bound (from Equation (1)). At this level, Na2+ conductance is decreased by 57% × 0.222=0.126, Ca2+ conductance is decreased by 57% × 0.20=0.114, etc. At the maximum simulated concentration (1 mM), there is complete receptor saturation and all conductance changes in Table 1 apply. An identical series of trials were performed for the schizophrenic model. These two virtual experiments correspond to clinical experiments in which control and schizophrenic patients were subjected to the ASSR task in the presence and absence of dopaminergic agents (Albrecht et al, 2013; Komek et al, 2012).

RESULTS

We first needed to confirm the validity of the control model by quantitatively comparing its behavior with that observed in clinical studies. Albrecht et al (2013) studied 44 unaffected (non-schizophrenic) subjects using the ASSR task, with 20 and 40 Hz stimulation. In one trial subjects were given dexamphetamine, to induce hyperdopaminergia, and in the other they received placebo. They used an experimental protocol in which stimuli (20 or 40 Hz click trains) were presented for 500 ms, with 500ms interstimulus intervals, and they found that when treated with dexamphetamine subjects showed a significantly greater 40 Hz response to 40 Hz stimulation and also significantly greater 40 Hz response to 20 Hz stimuli (Figure 1a). When we carried out their experiment computationally, we achieved similar results (Figure 1b).

Figure 1
figure 1

Auditory steady state response (ASSR) task in control subjects: experimental vs computational results. (a) Findings of Albrecht et al (2013), in which subjects were exposed to 20 and 40 Hz stimuli. Dexamphetamine was used to induce hyperdopaminergia. Bars indicate standard error. (b) Computational model outputs for analogous task. Because it is not entirely clear how dexamphetamine dosage used by Albrecht et al (0.45 mg/kg) would translate quantitatively to serum level, we used a range of DA concentrations. To simulate the clinical experimental task, we stimulated with background noise for 500 ms, then drove the model (as described in the text) for 500 ms, then delivered background noise. Response power at 40 Hz is shown on the y-axis; the x-axis indicates time from onset of this stimulus. Data are binned at 100 ms. To emulate clinical experiment as closely as possible, all simulations were run with 20 ‘simulated subjects’. To generate a simulated subject, we re-seeded the simulation’s random number generator and re-created the hippocampal model. This resulted in a model with a statistically identical pattern of connectivity (defined by the probability with which a given cell type projects to another cell type), but a different specific cell-to-cell pattern of connectivity. Bars indicate standard error.

PowerPoint slide

Next, we performed the ASSR task in the control and schizophrenic models at increasing levels of DA, as detailed in Methods. Significantly, increasing levels of simulated DA affected these two models in a dissimilar manner. In the schizophrenic case, it suppressed the 40 Hz ASSR response at intermediate concentrations. As indicated in Figure 2, this effect was not simply a result of suppression of oscillatory behaviors generally. In the control model, it has an opposite effect, modestly increasing the 40 Hz response at these concentrations. At higher DA concentrations, both control and schizophrenic models showed increased 40 Hz response to increased simulated receptor occupancy. Thus, as measured by gamma frequency response, the control model showed a monotonic response to increasing dopaminergic drive, and the schizophrenic model showed a U-shaped response (Figure 2 and Table 2).

Figure 2
figure 2

Response of control (a) and schizophrenic (b) model at increasing levels of simulated dopamine. As indicated in the legend, 20 Hz response power to 20 Hz drive, 30 Hz response to 30 Hz drive, 40 Hz response to 40 Hz drive, and 20 Hz response to 40 Hz drive are shown. Eleven DA concentrations, ranging from 0 to 1 mM (=1000 μM), were performed. It is evident that the effect of DA in suppressing gamma activity in the schizophrenic case (b) is specific to that frequency: at the DA concentration associated with the greatest decrease in 40 Hz response (31 μM), 20 Hz response is not statistically significantly different from baseline (P=0.84), and 30 Hz response is actually greater than baseline (P<0.0001). At 1000 μM, the model showed a behavior consistent with the graphic trends shown (data not shown). For each trial, 20 ‘simulated patients’ (see Figure 1 caption) were used. Error bars indicate standard deviation. P-values are given in Table 2 and text.

PowerPoint slide

Table 2 40 Hz Response of Control and Schizophrenic Models at Various Levels of Simulated Dopamine

In order to investigate whether or not a particular parameter is more or less important than others in the instantiation of the concentration-dependent DA effect, we performed a ‘sensitivity analysis’, as described in Supplementary Results. It was seen that GABA synaptic conductance and the AMPA decay time constant (τ2) were particularly important mediators of DA’s effect on oscillatory activity. Model behavior with these effects removed, alone and in combination, is shown as Supplementary Figures S1, S2, and S3.

