INTRODUCTION

Cannabinoid research is currently one of the most active areas of neuroscience, largely because drugs acting on the cannabinoid system may be beneficial for treating a wide variety of disorders, including substance abuse. This system appears to be capable of modulating the rewarding effects of many drugs of abuse, including opioids (De Vries et al, 2003; Navarro et al, 2001; Solinas et al, 2003), nicotine (Cohen et al, 2002; Le Foll and Goldberg, 2005), and alcohol (De Vries and Schoffelmeer, 2005; Economidou et al, 2006). Ironically, little attention has been focused on potential pharmacotherapies specifically targeting the substantial and growing problem of cannabis use disorders (Compton et al, 2004). General population household survey data showed that annual prevalence of cannabis use in the United States of America in 2006 was 10.3% of the population (compared with 2.5% for cocaine) and 1.2 million people reported receiving treatment for cannabis use (compared with 928 000 for cocaine; SAMHSA, 2007). Moreover, primary cannabis admissions are on the rise. For example, in New York City, the primary cannabis admissions to all treatment programs increased from less than 5% in 1991, to 24.3% in 2003 and 27.8% in 2006 (CEWG, 2004, 2007). In the past, one obstacle to finding new pharmacotherapies has been the lack of animal models of cannabis abuse (Justinova et al, 2005a). However, we have recently developed procedures for establishing robust self-administration of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC, the main psychoactive constituent of marijuana) and other cannabinoid agonists in squirrel monkeys (Justinova et al, 2003, 2004, 2005b; Tanda et al, 2000).

An advantage of drug self-administration as an animal model of human drug abuse is that it can be modified in various ways to concentrate on specific components of drug abuse (Panlilio and Goldberg, 2007). One particular type of self-administration procedure, the second-order schedule (Goldberg et al, 1975; Schindler et al, 2002), has been strongly advocated as the most advantageous animal model of drug-seeking behavior (Arroyo et al, 1998; Everitt and Robbins, 2000). This drug-seeking schedule has many advantages related to its incorporation of drug-related stimuli, which model the environmental cues that can maintain drug seeking and induce drug craving and relapse in humans. Thus, this procedure makes it possible to evaluate treatments that target drug seeking, per se, in a drug-free state, as opposed to treatments that only block the effects of a drug after it has been taken. This is important because treatments that reduce drug seeking might provide an especially effective means of achieving and maintaining drug abstinence. The second-order drug-seeking schedule can also be used to study relapse induced by drug-related cues, as well as relapse caused by re-exposure to the abused drug or exposure to other drugs (Spealman et al, 1999). Relapse is one of the defining features of addiction, and perhaps the most important impediment to effective treatment (O'Brien, 2001).

In the present study, we provide the first demonstration of THC self-administration using this drug-seeking procedure. In this procedure, squirrel monkeys' drug-seeking behavior (lever pressing) intermittently produced only brief presentations of a visual stimulus, until the end of the 30-min session, when the last response of the session produced both a stimulus and an intravenous delivery of THC. Thus, all drug-seeking behavior occurred prior to drug delivery, and, unlike other self-administration procedures, this behavior was not influenced by other pharmacological effects of the drug, such as behavioral stimulation or depression. Importantly, behavior was influenced by the drug-paired stimulus (cue), which is intended to model the effects of drug-associated environmental stimuli that may maintain drug seeking in humans. Thus, it was possible to study the effects of potential treatment drugs specifically on THC seeking. The drug-seeking procedure was also used to model cue-induced relapse by measuring the ability of the drug-paired stimulus to reinstate drug seeking after a period of abstinence from THC. The ability of THC, several other cannabinoid drugs, cocaine, and morphine to reinstate drug seeking was also studied, as was the ability of the potential treatment drugs rimonabant (SR141716A, a cannabinoid CB1-receptor antagonist) and naltrexone (an opioid antagonist) to block this drug-induced reinstatement.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects

