INTRODUCTION

Startle can be reduced when the startling stimulus is preceded by a weak nonstartling prepulse (Ison and Hammond, 1971; Graham, 1975), in a process called prepulse inhibition (PPI). The degree to which the weak sensory prepulse inhibits the motor reflex in PPI is used as a measure of sensorimotor gating. PPI is disrupted in specific neuropsychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia (Braff et al, 1978; cf. Braff et al, 2001), Tourette syndrome (Castellanos et al, 1996), obsessive compulsive disorder (Swerdlow et al, 1993; Hoenig et al, 2005), Huntington's disease (Swerdlow et al, 1995), and Fragile X syndrome (Frankland et al, 2004).

To clarify the pathophysiology of deficient sensorimotor gating in these disorders, one strategy has been to investigate the effects of pharmacological manipulations on PPI in normal human and animal models. For example, many studies document the PPI-disruptive effects of dopamine (DA) agonists on PPI in rodents and normal humans (Mansbach et al, 1988; Hutchison and Swift, 1999; cf. Swerdlow et al, 2008). Even within this apparently consistent literature, there is clear evidence of cross-species differences. For example, the DAergic regulation of PPI may involve different DA receptor subtypes in rats vs mice (Ralph and Caine, 2005). Furthermore, in normal humans, the effects of DAergic manipulations on PPI may differ across subgroups characterized by low vs high baseline PPI levels (Swerdlow et al, 2003; Bitsios et al, 2005), and/or distinct personality dimensions (Hutchison et al, 1999; Swerdlow et al, 2006; Talledo et al, 2008).

Perhaps even more profound cross-species differences have been identified in the NMDA regulation of PPI. In rats, PPI is reduced by noncompetitive NMDA receptor antagonists, such as dizoclipine, ketamine and phencyclidine (Mansbach and Geyer, 1989; Mansbach and Geyer, 1991; Martinez et al, 1999, 2000). However, in healthy humans, ketamine has been shown to cause either an increase of PPI at short prepulse intervals (30 ms (Duncan et al, 2001; Abel et al, 2003), or have no significant effect on PPI (van Berckel et al, 1998). A similar PPI-enhancing effect was reported by Swerdlow et al (2002) in normal subjects who were administered the mixed NMDA antagonist/DA agonist, amantadine.

To better understand the role of NMDA neurotransmission in the normal regulation of PPI in humans, we tested the effects of the low-to-moderate affinity NMDA-receptor antagonist memantine on PPI. Memantine was chosen because it is well tolerated (compared with ketamine), has linear kinetics, 100% bioavailability after oral administration and relative NMDA-receptor selectivity (Wilcock et al, 2002; Areosa et al, 2005; Cummings, 2004). Memantine does not cause sensory or dissociative symptoms similar to those reported in past PPI studies with ketamine (Duncan et al, 2001; Abel et al, 2003), and which might potentially complicate the interpretation of changes in PPI. Wiley et al (2003) reported that in rats, memantine produced a dose-dependent reduction in PPI, using 100 ms prepulse intervals; initial studies here were designed to extend these studies of memantine in rats, across the range of prepulse intervals (10–120 ms) with which PPI was measured in humans. These studies next examined the effects of two doses of memantine (20 and 30 mg) on PPI in normal men. On the basis of several reports with DA agonists, this study also assessed the possible relationship between baseline PPI levels, normal personality dimensions, and sensitivity to the effects of memantine on PPI.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

The methods used in this study were approved by the UCSD Human Subject Institutional Review Board (human studies) or were in accordance with the National Institute of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (NIH publication no. 80-23) and approved by the UCSD Animal Subjects Committee (protocol no. S01221 (rodent studies)).

Rodent Pilot Testing

To confirm and extend findings of PPI-disruptive effects of memantine in rats (Wiley et al, 2003), adult male SD (n=13) rats (225–250 g; Harlan Laboratories, San Diego, CA) were housed in groups of 2–3 animals per cage, and maintained on a reversed light/dark schedule with water and food available ad libitum. Rats were handled within 2 days of arrival. Testing occurred during the dark phase. Memantine HCl was dissolved in saline vehicle and administered s.c. in doses of 0, 10 or 20 mg/kg (comparable to Wiley et al (2003)). Startle chambers (San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA) were housed in a sound-attenuated room, and consisted of a plexiglas cylinder 8.2 cm in diameter resting on a 12.5 × 25.5-cm plexiglas frame within a ventilated enclosure. Noise bursts were presented through a speaker mounted 24 cm above the cylinder. A piezoelectric accelerometer mounted below the plexiglas frame transduced motion within the cylinder. Stimulus delivery was controlled by the SR-LAB microcomputer and interface assembly, which also digitized (0-4095), rectified, and recorded stabilimeter readings. A total of 100 readings of 1 ms in duration were collected at the beginning of stimulus onset. Startle amplitude was defined as the average of the 100 readings.

