INTRODUCTION

Nicotine and other psychomotor stimulants produce their behavioral effects, including self-administration, by activating the mesoaccumbens dopamine (DA) system. Nicotine acts at nicotine acetylcholine receptors (nAChRs) in the midbrain ventral tegmental area to increase locomotion and DA overflow in the nucleus accumbens and to support its self-administration (Vezina et al, 2007). Activation of nAChRs has also been implicated in the self-administration of other psychostimulants including cocaine and methamphetamine (Becktholt and Mark, 2002; Glick et al, 2002; Levin et al, 2000) as well as in cue-induced craving of cocaine in humans (Reid et al, 1999). These results suggest not only a common neuronal pathway for the reinforcing actions of nicotine and other psychomotor stimulants, but also a critical role for nAChRs in the effects of other drugs.

As with other psychomotor stimulants (Vezina, 2004), rats repeatedly exposed to nicotine develop sensitization to its locomotor and nucleus accumbens DA-activating effects (Iyaniwura et al, 2001; Ksir et al, 1985; Schoffelmeer et al, 2002; Shim et al, 2001) as well as locomotor cross-sensitization to amphetamine (Birrell and Balfour, 1998; Schoffelmeer et al, 2002). Again, nAChR activation is critical for the development of locomotor and dopaminergic sensitization by amphetamine and cocaine (Schoffelmeer et al, 2002) as well as cocaine-induced conditioned place preference (Zachariou et al, 2001). These results together with those from a number of epidemiological studies (Kandel, 2002; Kandel et al, 1992) have lent support to proposals that nicotine may serve as a gateway drug leading to the pursuit of other psychostimulants (Levine et al, 2011; Weinberger and Sofuoglu, 2009). Surprisingly few preclinical studies have investigated this possibility directly and to date, such studies have provided weak support for this view. Rats exposed to nicotine as adults subsequently showed enhanced acquisition of cocaine self-administration in one study (Horger et al, 1992) but no effects in another (McQuown et al, 2007). In two other studies assessing the effect of nicotine exposure on subsequent nicotine self-administration, one reported modest effects limited to the first few days of acquisition under a fixed ratio 1 (FR1) schedule of reinforcement (Adriani et al, 2003) and another found mixed trends for enhancement and disruption (Shoaib et al, 1997). Multiple factors can influence the extent to which previous exposure to nicotine can affect subsequent drug-induced responding. These include the intensity of the exposure regimen and the withdrawal period between exposure and testing (Schoffelmeer et al, 2002; Vezina et al, 2007) as well as sex and age of the organism at the time of exposure and testing (Adriani et al, 2003; Collins et al, 2004; McQuown et al, 2007). A factor less often considered is the additional potential influence of drug-associated stimuli.

In the above experiments, adult rats exposed to nicotine in the self-administration chamber subsequently showed enhanced drug self-administration (Horger et al, 1992) while those exposed to nicotine in the home cage did not (Adriani et al, 2003; McQuown et al, 2007; Shoaib et al, 1997), suggesting that nicotine-paired contextual stimuli may have enabled the expression of enhanced drug self-administration resulting from previous exposure to nicotine. The ability of contextual stimuli to control the expression of sensitization to stimulant drugs is well established. As demonstrated in experiments with amphetamine, the presence of drug-paired or drug-unpaired stimuli during testing can regulate the intensity of the sensitized responses observed and in some cases determine whether sensitization is expressed at all (Anagnostaras and Robinson, 1996; Stewart and Vezina, 1988; Vezina and Leyton, 2009). Such stimuli have been shown to regulate the expression of locomotor and nucleus accumbens DA sensitization by nicotine as well (Reid et al, 1996, 1998). Given the demonstrated importance of drug-paired cues for the self-administration of low doses of cocaine (Schenk and Partridge, 2001) or a relatively weak primary reinforcer like nicotine (Caggiula et al, 2001), it is reasonable to expect that stimuli previously paired or unpaired with nicotine during exposure to the drug should also influence the subsequent self-administration of nicotine and other psychostimulants. This possibility remains untested, although results consistent with this outcome have been reported. For example, contextual stimuli paired with nicotine self-administration have been shown to slow extinction of responding and reinstate drug seeking in animals (Diergaarde et al, 2008; Wing and Shoaib, 2008) and, in humans, to elicit craving to smoke (Conklin, 2006). The present preclinical experiments assessed the ability of contextual stimuli paired or unpaired with nicotine exposure injections to subsequently enable or inhibit enhanced responding for amphetamine.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