Male squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) weighing 0.8–1.2 kg were housed in individual cages in a temperature- and humidity-controlled room with unrestricted access to water. Monkeys were fed (2 h after the session) five biscuits of a high-protein monkey diet (Lab Diet 5045; PMI Nutrition International, Richmond, IN) and two pieces of Banana Softies (Bio-Serv, Frenchtown, NJ) to maintain their body weight at a stable level throughout the study. Fresh fruit, vegetables, and environmental enrichment were also provided daily. The facilities were fully accredited by AAALAC, and all experiments were approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the NIDA Intramural Research Program. Five monkeys (23-90, 5-91, 1551, 1577, and 1584) were used in these experiments. Two monkeys (23-90 and 5-91) had previous experience with self-administering i.v. amphetamine under the second-order schedule, and the other three had experience with fixed-ratio schedules of cocaine self-administration (1577 and 1584) or food reinforcement (1551). Prior to training with the second-order drug-seeking procedure, all monkeys had prior experience with extinction (non-reinforcement), including 10 sessions immediately before the start of THC self-administration training.

Apparatus

Experimental chambers were described previously in detail (Justinova et al, 2003). The sound-attenuating isolation chambers were equipped with a white house light and white noise for masking external sound and contained a Plexiglas chair. The chair had a response lever mounted on a transparent front wall and pairs of green and amber stimulus lights, mounted behind the transparent wall of the chair, which could be illuminated and used as visual stimuli. Monkeys were prepared with chronic indwelling venous catheters (polyvinyl chloride), as described previously (Goldberg, 1973). The monkey's catheter was connected to polyethylene tubing, which extended out of the isolation chamber and was attached to a motor-driven syringe pump. Before start of each session, monkeys were placed in chairs and lightly restrained in the seated position by waist locks.

Drug-Seeking Procedure

Under the drug-seeking schedule, a green light was used, and every tenth lever press during a 30-min fixed interval of time (FI 30′) changed the light's color from green to amber (brief stimulus) for 2 s (10-response fixed-ratio schedule of reinforcement; FR 10). The first FR 10 completed after the 30-min fixed interval elapsed produced 10 consecutive injections of THC (0.2 ml delivered over 0.2 s, with 10 s between injections), each paired with a 2-s presentation of amber light. In standard nomenclature (Schindler et al, 2002), this was an FI 30′ (FR 10:S) schedule. THC administration occurred only at the end of each daily session (Monday to Friday). Drug seeking was shaped under the second-order schedule by slowly increasing the length of the FI (from FI 2′ (FR 10:S) to FI 30′ (FR 10:S)). Sessions typically lasted about 32 min, and after the FI elapsed, adequate time (up to 30 min) was always allowed for completion of the final FR and drug delivery. THC injection doses of 1, 2, 4, and 8 μg/kg were used to give total end-of-session doses of 10, 20, 40, and 80 μg/kg, respectively. Each of these doses of THC was tested for 9–10 consecutive sessions, preceded by 9–10 sessions in which vehicle was delivered at the end of the session instead of THC (extinction).

The effects of pre-session intramuscular (IM) treatment with rimonabant were determined using the following procedure, in which THC (40 μg/kg) was delivered at the end of each session. First, vehicle was administered intramuscularly 60 min before each of at least 10 consecutive sessions. Then 0.3 mg/kg of rimonabant was administered intramuscularly 60 min before each session for 10 consecutive sessions. Finally, IM vehicle injections were administered before each session for 10 sessions. After testing with rimonabant, a similar procedure was used with pre-session injections of naltrexone (0.1 mg/kg, IM), given 15 min before the session.

The effects of brief-stimulus presentations were assessed by discontinuing the stimulus (but still delivering end-of session THC) for 10 sessions. These no-stimulus test sessions were preceded by 10 sessions in which brief stimuli were presented and THC (40 μg/kg) was delivered at the end of each session. (Brief stimulus presentations were only discontinued during the 30-min drug seeking period; the stimulus always accompanied the 10 end-of session i.v. injections.)