Approximately 7 days after shipment arrival, rats were exposed to a short ‘matching’ startle session. They were placed in the startle chambers for a 5-min acclimation period with a 70 dB(A) background noise, and then exposed to a total of 17 P-ALONE trails (40 ms—120 dB(A) noise bursts) that were interspersed with three PP12 dB+P-ALONE trials (P-ALONE preceded 100 ms (onset-to-onset) by a 20 ms noise burst of 12 dB above background). Rats were assigned to dose groups based on average %PPI from the matching session to ensure similar baseline PPI levels between groups. Four days later, rats were injected with memantine (0, 10, or 20 mg/kg), and 30 min later were placed in the startle chambers for a 5-min acclimation period with a 70-dB(A) background noise. They were then exposed to a series of trial types, which were presented in pseudorandom order: (1) P-ALONE (40 ms, 120 dB(A) noise burst); or P-ALONE preceded 10, 20, 30, 60, or 120 ms (onset-to-onset) by a 5-ms noise 15 dB over background. Interspersed between these trials were trials in which no stimulus was presented, but cage displacement was measured (NOSTIM trials). The session began and ended with three consecutive P-ALONE trials; between these trials were six presentations of each trial type, in pseudorandom order. Intertrial intervals were variable and averaged 15 s. NOSTIM trials were not included in the calculation of intertrial intervals. Total session duration was 15.5 min.

Human Participants

A total of 40 healthy right-handed male healthy volunteers participated in the study. Female subjects were not included in the study because PPI is sexually dimorphic and fluctuates across the menstrual cycle. (Swerdlow et al, 1997; Jovanovic et al, 2004; Aasen et al, 2005). Exclusion criteria are outlined in Table 1 .

Table 1 Exclusion Criteria

Subjects who passed a preliminary phone checklist were invited for an in-person screening session that included a urine toxicology test (exclusion for any recreational drug), hearing test (Saico Audiometer, Assens Denmark; excluded for threshold >40 dB(A) at 1000 Hz), and a startle reflex test (exclusion for mean startle magnitude <50 U). During the screening, subjects also completed several questionnaires related to ethnicity, sexual preference, smoking, and caffeine consumption. Personality traits were quantified using the tridimensional personality questionnaire (TPQ; Cloninger et al, 1991) and the sensation seeking scale (SSS; Zuckerman et al, 1974).

For measures of acoustic startle and PPI, Ag/AgCl electrodes (In vivo metric model E220X-LS) were positioned over and lateral to the orbicularis oculi underneath both eyes. A ground electrode was placed behind the left ear (R<10 kΩ). Eyeblink magnitude was measured using EMG recordings (San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA). EMG activity was band-pass filtered (1–1000 Hz) and 60 Hz notch filtered, digitized, and 250 readings of 1 ms were recorded at the starting of the startle stimulus onset (1.22 μV/U). The startle test started with 3 min of 70 dB(A) white noise, which persisted as a background noise during the test session. During acclimation, blinks were counted by trained observers using a radio shack security camera and monitor (interobserver R=0.97). The startle session consisted of 42 trials, with six conditions: a 118 dB(A) white noise burst of 40 ms (pulse alone) and the same burst preceded 10, 20, 30, 60, and 120 ms by a prepulse of 16 dB above the background. To measure startle habituation, three pulse alone trials were presented at the session beginning and end. The variable intertrial interval averaged 15 s (range=10–20 s). Total test time was 15 min.