Male Long–Evans rats (Harlan Teklad, Madison, WI) weighing 250–275 g upon arrival were housed individually in a 12-h light/12-h dark reverse cycle room (lights off at 0700 hours and on at 1900 hours) with food and water freely available throughout the experiments. All procedures were conducted during the dark cycle according to protocols approved by the University of Chicago Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Drug injections were initiated at least 1 h into the dark cycle and administered at the same time of day throughout the experiments. All experiments were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals as adopted and promulgated by the National Institutes of Health.

Apparatus

Experimental sessions were conducted in 16 operant chambers (model H10-11R-TC, Coulbourn Instruments, Whitehall, PA) measuring 31 × 25 × 30 cm. A single retractable lever (6 cm above the floor) and a cue light (13 cm above the lever) were positioned on the right side wall of each chamber. Each operant chamber was contained within a sound-attenuating chamber outfitted with an exhaust fan that shielded animals from extraneous disturbances. Each chamber was equipped with a counterbalanced arm, a steel-spring tether, and an infusion pump (model PHM-100, Med Associates, St Albans, VT) that permitted free movement of the animal in the chamber and delivery of drug upon depression of the lever. Lever presses and drug infusions were recorded and controlled via an electrical interface by a computer using Med-Associates software.

Drugs

(−) Nicotine tartrate and S (+)-amphetamine sulfate were obtained from Sigma (St Louis, MO). Drugs were dissolved in sterile saline (0.9% w/v). The pH of the nicotine solution was adjusted to 7.0 with NaOH.

Surgery

At the appropriate point in the experiments, rats were anesthetized with a mix of ketamine (100 mg/kg, IP) and xylazine (6 mg/kg, IP) and surgically implanted with an IV catheter into the right external jugular vein using procedures described previously (Pierre and Vezina, 1997). Catheters were made of silastic tubing (Dow Corning, Midland, MI) connected to a custom-designed L-shaped 20 gauge guide cannula (Plastics One, Roanoke, VA) that was passed subcutaneously to a small incision on the head and secured in place with dental cement anchored to skull screws. Catheters were subsequently flushed daily with a sterile 0.9% saline solution containing 30 IU/ml heparin and 250 mg/ml ampicillin in order to maintain patency.

Design and Procedures

As illustrated in Figure 1, experiments consisted of six phases: nicotine exposure, withdrawal and surgical preparation of rats, amphetamine self-administration training, amphetamine self-administration testing, extinction, and testing for amphetamine-primed reinstatement.

Figure 1
figure 1

Outline of the experimental design showing the six different phases.