In an additional set of trials, D1-mediated effects on synaptic plasticity at NMDA synapses were included—this was implemented via intracellular Ca2+ concentration effects, using the methodology described in Siekmeier and Vanmaanen (2013). Resultant patterns of oscillatory activity were very similar to those of Figure 2 (data not shown). This is understandable, given the fact that this is an activity-dependent phenomenon, and the period of synchronous stimulation is relatively brief.

An interesting—and counterintuitive—behavior of the schizophrenic model is its tendency to show an increase in 20 Hz response to 40 Hz drive (dashed line in Figure 2), while showing a decrease in 40 Hz response, as DA concentration increased. This was not present in the control model. In an attempt to understand the mechanistic basis of this behavior, we examined a simulated EEG trace and associated power spectrum for a representative schizophrenic and control patient. The manner in which these changed as simulated DA concentrations are increased is shown in Figure 3.

Figure 3
figure 3

EEG response of a representative schizophrenic case to increasing levels of simulated dopamine. (a) Baseline (0 dopamine) condition for a sample simulated patient; EEG response to 40 Hz stimulation, and power spectrum of response, are shown. In panels (b) through (j), the simulated dopamine level is successively increased, with concentrations corresponding to those of Figure 2 and Table 2, to a maximum of 310 μM. Only 1 s of a 1.5 s run is shown so that the ‘beat-skipping’ behavior may be clearly seen.

PowerPoint slide

Of note, the decrease in 40 Hz response at intermediate DA concentrations seen in the schizophrenic model does not result from an across-the-board decrement in the amplitude of this frequency. Rather, the EEG trace reveals that every other peak is suppressed, thus creating decreased power at 40 Hz, and the emergence of a stronger 20 Hz signal.

DISCUSSION

A substantial research literature suggests that DA is involved in the genesis of schizophrenic symptoms, but the mechanism by which this occurs is not entirely clear. Moreover, since the original formulation of the DA hypothesis, large numbers of abnormalities—in other neurotransmitter systems, and in brain connectivity—have been identified in the illness. This computational modeling study offers a possible way of integrating these findings: it suggests how DA’s neurophysiological and neurocognitive effects may vary, depending on the neural substrate on which it acts. Specifically, in a hippocampus afflicted with the NMDA dysfunction and connectivity disturbances thought to be associated with schizophrenia, hyperdopaminergia appears to worsen to gamma band synchrony deficit that has been seen in the illness, whereas in an unaffected hippocampus this effect appears to be absent. Also, it potentially explains the episodic nature of schizophrenia and supports the view, articulated in the clinical research literature, that DA may act as ‘the wind of the psychotic fire’ (Laruelle and Abi-Dargham, 1999a).

Comparison with Experimental Findings

One clue to DA’s role in the emergence of schizophrenic symptoms comes from clinical studies in which dopaminergic agents (eg, amphetamine) are administered to control subjects and schizophrenics. Our modeling results are consistent with these findings, in that these studies have generally shown that such agents exacerbate psychotic symptoms in patient groups, but, with acute administration, do not tend to create psychotic symptoms de novo in controls. In a review of 36 studies, Lieberman et al (1987) found that 40% of schizophrenic patients showed a worsening of psychotic symptoms after acute administration of psychostimulants vs 2% of controls (P<0.0001).

There are a few clinical studies that have examined the effect of dopaminergic manipulation on the ASSR task in control (Albrecht et al, 2013) as well as schizophrenic subjects (Komek et al, 2012). As mentioned above, our work replicated the findings of Albrecht et al on control subjects (Figure 1). In Komek et al’s study of the effect of D-amphetamine administration on 40 Hz ASSR in schizophrenic patients, they included 12 schizophrenic patients as well as 12 controls and found that patients showed a modest increase in 40 Hz response (P<0.05) when they received amphetamine at a dosage of 0.5 mg/kg. It is somewhat difficult to interpret the results of their study in light of ours because, for practical reasons, only a single dose was given as opposed to a range of doses. It is possible that the dose administered led to a high degree of receptor occupancy, and therefore an enhancement of gamma response (as indicated at higher DA concentrations in Figure 1b). This group also found that the controls showed a decrease in gamma band ASSR in response to amphetamine challenge. One possible reason for this discrepancy from our findings is that these researchers focused on PFC—rather than hippocampus. These two brain areas may differ in DA-modulated ASSR response.