Participants who met study inclusion criteria returned for testing after 6–10 days, and for a second test one week later. Testing was performed double blind and drug order was randomized. Before the test days, subjects were instructed to maintain their normal patterns of caffeine intake before the testing, based on the effects of caffeine withdrawal on PPI and subjective rating scales. (Swerdlow et al, 2000). After arrival at 0830 h on the test day, subjects were given a standardized breakfast, followed by a urine toxicology test and a hearing test. At 0900 h, either memantine hydrochloride (20 or 30 mg) or placebo was administered. For the first 20 subjects, the active dose of memantine hydrochloride was 20 mg. For the next 20 subjects, the active dose of memantine hydrochloride was 30 mg. Heart rate was measured manually and blood pressure was measured by brachial cuff. Symptom rating scales (SRS) were assessed 30, 90, 150, 200, and 230 min after pill consumption. This SRS is a visual analog scale used to assess the physical and emotional states of the subjects during testing. Each symptom was rated on a piece of paper, using a 100-mm line to separate extreme ratings (‘not true’ vs ‘true’). The subjects rated emotional state (‘happy’), somatic symptoms (‘queasy’ and ‘dizzy’), and consciousness (‘drowsy’). The marks were manually measured by observers who were blind to the drug condition. At 1130 h, the subjects received a standardized lunch. Startle testing began 210 min after pill administration, based on the reported pharmacokinetics of memantine in normal humans (Sonkusare et al, 2005), using a test session identical to that used the screening day. Additional psychophysiological measures were completed after this startle test, which will be analyzed separately.

The primary dependent measures for rodent and human studies were startle magnitude and PPI. PPI was defined as (100−(100 × magnitude on prepulse trial/magnitude on pulse alone trials)). Planned comparisons utilized drug and prepulse interval as within-subject factors, with separate analyses completed for the 20 mg and 30 mg doses of memantine in humans. Additional planned comparisons in humans analyzed the impact of baseline (screening) PPI levels and specific personality measures (novelty seeking (NS) based on the total NS score from the TPQ, and SSS, based on the total SSS score) on memantine PPI effects. These analyses utilized categorical factors (‘low’ vs ‘high’ based on median splits) within ANOVAs, and continuous comparisons within regression analyses. Self-rating scales and autonomic measures at baseline (before pill administration) and immediately before startle testing were treated as continuous variables and analyzed by ANOVA as ‘change scores’ (difference from baseline); α was 0.05. Additional exploratory analyses were conducted, with α corrected for multiple comparisons. When average startle magnitude was <10 U during testing, subjects were excluded as ‘nonresponders’; this resulted in the exclusion of one subject in the 20 mg memantine sample and two subjects in the 30 mg memantine sample.

RESULTS

Rodent Pilot Study

Memantine caused a dose-dependent reduction in PPI for 60–120 ms prepulse intervals, confirming the findings of Wiley et al (2003). However, at short prepulse intervals (10–20 ms), the low dose of memantine increased PPI (Figure 1). ANOVA of %PPI revealed a significant main effect of memantine (F=4.27, d.f. 2,10, p<0.05) and prepulse interval (F=5.08, d.f. 4,40, p<0.03), but no significant dose x interval interaction (F=1.32, d.f. 8,40, ns). Post hoc comparisons confirmed an overall reduction in PPI after 20 mg memantine compared with vehicle (p<0.035). One explicit goal of this exploratory study was to examine memantine effects across the range of prepulse intervals, and a separate comparison of vehicle vs 10 mg memantine confirmed a dose × interval interaction (F=3.66, d.f. 4,28, p<0.02), reflecting memantine-induced increases in PPI over the 10–20 ms intervals, with the opposite effect at longer intervals (Figure 1). There were no effects of memantine on startle magnitude (F<1; Figure 1, inset) or reflex habituation (not shown).

Figure 1
figure 1

Pilot data to confirm and extend previous evidence of PPI-disruptive effects of memantine in rats (Wiley et al, 2003). Memantine produced a dose-dependent reduction in PPI with 60–120 ms intervals. However, at short intervals (10–20 ms), PPI was both increased (10 mg/kg) and decreased (20 mg/kg) by memantine. Neither dose significantly altered startle magnitude (inset).

Human Study

Subject characteristics

Human subject characteristics for the final cohorts in the 20 and 30 mg studies are shown in Table 2 . There were no differences in age, weight, ethnicity, or caffeine consumption (p>0.10) across dose groups; one subject in the 20 mg study and two subjects in the 30 mg study were smokers.

Table 2 Final Sample Characteristics

Personality questionnaire scores are shown in Table 3 . ANOVA for analyses personality questionnaire scores showed no difference between samples tested with 20 vs 30 mg memantine, with the exception of the TPQ subscale ‘harm avoidance’. The values for this personality subscale were significantly higher for 30 mg than for 20 mg cohorts (F=7.65, p<0.015). Score ranges were consistent with those published in large scale studies from other groups (Earleywine et al, 1992; Eisenberg et al, 2007) and from our own group (Swerdlow et al, 2002).