PowerPoint slide

Exposure

Starting 3–5 days after arrival, rats were administered a total of five injections, one injection every third day, of either nicotine (0.4 mg/kg, IP; dose refers to the weight of the base) or saline. This injection regimen is known to sensitize rats to nicotine's locomotor and DA-activating effects (Vezina et al, 2007). In the present experiments, it was tested for its ability to enhance the subsequent self-administration of amphetamine. In order to assess the potential additional contribution of contextual stimuli paired or unpaired with nicotine during exposure on the subsequent self-administration of amphetamine, exposure injections were administered in different environments to rats in different groups. In one case, nicotine and saline injections were administered to rats in different groups in the home cage (NIC HOME CAGE; SAL HOME CAGE). In another, injections were administered in the self-administration chambers with the levers retracted (NIC SA CHAMBER; SAL SA CHAMBER). In this case, rats remained in the self-administration chambers for 2 h following each injection. For rats administered nicotine, the cue light was illuminated for the entire 2 h. Finally, rats in two additional groups were administered nicotine either Paired or Unpaired with the self-administration chambers (NIC PAIRED; NIC UNPAIRED). Paired rats were administered nicotine in the self-administration chambers on 1 day, saline in the home cage on the following day, and were left undisturbed in their home cage on the third day. This 3-day block was repeated five times. Unpaired rats were administered these injections in the reverse order: saline in the self-administration chambers and nicotine in the home cage. Injections made in the self-administration chambers were as described above. This discrimination learning procedure is known to establish a contextual stimulus complex (ie, the self-administration chamber) not only as an excitatory conditioned stimulus (CS+) when it is explicitly paired with the drug in the case of the Paired rats but also as a conditioned inhibitor (CI) when it is explicitly unpaired with the drug in the case of Unpaired rats (Mackintosh, 1974; Vezina and Leyton, 2009).

Withdrawal

Following the 13–15 day drug exposure phase, rats were afforded a 2-week procedure and drug-free period. During this time, they were surgically prepared with an IV catheter and left to recover for at least 5 days before self-administration training.

Amphetamine self-administration training

Amphetamine self-administration sessions were held daily and lasted for a maximum of 3 h. In all cases, reinforced lever presses delivered an infusion of amphetamine through the IV catheter (100 μg/kg/infusion; dose refers to weight of the salt) delivered in a volume of 0.09–0.11 ml/infusion at a rate of 1.065 ml/min. For 10 s immediately following a reinforced lever press, the cue light above the lever was lit and lever presses were recorded but without consequence. An experimenter-delivered priming infusion of amphetamine (100 μg/kg, IV) was given at the beginning of each session. The initial schedule used was an FR1 schedule of reinforcement and it was increased to an FR2 schedule once animals successfully administered an additional nine infusions within the 3-h session. Animals were then again required to self-administer an additional nine infusions within a 3-h session under the FR2 schedule. This procedure kept exposure to amphetamine during self-administration training to a minimum so as not to compromise existing group differences in nicotine exposure history (Table 1; Vezina, 2004; Vezina et al, 2002). Animals that did not satisfy each of the FR1 and the FR2 criteria within 5 days were excluded from the study. Six rats were thus excluded (NIC HOME CAGE, 1; SAL HOME CAGE, 1; NIC SA CHAMBER, 1; NIC PAIRED, 1; NIC UNPAIRED, 2). Days to satisfaction of the training criteria were recorded.

Table 1 Mean±SEM Days to Criterion for Amphetamine Self-Administration Training

Amphetamine self-administration testing

Upon satisfactory completion of self-administration training under the FR schedules, rats were tested daily under a progressive ratio (PR) schedule of reinforcement for 6 days as described previously (Vezina et al, 2002). Under this schedule, the number of responses required to obtain each successive infusion of amphetamine was determined by ROUND(5XEXP(0.25 × infusion number)–5) to produce the following sequence of required lever presses: 1, 3, 6, 9, 12, 17, 24, 32, 42, 56, 73, 95, 124, 161, 208, and so on (Richardson and Roberts, 1996). In preliminary experiments using this exponential function with the dose of amphetamine self-administered in the present experiments (100 μg/kg/infusion, IV), it was found that the time of the last infusion occurred on average in <2 h. The daily PR sessions were thus terminated after 3-h or after 1-h elapsed without a drug infusion. Priming amphetamine infusions were not given during these sessions. The number of lever presses and infusions obtained in each PR session was recorded.