Cue-induced reinstatement was studied by first discontinuing both brief stimulus and THC delivery for 5–10 sessions, and then reintroducing the stimulus during a test session. Test sessions for cue-induced reinstatement were each separated by two to three intervening sessions without the stimulus. No end-of-session THC was delivered during these test sessions or intervening sessions. The procedure used for testing drug-induced reinstatement was similar, except that brief stimulus presentations and end-of-session THC delivery were discontinued during the test session and at least two preceding sessions, and THC (10–80 μg/kg), morphine (0.19–1.50 mg/kg), anandamide (30–560 μg/kg), methanandamide (3–100 μg/kg), AM404 (0.3–3 mg/kg), or cocaine (0.03–1 mg/kg) was administered i.v. by the experimenter immediately before the start of the test session. The ability of an opioid antagonist (naltrexone; 0.1 mg/kg, given IM 15 min before the session) or cannabinoid antagonist (rimonabant; 0.3 mg/kg, given IM 60 min before the session) to block reinstatement induced by THC (40 μg/kg) or morphine (0.75 mg/kg) was also tested.

Drugs

THC (Research Triangle, NIH), anandamide (N-arachidonylethanolamide; Tocris Cookson), methanandamide (Tocris Cookson), and AM404 (Tocris Cookson) were dissolved in vehicle containing 0.125–4% ethanol and 0.125–4% Tween 80 and saline. Rimonabant (SR141716; Research Triangle, NIH) was dissolved in vehicle containing 1% ethanol and 1% Tween 80 and saline. (−)-Cocaine HCl (Sigma), naltrexone HCl (Sigma), and morphine sulfate (NIH) were dissolved in saline.

Statistical Analysis

Data analyses were performed on the total number of THC-seeking responses per session. All data are presented as mean±SEM. For the THC dose–response function (Figure 1), the last three sessions for each subject under each dose were averaged. Statistical analysis was performed using single-factor repeated-measure ANOVA (data met the assumptions of the test) to assess differences between vehicle and test-drug pretreatment conditions, or between different doses of THC and the vehicle, as well as between different doses of drugs and vehicle during reinstatement experiments. Paired comparisons were performed using Dunnett's or Bonferroni procedures. Statistical significance was accepted at the P<0.05 level. SigmaStat software (http://www.systat.com) was used for all statistical analyses.

Figure 1
figure 1

THC dose–effect function (a). The number of responses per session is shown as a function of dose of THC under a second-order schedule of THC seeking, in which each tenth response produced a brief presentation of a visual stimulus. Each symbol represents the mean±SEM of the last three sessions within the block of 10 sessions in which each dose of THC or vehicle was tested (n=5). **P<0.01, post hoc comparisons with vehicle self-administration (ANOVA, Dunnett's test). Vehicle extinction (b). The number of THC-seeking responses per session during consecutive sessions before, during and after nine sessions in which vehicle injections were substituted for end-of-session THC injections. Each point represents the mean±SEM for five monkeys. **P<0.01, post hoc comparisons with session 4, the last THC session before vehicle extinction (ANOVA, Dunnett's test).

RESULTS

THC Seeking Under the Second-Order Schedule

Drug seeking under the second-order schedule (Figure 1a) was dose dependent (F4,16=14. 82; P<0.001), with THC doses of 20, 40, and 80 μg/kg, but not 10 μg/kg, maintaining significantly more responding than vehicle. The dose–effect function had an inverted-U shape, with the highest level of drug seeking occurring when the end-of-session dose of THC was 40 μg/kg.

When the vehicle was substituted for 40 μg/kg of THC (Figure 1b), drug seeking was significantly decreased, starting at the second session (session 6 in the figure), and continued to occur at a low rate during the sessions that followed (F9,36=7.73; P<0.001). When end-of-session delivery of THC was resumed, THC seeking stayed low during the first session (session 14 in the figure), but increased substantially in the next session and returned to baseline levels within three sessions (F5,19=6.60, P=0.001).

Effects of the Cannabinoid-Receptor Antagonist, Rimonabant, and the Opioid-Receptor Antagonist, Naltrexone, on THC Seeking

The cannabinoid antagonist, rimonabant (0.3 mg/kg), which earlier was found to block drug taking under a simple fixed-ratio schedule of THC self-administration (Tanda et al, 2000), significantly decreased THC seeking under the second-order schedule in the present study (F10,30=10.45, P<0.001; Figure 2a). This decrease was significant, starting with the first session of rimonabant treatment, and it remained so throughout the 10-day course of treatment, during which the average level of responding was 78% lower than the baseline. Pretreatment with the opioid antagonist, naltrexone (0.1 mg/kg), also decreased THC seeking (F9,18=2.82, P=0.029; Figure 2b). Although naltrexone, like rimonabant, decreased THC seeking in the first two sessions, this effect became weaker during the next seven sessions of naltrexone treatment, during which the average level of THC seeking was 42% lower than the baseline. When treatment with either rimonabant or naltrexone ended, THC seeking gradually resumed over 2–3 sessions.