Table 3 Personality Questionnaire Scores

Subjective and physiological measures

An increase in self-reported level of happiness at the time of startle testing was detected after administration of 20 mg memantine (F=4.80, d.f. 1,18, p<0.042), but not after the 30 mg dose (F<1). This effect at 20 mg reflected a part of the sustained increase in ‘happy’ ratings, from the initial evaluation (30 min after pill), extending to the evaluation that followed the startle test (F=5.10, d.f. 1,18, p<0.04 for 30–230 min after pill; Figure 2a). Increased dizziness ratings were evident at the time of startle testing with 30 mg memantine (F=7.27, d.f. 1,17, p<0.016), but not with the 20 mg dose (F=2.58, d.f. 1,18, ns). This effect at 30 mg reflected a part of the sustained increase in ‘dizzy’ ratings, extending from the third assessment (150 min after pill) to the evaluation after startle testing (F=9.23, d.f. 1,17, p<0.008 for 30–230 min after pill; Figure 2b). No significant changes in self-rated queasiness or drowsiness were detected at the time of startle testing with either dose of memantine (not shown). In general, subjective ratings of memantine effects (‘happy’ or ‘dizzy’) during the time of startle testing were not related to personality measures (data not shown).

Figure 2
figure 2

Evidence of bioactivity of 20 mg (left figures) and 30 mg (right figures) memantine. The 20 mg dose increased ratings of ‘happy’ (a) and reduced spontaneous blink rates (c); the 30 mg dose increased ratings of ‘dizzy’ (b). Each of these effects was statistically reliable immediately before startle testing (*).

Autonomic measures were not significantly changed by either dose of memantine. ANOVAs of heart rate, systolic, and diastolic blood pressure immediately before startle testing revealed no significant effects of either 20 mg memantine (F=1.25, <1 and 1.07, respectively; ns) or 30 mg memantine (F=3.43, <1 and 1.72, respectively; ns). Spontaneous blink rate immediately before startle testing was significantly reduced after 20 mg memantine (F=4.75, d.f. 1,18, p<0.045), but only a weak trend towards reduced blink rate was detected after 30 mg memantine (F=1.41, d.f. 1.17, ns).

Startle magnitude and habituation

ANOVAs revealed no significant effect of 20 or 30 mg memantine on startle magnitude (F<1, both comparisons). In each case, startle magnitude was greater for right eye than left (F=9.26, d.f. 1,18 p<0.008 and F=19.96, d.f. 1,17, p<0.0004 for 20 and 30 mg doses, respectively), but there were no significant interactions of dose × eye side (F<1 and 1.37, respectively). Similarly, ANOVAs revealed no significant drug effects on reflex habituation, with significant effects of trial block for 20 and 30 mg memantine tests (F=41.91 and 36.28, respectively), but no significant main effects of drug dose (F<1 and 1.20, respectively) or significant dose × block interaction (F<1).

Figure 3
figure 5

Startle magnitude on pulse alone trials during PPI testing (a), and at the beginning and end of each test session (b) were not significantly affected by either dose of memantine. Similarly, reflex habituation (response decrement from session beginning to end) was not affected by memantine (b).

PPI

ANOVAs of PPI yielded different outcomes for the 20 and 30 mg doses of memantine (Figure 4a). For the 20 mg dose, ANOVA revealed no significant main effect of dose (F<1), a significant main effect of prepulse interval (F=33.62, d.f. 4,72, p<0.0001), and a significant interaction of dose x interval (F=2.70, d.f. 4,72, p<0.038). Post hoc comparisons revealed significant memantine-induced increases in PPI at 120 ms prepulse intervals (p<0.025). A memantine-induced reduction in PPI at 10 ms intervals was not statistically significant (F=1.61), and achieved only a moderate effect size (d=0.42). For the 30 mg dose of memantine, ANOVA revealed no significant main effect of dose (F<1), a significant effect of prepulse interval (F=21.07, d.f. 4,68, p<0.0001), and no significant dose × interval interaction (F<1). For both 20 and 30 mg doses, ANOVAs of PPI noted no significant effects of eye side (F<1) or significant interactions of eye side with dose or interval; as a result, eye side was not entered as a factor in subsequent ANOVAs.