Another clinical study whose findings are consistent with those of our computational study is that of Barr et al (2011). This work showed that repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation delivered to the dorsolateral PFC while subjects were performing an N-back working memory task elicited a reduction in gamma power in schizophrenic patients, but resulted in an enhancement in gamma in controls. This is highly pertinent, as in vivo microdialysis experiments in non-human primates have indicated that a significant increase in DA levels exists in PFC during the performance of working memory tasks (Watanabe et al, 1997).

A potentially significant confound in interpreting all of these clinical studies is the impact of medications. Although some have found no statistically significant relationship between ASSR and antipsychotic dosage in schizophrenic patients (Spencer et al, 2008), others (Hong et al, 2004a) found that patients treated with atypical antipsychotics showed greater 40 Hz ASSR than both controls and those treated with typical antipsychotics. EEG readings in these studies were taken at the fronto-central electrodes, so they may not be directly translatable to our hippocampal model. However, they do raise the very important point that because all currently used antipsychotics have activity on the DA system, going forward it would be important to specifically consider the nature of these effects in ASSR studies.

Mechanistic Implications

Neurophysiologically, our study suggests that the decreased gamma band response to 40 Hz stimulation observed under hyperdopaminergia may arise from a ‘beat-skipping’ phenomenon—that is, a 40 Hz response is converted to a mixed-mode 40 and 20 Hz response via suppression of every other peak of the phasic response. This mechanism was also shown to be operative in the schizophrenic response in the computational studies of Vierling-Claassen et al (2008) as well as Siekmeier and Vanmaanen (2013). Significantly, both of these studies instantiated schizophrenogenic neuropathologies that were dissimilar from those of the current model. Vierling–Claassen et al implemented an increased inhibitory time constant (τ2) on chandelier cell projections to pyramidal cells, and Siekmeier and vanMaanen modeled a joint reduction in NMDA conductance and dendritic spine density. This suggests that this particular neurophysiologic signature may be a ‘final common pathway’, which may be worthy of investigation as a potentially sensitive biomarker for schizophrenic symptoms.

This, of course, is not meant to suggest that ‘beat skipping’ is the only means by which gamma and beta oscillations may jointly arise in neural tissue. Computational modeling work by Traub et al (1999) investigating the phenomenon of the gamma-to-beta transition that has been observed in hippocampal slice preparations as well as human ERP studies (Hong et al, 2004b) have suggested that the synchronization of gamma and the synchronization of beta may rely on distinct mechanisms, and ones different from the mechanism we have identified.

Pathophysiologic and Treatment Implications

Many studies have shown that patients experiencing acute psychotic episodes had significantly elevated indices of synaptic DA transmission in the striatum (eg, Howes et al, 2007), indicating that DA levels here may fluctuate with the phase of the illness. Our work suggests similar effects may be at play in the mesolimbic pathway—in particular, those branches projecting to the hippocampus. This points to the manner in which hyperdopaminergia may interact with hippocampal neuropathology to produce symptoms, which can wax and wane over time. Specifically, our model suggests that increasing dopaminergic stimulation affects hippocampal function in a U-shaped manner in schizophrenia (as shown in Figure 2). This should be contrasted to the theorized effects of DA in other brain areas, such as PFC. Here, it is felt that intermediate levels of DA are associated with optimal functioning—that is, functionality appears to follow an inverted U-shaped curve. Modeling work has suggested that appropriate DA levels—that is, levels that are neither too low or too high—are crucial in stabilizing working memory representations (Durstewitz et al, 2000), working memory being a central functional role of PFC. Komek et al (2012) present a computational model of PFC showing a similar phenomenon for gamma band oscillations. Although it is not clear how the response curves to DA in various brain areas ‘align’, the apparent dissimilarities in hippocampus and PFC indicate why past attempts at developing antipsychotics that have focused on global manipulation of DA activity as indicated by receptor binding may not have been entirely successful.

The modeling described here presents the possibility of a drug development approach in which the ‘target’ is not a receptor or a single neurobiologic entity, but rather a system-level neurophysiologic behavior associated with schizophrenia. As the authors and others (Geerts et al, 2013) have demonstrated in previous work, models of this kind can be used to suggest novel sets of cellular level interactions that may be ameliorative. This could be a uniquely powerful approach for clarifying the neuropathology underlying schizophrenia and pointing the way to fundamentally new treatments.

FUNDING AND DISCLOSURE

PJS’s work has been funded by the NIMH (MH072771). In addition, he has received compensation for his work as the Web Editor of The Archives of General Psychiatry/JAMA Psychiatry. DPV declares no conflict of interest.