Figure 2
figure 2

Effects of pretreatment with the selective cannabinoid CB1-receptor antagonist rimonabant (a) and opioid antagonist naltrexone (b) on THC-seeking behavior. A dose of 0.3 mg/kg of rimonabant was given IM 60 min before each of 10 consecutive sessions, or a dose of 0.1 mg/kg naltrexone was given IM 15 min before each of nine consecutive sessions. Each point represents mean±SEM. *P<0.05, **P<0.01, post hoc comparisons with the last THC session with vehicle pretreatment (session 4) (ANOVA, Dunnett's test).

Effects of Omitting Brief-Stimulus Presentations

Although brief-stimulus presentations alone did not maintain responding when end-of-session THC delivery was discontinued for repeated sessions (as shown in Figure 1b), the brief stimulus nonetheless played a critical role in maintaining the high rates of drug seeking seen under baseline conditions (ie, when both the stimulus and end-of-session THC were presented). This contribution is clear from the results obtained when brief-stimulus presentation was discontinued but end-of-session THC was still delivered. Under these conditions, there was an 85% decrease in THC seeking compared with the baseline (F10,30=21.31, P=0.029; Figure 3a). This decrease was significant, starting with the first session without the brief stimulus. Even though THC continued to be delivered at the end of each session for 10 days, THC seeking showed no sign of recovery until the brief-stimulus presentations were resumed, at which point THC seeking immediately recovered (F5,15=7.58, P<0.001).

Figure 3
figure 3

Effects of omitting brief-stimulus presentations under the THC-seeking schedule (a). Circles represent the mean number of responses per session (n=4) before, during, and after each of 10 sessions in which brief stimuli were not presented during the session, although the 10 end-of-session THC injections were always paired with stimulus. **P<0.01, post hoc comparisons with the last THC session (session 4) with brief stimulus present (ANOVA, Dunnett's test). Cue-induced reinstatement (b). Stimulus presentation and end-of-session delivery of THC were discontinued until THC seeking only occurred at low levels (black bars represent average responding on a day immediately preceding a reinstatement test). Then, brief-stimulus presentations were resumed during three test sessions (white bars), separated by two to three sessions with no brief-stimulus presentations. Each bar represents an average from four monkeys. *P<0.05, paired comparisons with a respective day before each test (ANOVA, Bonferroni test). Effect of daily pretreatment with rimonabant (0.3 mg/kg) on cue-induced reinstatement (c). White bars represent an average (n=3) of all reinstatement tests with brief stimulus present either after pretreatment with vehicle or rimonabant. Black bars represent average (n=3) responding on days immediately preceding a reinstatement test. *P<0.05, paired comparisons with a respective day before the reinstatement test (ANOVA, Bonferroni test).

Cue-Induced Reinstatement of THC Seeking

When both brief stimulus and THC delivery were discontinued simultaneously, THC seeking occurred at very low levels (Figure 3b). However, after repeated sessions in which no brief stimuli were presented, reintroduction of the brief stimulus dramatically reinstated THC seeking. This sequence, 2–3 sessions with no stimulus and no THC, followed by reintroduction of the brief stimulus (but not end-of-session THC) during a test session, was repeated three times in each monkey, and the cue continued to reinstate THC seeking (first test: F1,3=24.51, P=0.016; second test: F1,3=11.23, P=0.044; third test: F1,3=12.54, P=0.038).

When monkeys received daily pretreatment with rimonabant (0.3 mg/kg) for 9–10 consecutive sessions while brief stimulus and THC delivery were discontinued, reintroduction of brief-stimulus presentations failed to reinstate THC seeking (Figure 3c, F1,2=3.19, P=0.22). The testing sequence consisted of several days with no stimulus and no THC, followed by reintroduction of the brief stimulus (but no THC) during the test session. The sequence was repeated one to three times in each monkey, and Figure 3c shows averaged data for all three monkeys. The testing sequence with vehicle pretreatment was performed within 4 weeks of rimonabant testing and shows that brief-stimulus presentations (repeated three times in each monkey), in the absence of rimonabant, continued to reinstate THC seeking significantly (Figure 3c, F1,2=23.16, P=0.04).