Figure 4
figure 3

Effects of memantine on PPI. (a) PPI was significantly increased by 20 mg memantine at 120 ms prepulse intervals (left), but was not significantly affected by 30 mg memantine (right). (b) When subjects in the 20 mg study were divided into those in the upper or lower 50% of screening PPI values (left figure, ‘high’ and ‘low’, respectively), ANOVA revealed PPI-enhancing effects of memantine only among the ‘low gaters’. PPI-reducing effects of memantine in ‘high gaters’ at short intervals did not reach statistical significance. (c) A similar ‘low vs high gater’ split uncovered no significant effects of 30 mg memantine on PPI.

As planned, we next assessed the relationship between baseline PPI and memantine-induced changes in PPI, based on earlier reports of differential drug effects on PPI in low- vs high-gating individuals (Swerdlow et al, 2002, 2006; Bitsios et al, 2005; Csomor et al, 2008). Because different studies have used slightly different definitions of ‘baseline PPI’, we examined this relationship in a number of ways. For example, Bitsios et al (2005) distinguished ‘low- vs high-gating’ subgroups based on mean screening PPI for 80 ms prepulse intervals. Because no 80 ms interval condition was included in this study, we used a similar approach, based on the averaged screening values for 60 and 120 ms prepulse intervals. Csomor et al (2008) stratified subjects based on vehicle-PPI levels for 60 ms prepulse intervals, and we (Swerdlow et al, 2002) had previously stratified subjects based on mean screening PPI values across 10–120 ms prepulse intervals. Importantly, all of these strategies yielded comparable outcomes with the present data; for simplicity, we will report the statistical outcomes only for analyses using the methods of Bitsios et al (2005).

For tests with 20 mg memantine (Figure 4b), ANOVA of PPI revealed significantly higher PPI levels in ‘high vs ‘low gaters’ (reflecting the fact that screening PPI levels (4A, left) significantly predict testing PPI levels; F=8.70, d.f. 1,17, p<0.01), no overall main effect of drug dose (F<1), a significant interaction of drug × ‘gating level’ (F=4.96, d.f. 1,17, p<0.04), a significant main effect of prepulse interval (F=44.12, d.f. 4,68, p<0.0001), and significant interactions of dose × interval (F=2.75, d.f. 4,68, p<0.04) and ‘gating level’ × interval (F=5.88, d.f. 4,68, p<0.0005). On the basis of the interaction of drug × gating levels, separate ANOVAs were conducted in each gating subgroup that revealed a significant interaction of drug × prepulse interval in ‘high gaters’ (F=2.66, d.f. 4,36, p<0.05), but not ‘low gaters’ (F=1.14, d.f. 4,32, ns). Inspection of the data (Figure 4b) revealed that memantine increased PPI in the ‘low-gating’ group across all intervals, but particularly at the 120 ms prepulse interval (F=11.44, d.f. 1,9, p<0.009), and that it tended to reduce PPI in the ‘high-gating’ group at the 10–20 ms prepulse intervals (d=0.75–0.77).

For tests with 30 mg memantine (Figure 4c), ANOVA of PPI confirmed higher PPI levels in ‘high gaters’ (F=5.32, d.f. 1,16, p<0.035), no overall main effect of drug (F<1) or interaction of drug × ‘gating level’ (F<1), a significant effect of prepulse interval (F=23.42, d.f. 4,64, p<0.0001), and a significant interaction of gating level × interval (F=2.91, d.f. 4,64, p<0.03), but not gating level × interval × drug (F<1). Inspection of the data confirmed no differential impact of 30 mg memantine on low- vs high-gating normal subject groups.