Extinction

Starting the day after the amphetamine self-administration testing phase, all rats were subjected to daily extinction sessions during which amphetamine was withheld and the cue light alone was presented under the PR schedule. Each session lasted 3-h or less if 1-h elapsed without presentation of the cue light. The extinction criterion was set at 10 or fewer lever presses in each of two consecutive sessions. After meeting this criterion or after undergoing 20 daily extinction sessions, rats were moved on to the final phase of the experiment, testing for amphetamine-primed reinstatement. The number of lever presses emitted during each session and the number of sessions required to meet the extinction criterion were recorded. Nine rats were excluded because of illness or dislocated head mounts during this phase (NIC HOME CAGE, 3; SAL HOME CAGE, 3; NIC SA CHAMBER, 1; SAL SA CHAMBER, 1; NIC UNPAIRED, 1).

Test for amphetamine-primed reinstatement

This test was conducted on the day following the last extinction session. On this test, rats were first subjected to a 3-h extinction session in which both amphetamine and cue light presentations were withheld. They were then tested for reinstatement primed by amphetamine injections (0, 0.20, 0.40, and 0.75 mg/kg, IP) during four 1-h sessions each separated by 5 min. Saline and amphetamine injections were administered just before the 1-h test sessions in an ascending order to minimize carry-over effects of residual amphetamine. All lever presses on this test were without consequence. The number of lever presses on the third hour of the extinction session and during each subsequent 1-h priming session was recorded. Three final rats were excluded because of illness or dislocated head mounts during this phase (SAL SA CHAMBER, 2; NIC UNPAIRED, 1).

Data Analyses

The data obtained in the three different experiments (HOME CAGE exposure; SA CHAMBER exposure; PAIRED/UNPAIRED exposure) were analyzed separately. Independent samples t-tests were used to analyze days to satisfaction of the training criteria during amphetamine self-administration training as well as number of days to meet the extinction criterion and number of lever presses emitted on the last day of extinction. Between-within ANOVA with exposure (two levels: nicotine and saline or nicotine paired and unpaired) as the between factor and test days (6 days) as the within factor were used to analyze the number of infusions obtained on the six amphetamine self-administration test days. Similarly, between-within ANOVA with exposure (two levels: nicotine and saline or nicotine paired and unpaired) as the between factor and hours of testing (5 h) as the within factor were used to analyze the number of lever presses emitted on the 5-h test for amphetamine-primed reinstatement. When required, post-hoc comparisons were made using the Scheffé test according to Kirk (1968). Finally, the proportion of rats that failed to meet the extinction criterion after 20 sessions was calculated for the different exposure groups and compared in each experiment using the χ2 test for two independent samples.

RESULTS

Amphetamine Self-Administration Training

In agreement with previous results obtained following amphetamine exposure (Lorrain et al, 2000; Vezina et al, 2002), rats exposed to nicotine either in the home cage or the self-administration chambers did not differ from their saline-exposed counterparts in days to criterion during amphetamine self-administration training. Similarly, rats exposed to nicotine either paired or unpaired with the self-administration chambers did not differ from one another (Table 1).

Amphetamine Self-Administration Testing

Exposure to nicotine enhanced the subsequent self-administration of amphetamine under the PR schedule of reinforcement but only when rats were tested in the chamber they were previously exposed to nicotine in. In addition, rats exposed to nicotine explicitly unpaired with these chambers failed to show enhanced amphetamine self-administration (Figure 2). Analysis by ANOVA of the number of amphetamine infusions obtained over the 6-test days revealed no significant effects when rats were administered their exposure injections in the home cage (Figure 2a). When exposure injections were administered in the self-administration chambers (Figure 2b), nicotine-exposed rats obtained significantly more amphetamine infusions over the 6 days of testing than their saline-exposed counterparts (F(1, 15)=8.93, p<0.01). Finally, rats exposed to nicotine explicitly unpaired with the self-administration chambers (Figure 2c) obtained significantly fewer infusions compared with rats exposed to nicotine paired with the chambers (F(1, 15)=6.06, p<0.05) and showed levels of responding comparable to rats exposed to saline either in the home cage or the self-administration chambers (Figures 2a and b).