Personality and memantine effects on PPI

As planned, on the basis of the evidence for greater drug effects on PPI associated with specific personality dimensions, we assessed the relationship between memantine effects on PPI and scores for NS (NS total score from the TPQ), sensation seeking (SSS total score), and disinhibition (DIS subscale from the SSS scale; Figure 5a and b). Importantly, these scores were interrelated: NS scores correlated significantly with SSS scores (R=0.64, p<0.004), but not DIS scores (R=0.17, ns), and total SSS scores correlated significantly with scores on its DIS subscale (R=0.49, p<0.035; in contrast, HA scores did not correlate significantly with any of these scales). Subgroups were formed based on median splits of NS and SSS scales, and the DIS subscale. We first examined the effect described above for the inclusive group of subjects tested with 20 mg memantine: a drug-induced increase in PPI at the 120 ms prepulse interval. Separate ANOVAs showed that this effect was evident in subgroups with the highest scores in NS (F=5.54, d.f. 1,8, p<0.05), SSS (F=13.50, d.f. 1,9, p<0.006) and DIS (F=7.75, d.f. 1,8, p<0.025), but not in subgroups with the lowest scores in these domains (F=1.40, <1, and <1, respectively). Looking beyond the 120 ms interval, DIS scores significantly predicted the overall impact of 20 mg memantine on PPI; ANOVA revealed a significant interaction of dose × DIS group (F=12.31, d.f. 1,17, p<0.003); a separate ANOVA in the high DIS subgroup revealed PPI-enhancing effects of memantine at all prepulse intervals (main effect of dose, F=6.90, d.f. 1,8, p<0.035), whereas a separate ANOVA in the low DIS group revealed significant PPI-reducing effects of memantine (F=5.38, d.f. 1,9, p<0.05) that were most prominent at the short prepulse intervals (dose × interval interaction: F=3.28, d.f. 4,36, p<0.025). As a result, DIS scores correlated significantly with the impact of memantine on PPI (calculated by the difference score (memantine−placebo) for mean %PPI across all intervals; R=0.63, p<0.009). Importantly, only the effects of memantine on PPI, and not baseline PPI levels per se, or post-placebo or -memantine levels of startle magnitude, differed among subgroups with low vs high NS, SSS, or DIS scores.

Figure 5
figure 4

Differential effects of memantine on PPI among subjects with low vs high scores in personality measures of novelty seeking (a), sensation seeking (b), and disinhibition (c). PPI-enhancing effects of memantine at 120 ms prepulse intervals were evident only among individuals with high NS (a) or SSS scores (b). Subjects with high DIS scores exhibited PPI-enhancing effects of memantine across all prepulse intervals, whereas subjects with low DIS scores exhibited the opposite effects, particularly at short prepulse intervals.

Similar analyses using data from the 30 mg memantine trial revealed no significant differences in memantine effects among subgroups with low vs high scores on NS, SSS, or DIS scales.

DISCUSSION

The main aim of this study was to assess the effects memantine on PPI, psychological, and somatic measures in clinically normal men. Initial pilot data obtained in rodents confirmed earlier reports of memantine's PPI-disruptive effects at longer prepulse intervals (Wiley et al (2003)), but also detected PPI-enhancing properties of memantine at short prepulse intervals, at a moderate dose of memantine, ie one that caused a submaximal disruption at long intervals. Thus, even with a cursory assessment, animal data suggested both dose- and interval-dependent effects of memantine on PPI, generally consistent with those previously observed with ketamine (Mansbach and Geyer, 1989).

In humans, we detected evidence for bioactivity of both doses of memantine at the time of startle testing: 20 mg increased ratings of happiness and reduced spontaneous blink rate, whereas 30 mg increased the ratings of dizziness. Interestingly, these drug effects did not overlap across doses: 30 mg memantine did not change ‘happiness’ ratings, and although 20 mg memantine may have generated some mild dizziness (Figure 2b) and 30 mg memantine may have slightly slowed spontaneous blinking (Figure 2c), neither of these effects approached statistical significance. In fact, beyond changes in happiness and blink rate (20 mg) and dizziness (30 mg), there were no significant drug effects on drowsiness, queasiness, autonomic measures, startle magnitude, or startle habituation.

These findings (and lack thereof) with subjective measures are in line with the effects of memantine reported in the literature. These drug effects on happiness, blink rate (20 mg memantine), and dizziness (30 mg memantine) were detected at a time point corresponding to peak memantine blood levels (Sonkusare et al, 2005). Euphoric effects were reported after 40 mg memantine in adults with a history of methamphetamine abuse, and in patients with Alzheimer's disease (Hart et al, 2002; cf. Areosa et al, 2005). Dizziness is one of the most common side effects of memantine in clinical trials (Wilcock et al, 2002, cf. Kumar, 2004). Changes in blink rate are often viewed as a marker of central DAergic tone (Karson, 1983); in our experience, however, this association can be complicated by levels of drowsiness (that can increase blink rates) or alertness (that can decrease blink rates).

Generally, consistent with earlier reports with NMDA antagonists, such as ketamine (Duncan et al, 2001; Abel et al, 2003) and amantadine (Swerdlow et al, 2002 (but see Bitsios et al, 2005)), 20 mg memantine significantly increased PPI at the 120 ms prepulse interval. In earlier reports, ketamine effects on PPI were either short lived (Duncan et al, 2001), or accompanied by reduced startle magnitude (Abel et al, 2003). Abel et al (2003) detected these PPI-enhancing effects of ketamine at both 30 and 120 ms prepulse intervals. Notably, the temporal pattern of memantine effects on PPI in humans contrasts sharply with that detected in rats: where humans exhibit maximal PPI enhancement (120 ms intervals), rats exhibit maximal PPI disruption; where rats exhibit maximal PPI enhancement (10–20 ms intervals), humans exhibit a trend towards PPI disruption.

On the basis of recent reports of differential drug effects on PPI between individuals with low vs high baseline PPI levels, we conducted separate analyses of 20 mg memantine effects on PPI in low vs high ‘gating’ subgroups. Memantine-induced increases in PPI were evident only in subjects ranked in the lowest 50% for baseline PPI; in this group, PPI-enhancing effects were apparent across the 10–120 ms range of prepulse intervals, but remained most robust at the 120 ms interval. In contrast, among ‘high-gating’ subjects, no PPI-enhancing effects of memantine were detected, and the opposite trend—decreased PPI—was apparent at short prepulse intervals.

These patterns of PPI-enhancement only in ‘low-gating’ subjects cannot be easily ascribed to a simple ‘regression to the mean’ of PPI levels. PPI (particularly at 60–120 ms intervals) has very high test–retest reliability, exceeding 0.80 over periods as long as 1 year (Cadenhead et al, 1999; Light et al, 2007). This makes it very unlikely that PPI levels would systematically fluctuate between ‘low’ and ‘high’ PPI levels across tests. More evidence that PPI-enhancing effects of memantine do not simply reflect an arithmetic consequence of low baseline PPI was seen when subgroups were formed based on specific personality domains. These subgroups did not differ significantly in baseline PPI, and yet PPI-enhancing effects of memantine at 120 ms prepulse intervals were detected only in subjects with the highest NS, SSS or DIS scores.

Low vs high gating and low vs high DIS subgroups differ not only in the ‘direction’ of memantine effects on PPI, but also in the prepulse intervals most sensitive to these effects. For example, although high DIS subjects exhibited PPI-enhancing effects of memantine across all prepulse intervals, low DIS subjects exhibited significant PPI-reducing effects of memantine only at the short prepulse intervals; a similar pattern characterized low- vs high-gating subgroups, respectively. In preclinical studies, short and long PPI intervals have been shown to be differentially sensitive to drug effects; indeed, this was the case in the present pilot study: short-interval PPI was increased by 10 mg/kg memantine, whereas at long PPI intervals, memantine produced only dose-dependent PPI-reducing effects. Some findings suggest differential neurochemical substrates regulating drug effects on short- and long-interval PPI, eg a prominent role of D1-like systems in regulating short-interval PPI, and D2-like systems in regulating long-interval PPI (Swerdlow et al, 2004). At a psychological level, short-interval PPI is linked to substrates regulating more automatic, preconscious inhibition, whereas long-interval PPI is linked to more volitional, consciously controlled inhibition (cf. Swerdlow et al, 2008). Conceivably, individuals distinguished by low vs high DIS scores, or high vs low baseline PPI, might be differentially sensitive to short- vs long-interval PPI effects based on intrinsic differences in both these neurochemical and/or psychological substrates.

Perhaps most striking is the fact that no significant changes in PPI were evident after 30 mg memantine, regardless of baseline PPI levels, or scores on NS, SSS, or DIS scales. Two parsimonious explanations for differences in memantine dose effects are (1) different sample characteristics and (2) a narrow range of dose sensitivity. Cohorts tested with 20 and 30 mg doses were recruited from the same populations, using identical inclusion/exclusion criteria, and did not differ significantly in age, ethnicity, education level, weight, caffeine use, scores for NS, SSS and DIS scales, and baseline PPI levels (p>0.05, both across all intervals, and for the critical mean of 60 and 120 ms intervals used for median split analyses). Groups did differ significantly in startle magnitude (see Figure 3) with lower startle magnitude exhibited by the 30-mg dose group. However, both the PPI-enhancing effects of 20 mg memantine and the lack thereof with 30 mg memantine were independent of startle magnitude: median split analyses revealed no significant interaction of low vs high startle magnitude and memantine dose effects with either 20 mg (F<1) or 30 mg (F<1) doses. Furthermore, startle magnitude was not related to low- vs high-gating group assignment for either dose of memantine, nor did inclusion of low vs high baseline startle magnitude as a grouping factor alter the critical main or interaction effects in these studies. Groups also differed significantly in HA scores (Table 3). However, a subgroup of subjects from the 30-mg dose group (n=12) with mean HA score (6.0 (SEM=0.88)) nearly identical to that of the 20-mg dose group (5.95 (SEM=1.25)) exhibited no significant effect of memantine dose on PPI (F<1), and no dose × interval interaction (F<1). Thus, group differences in HA scores do not appear to account for differential effects of 20 vs 30 mg memantine on PPI.