Figure 2
figure 2

Previous exposure to nicotine in the drug-taking environment enhances the subsequent self-administration of amphetamine under a PR schedule of reinforcement. Data are shown as mean (±SEM) number of amphetamine infusions obtained over the 6 days of testing. The cumulative number of lever presses required to obtain these infusions is also shown. The bar graphs (means+SEM) in the insets were derived from the means of the values obtained for each subject on each of the six PR test days. Numbers at the bottom of each bar indicate n per group. Exposure injections were made in the home cage (a), the self-administration (SA) chambers with the levers retracted (b), or either explicitly paired or unpaired with the SA chambers (c). *p<0.05, compared with NIC UNPAIRED. **p<0.01, compared with saline exposed.

PowerPoint slide

Extinction

Figure 3 illustrates the effects of previous exposure to nicotine on the extinction of amphetamine self-administration. Consistent with the effects observed during amphetamine self-administration testing under the PR schedule, nicotine exposure rendered rats resistant to extinction. Unlike the above findings, however, this effect was observed regardless of nicotine exposure context. Relative to their saline-exposed controls, rats exposed to nicotine in the home cage (Figures 3a and d) showed a greater number of days to meet the extinction criterion (t(21)=1.98, p<0.05) and a greater number of lever presses on the last day of extinction (t(21)=2.40, p<0.05). Rats exposed to nicotine in the self-administration chambers (Figures 3b and e) exhibited similar effects relative to their saline-exposed controls, showing a nonsignificant trend for more days to extinction (t(13)=1.57, p=0.071) and significantly more lever presses on the last day of extinction (t(13)=1.99, p<0.05). No significant group differences were observed in either measure in rats exposed to nicotine paired or unpaired with the self-administration chambers (Figures 3c and f). Rats in these groups showed levels of responding similar to the above nicotine-exposed rats. χ2 analyses of the proportion of rats failing to meet the extinction criterion in the different groups revealed a significantly greater number of failures in nicotine (4/10) relative to saline (1/13) exposed rats (p<0.05) in the home cage exposure condition. Similarly, relative to their saline exposure controls (2/8), significantly more nicotine-exposed rats (5/7) failed to meet this criterion (p<0.01) in the self-administration chamber exposure condition. Rats in the Paired and Unpaired nicotine exposure groups did not differ from each other in this measure.

Figure 3
figure 3

Previous exposure to nicotine rendered rats resistant to extinction regardless of nicotine exposure context. Data are shown as mean (+SEM) number of days in extinction training (a–c) and presses on the last day of extinction (d–f). Exposure injections were made in the home cage (a, d), the SA chambers (b, e), or either explicitly paired or unpaired with the SA chambers (c, f). Numbers at the bottom of each bar indicate n per group. *p<0.05, compared with saline exposed.

PowerPoint slide

Test for Amphetamine-Primed Reinstatement

Similar to what was observed during amphetamine self-administration testing, exposure to nicotine enhanced the ability of amphetamine priming injections to reinstate drug seeking but only when rats were tested in the environment in which they were previously exposed to nicotine. Again, amphetamine-induced reinstatement was not enhanced when rats were tested in chambers explicitly unpaired with nicotine during exposure (Figure 4). Analysis by ANOVA of the number of lever presses emitted on this test showed a significant effect of priming injections under all context exposure conditions: home cage (F(4, 84)=4.54, p<0.01), self-administration chamber (F(4, 44)=4.76, p<0.01), and paired/unpaired (F(4, 52)=10.71, p<0.001). Post-hoc Scheffé comparisons revealed that compared with levels observed in the last hour of extinction, the amphetamine priming injections significantly increased lever pressing in rats exposed to nicotine in the self-administration chambers (p<0.05) but not in the home cage. This effect was also observed in rats exposed to nicotine paired (p<0.01) but not in rats exposed to nicotine unpaired with the chambers.