A narrow dose range of memantine effects on PPI in humans would be consistent with the present animal data for short prepulse intervals. Here, an ‘inverted-U’ dose effect was detected; the ‘low dose’ (10 mg/kg) increased PPI compared with vehicle, whereas the ‘high dose’ (20 mg/kg) reduced PPI compared with vehicle. Such a pattern suggests interactions among multiple substrates, either among different pharmacological effects of memantine (eg DA agonist as well as NMDA antagonist properties (Seeman et al, 2008)), or multiple brain systems regulating PPI. For example, we could speculate that the ratio of DA agonist: NMDA antagonist effects of memantine differs at these two doses, such that a predominant NMDA antagonism at the low dose leads to increased PPI, and more prominent DA agonist effects of the high dose opposes these effects of NMDA blockade. It would be less speculative to suggest that the overall effect of systemically administered memantine on PPI will reflect its integrated effects on different brain substrates. Animal studies suggest that different NMDA systems have opposing effects on PPI. For example, PPI is disrupted by infusion of NMDA into the ventral hippocampus (Wan et al, 1996, Zhang et al, 2002, Swerdlow et al, 2001) and by infusion of NMDA antagonists into the basolateral amygdala (Fendt et al, 2000). Thus, a narrow dose range for memantine-induced changes in PPI might reflect its simultaneous action within two or more brain systems with opposing effects on PPI.

It would be reasonable to ask whether different dose effects on PPI might relate to different effects of 20 vs 30 mg of memantine on subjective ratings in this study. Increased ‘happy’ scores after 20, but not 30 mg memantine, suggests that the lower dose had mild hedonic effects that might correspond to pro-DAergic properties of this drug (Seeman et al, 2008). However, PPI-enhancing effects of pro-DAergic drugs, such as amphetamine, are generally not reported in humans, except perhaps among low-gating subgroups (Swerdlow et al, 2008). Also arguing against a pro-DAergic mechanism for PPI-enhancing effects of memantine is the fact that high novelty-seeking subjects—who are most sensitive to the PPI-enhancing effects of memantine—are also most sensitive to the PPI-reducing effects of DA agonists (Hutchison et al, 1999; Swerdlow et al, 2008). Thus, it would be challenging to propose a parsimonious neurochemical connection between the hedonic and PPI-enhancing effects of 20 mg memantine.

The present data also make it hard to explain reduced PPI in schizophrenia, simply on the basis of reduced NMDA neurotransmission. Rather, the present evidence —together with the findings of increased PPI after ketamine and amantadine in normal comparison subjects—suggests that acute systemic blockade of NMDA does not reproduce the loss of sensorimotor gating exhibited by schizophrenia patients. If reduced NMDA neurotransmission contributes to PPI deficits in these patients, such effects might reflect regionally specific changes, or neuroadaptive responses to a chronic hypoglutamatergic state, that are not modeled in normal comparison subjects by an acute drug challenge.

In summary, bioactive doses of the NMDA antagonist, memantine, cause dose- and interval-dependent changes in PPI in rats and normal humans. In rats, 10 mg/kg memantine increases short-interval PPI, and 20 mg/kg decreases long-interval PPI. In humans, 20 mg memantine has mild hedonic effects and reduces spontaneous blink rate, and significantly increases long-interval PPI. This latter effect is most evident among individuals with low baseline PPI, or with personality scale scores suggesting high NS, high SSS, or high DIS. At 30 mg, memantine causes significant dizziness, but has no effects on PPI at any prepulse interval or in any sample subgroup. The PPI-enhancing effects of memantine are generally consistent with reports of other NMDA antagonist effects in humans, but are distinct from the effects of memantine or other NMDA antagonists on PPI in rats.