Figure 4
figure 4

Previous exposure to nicotine in the drug-taking environment dose-dependently enhances subsequent amphetamine (AMPH)-primed reinstatement. Data are shown as mean (±SEM) number of lever presses during each 1-h segment of the 5-h reinstatement test. Nicotine exposure contexts are as described in Figure 2. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, compared with the last hour of the extinction component of this test as revealed by Scheffé post-hoc comparisons following ANOVA.

PowerPoint slide

DISCUSSION

In the present experiments, exposure to nicotine enhanced the subsequent self-administration of amphetamine under a PR schedule, rendered rats resistant to extinction when amphetamine was withheld, and enhanced the reinstatement of amphetamine-primed drug seeking. These results show that previous exposure to nicotine can enhance the incentive motivational effects of other psychostimulants like amphetamine. The finding that amphetamine self-administration and reinstatement were enhanced only when rats were tested in the chamber in which they were previously exposed to nicotine indicates a critical role for nicotine-paired contextual stimuli in the mediation of these effects.

Repeated intermittent exposure to nicotine, a pattern often associated with initial exposure to tobacco, leads to sensitization of its locomotor and nucleus accumbens DA-activating effects (Iyaniwura et al, 2001; Ksir et al, 1985; Schoffelmeer et al, 2002; Shim et al, 2001) and has been shown to produce locomotor cross-sensitization to amphetamine (Birrell and Balfour, 1998; Schoffelmeer et al, 2002). The present results extend these findings to include the enhancement of amphetamine self-administration by previous nicotine exposure. They are consistent with incentive motivational views of enhanced drug taking (Robinson and Berridge, 1993; Stewart et al, 1984) and with a number of reports showing that exposure to a regimen of drug injections known to sensitize midbrain DA neuron reactivity leads to a long-lasting increase in the predisposition to pursue these and other drugs (Vezina, 2004; Vezina et al, 2007). Importantly, exposure to a drug like Δ9-THC that does not alter subsequent nucleus accumbens DA overflow in response to amphetamine does not enhance the subsequent self-administration of amphetamine (Cortright et al, 2011). Together, these results support suggestions by some that exposure to nicotine may induce changes in the mesoaccumbens DA system (Kandel et al, 1994) that increase individuals' predisposition to pursue it and other drugs (Kandel, 2002; Kandel et al, 1992; Levine et al, 2011; Weinberger and Sofuoglu, 2009).

The ability of contextual stimuli previously associated with a drug to regulate the expression of sensitized locomotion and nucleus accumbens DA overflow by nicotine (Reid et al, 1996, 1998) and amphetamine (Anagnostaras and Robinson, 1996; Guillory et al, 2006; Stewart and Vezina, 1988; Vezina and Leyton, 2009), and locomotor cross-sensitization from one drug to another (Stewart and Vezina, 1987; Vezina et al, 1989) is well established. The present findings show that such contextual stimuli can also regulate the expression of enhanced drug self-administration and the reinstatement of drug seeking. They are consistent with those of Horger et al (1992) who reported that rats exposed to nicotine in the self-administration chamber subsequently show enhanced drug self-administration in this environment and provide an explanation for the failure of others to observe this effect (Adriani et al, 2003; McQuown et al, 2007; Shoaib et al, 1997). In these latter reports, rats were exposed to nicotine in their home cage and were subsequently tested in the self-administration chambers where they did not show enhanced drug self-administration. Together, these results suggest that contextual stimuli previously paired with nicotine can enhance the incentive motivational effects of other psychostimulants and thus promote enhanced drug self-administration and reinstatement of drug seeking. Indeed, such contextual stimuli are well known to increase craving to smoke in humans (Conklin, 2006). As noted earlier, multiple factors can influence the extent to which previous exposure to nicotine can affect subsequent drug-induced responding. The different nicotine exposure regimens and withdrawal times used in the present experiments and above reports likely contributed in different ways. The present findings indicate that nicotine-paired contextual stimuli can be critical determinants of the effects ultimately observed.

The prevailing evidence suggests that contextual stimuli paired or unpaired with drug exposure can regulate the expression of sensitization by respectively acting as facilitators to enable it (Anagnostaras and Robinson, 1996) or as CIs to suppress it (Stewart, 1992; Stewart and Vezina, 1988; see Vezina and Leyton, 2009). Our results suggest that both mechanisms may have participated in the present experiments. The self-administration chambers clearly enabled the expression of enhanced amphetamine self-administration and reinstatement in rats previously exposed to nicotine in this context (NIC SA CHAMBER); these effects were not observed when the nicotine-paired stimuli were not present (in rats exposed to nicotine in the home cage; NIC HOME CAGE). It is unlikely that the enhanced responding observed was due to the accrual of excitatory contextual conditioning of drug-taking responses by nicotine and their summation with the amphetamine-induced behaviors. Rats were trained to emit a novel response (lever press) to self-administer amphetamine and this was done 2 weeks after exposure to nicotine. In addition, there was no evidence that prior exposure to nicotine or nicotine-paired stimuli enhanced conditioning of the lever press response (Olausson et al, 2003) as no group differences were detected in its acquisition. The discrimination learning procedure used in the NIC PAIRED/UNPAIRED experiment is known to establish a contextual stimulus complex (the self-administration chamber) not only as a conditioned facilitator when it is explicitly paired with the drug in the case of the Paired rats but also as a CI when it is explicitly unpaired with the drug in the case of Unpaired rats (Mackintosh, 1974; Vezina and Leyton, 2009). The lack of enhanced amphetamine self-administration and reinstatement observed in rats exposed to nicotine explicitly unpaired with the self-administration chambers in the present experiment could thus also have resulted from inhibition of these effects by the CI properties of these contextual stimuli. This possibility is supported by reports showing that removal of this inhibition allows the expression of sensitized responding in rats tested in the previously drug unpaired environment (Anagnostaras et al, 2002; Stewart and Vezina, 1991). Together, these results show powerful excitatory and inhibitory associative control over the expression of enhanced drug taking and seeking with important implications for the treatment of addictions in humans.

Repeated intermittent drug exposure provides many opportunities for the formation of associations between the drug and multiple environmental stimuli. The present findings stress the need to identify the relevant associations, determine how they interact with other factors, and decipher how they can subsequently regulate behavior. Drug-context associations may be particularly important regulators of drug-taking and -seeking behaviors. For example, while locomotor sensitization by nicotine and its cross-sensitization to amphetamine can be expressed in a context-independent manner (Schoffelmeer et al, 2002), nicotine-associated contextual stimuli were found to be necessary for the expression of enhanced amphetamine self-administration and amphetamine-primed reinstatement in the present experiments. This does not preclude the possibility that nicotine also influences self-administration behaviors in a manner independent of contextual conditioning. Indeed, the ability of nicotine to interact associatively and non-associatively with non-pharmacological stimuli has been described (Caggiula et al, 2009) and exposure to nicotine in the home cage has been shown to enhance lever pressing for a tone-light conditioned reinforcer (Olausson et al, 2004). Consistent with these results, previous exposure to nicotine enhanced responding for the amphetamine-paired cue light during extinction in the present experiments regardless of nicotine exposure context. Although these non-associative effects clearly provide an important mechanism by which nicotine exposure can affect motivated behaviors, it remains to be determined how they relate to the associative effects regulating drug self-administration and reinstatement observed in the present experiments, what potentially independent substrates underlie the two types of effects, and how these are differentially recruited. Interestingly, unlike what is seen in adults, rats exposed to nicotine as adolescents appear more sensitive to the drug but less sensitive to contextual stimuli when subsequently tested for drug self-administration (Adriani et al, 2003; McQuown et al, 2007). This may reflect a critical developmental factor putting younger individuals at greater risk for later unregulated vulnerability for addiction. In individuals exposed to nicotine as adults, drug-paired and -unpaired contextual stimuli may thus have a more important role to enable or inhibit enhanced responding for nicotine and other drugs.