Drug-Induced Reinstatement of THC Seeking by THC and Morphine

The effects of pre-session priming injections with THC and morphine were also studied during a phase in which brief stimuli and end-of-session THC delivery were discontinued. THC seeking was robustly reinstated in a dose-dependent manner by pre-session treatment with 20 or 40 μg/kg THC (F4,16=18.08, P<0.001; Figure 4a), but not by the lowest (10 μg/kg) or the highest dose (80 μg/kg, which may have had satiating or behaviorally disruptive effects). Morphine dose dependently reinstated THC seeking, with the 0.75 mg/kg dose of morphine producing the strongest effect (F4,8=4.65, P=0.031; Figure 4b).

Figure 4
figure 4

Drug-induced reinstatement. Effects of passive intravenous exposure to different doses of THC (a) and morphine (b) on THC seeking during sessions in which the brief stimulus was not presented and end-of-session THC was not delivered. Each bar represents the mean±SEM of the number of responses per session from five (THC) or three (morphine) monkeys. *P<0.05, **P<0.01, post hoc comparisons with vehicle extinction (dose, 0 mg/kg) (ANOVA, Dunnett's test). Effects of a cannabinoid antagonist and an opioid antagonist on THC- (c) and morphine-induced (d) reinstatement of THC seeking. Rimonabant (RIM; IM, 60 min before session) or naltrexone (NAL; IM, 15 min before the session) was injected, and then THC or morphine (MORPH) was injected i.v. immediately before the session. Brief stimulus was not presented and end-of-session THC was not delivered. Each bar represents the mean±SEM of the number of responses per session from three monkeys. *P<0.05, paired comparisons with vehicle; #P<0.05, paired comparisons with THC reinstatement; ##P<0.01, paired comparisons with morphine reinstatement (ANOVA, Bonferroni test).

Blockade of THC- and Morphine-Induced Reinstatement

The ability of a cannabinoid antagonist (rimonabant, 0.3 mg/kg) and an opioid antagonist (naltrexone, 0.1 mg/kg) to alter the reinstatement induced by pre-session injections of THC (40 μg/kg) or morphine (0.75 mg/kg) was also tested (Figure 4). Rimonabant blocked THC-induced reinstatement (Figure 4c, F1,2=20.27, P=0.046, paired comparisons with THC 40 μg/kg) but not morphine-induced reinstatement (Figure 4d, F1,2=0.03, P=0.88, paired comparisons with morphine 0.75 mg/kg; F1,2=26.90, P=0.035, paired comparisons with the vehicle). Naltrexone blocked morphine-induced reinstatement (Figure 4d, F1,2=273.84, P=0.004, paired comparisons with morphine 0.75 mg/kg), but failed to affect THC-induced reinstatement (Figure 4c, F1,2=2.03, P=0.29, paired comparisons with THC 40 μg/kg; F1,2=19.31, P=0.048, paired comparisons with the vehicle).

Reinstatement by Other Drugs

Pre-session injections of other drugs of abuse like the psychostimulant cocaine, other cannabinoid agonists (anandamide and methanandamide), and an anandamide-transport inhibitor (AM404) were also tested for their ability to reinstate THC seeking. The test sequence was the same as that described above. Over a wide range of doses, cocaine (0.03, 0.1, 0.3, 1 mg/kg) failed to reinstate THC seeking (F4,8=1.81, P=0.22; Figure 5c). Pre-session treatment with the endogenous cannabinoid agonist, anandamide (F4,8=4.43, P=0.035; Figure 5a), or its longer-acting congener, methanandamide (F4,8=4.05, P=0.044; Figure 5b), produced dose-dependent reinstatement of THC seeking. AM404 (0.3, 1, 3 mg/kg) also produced significant reinstatement, but only at the highest dose tested (F3,6=18.19, P=0.002; Figure 5d).

Figure 5
figure 5

Drug-induced reinstatement. Effects of passive intravenous exposure (immediately before the session) to anandamide (a), methanandamide (b), cocaine (c), and AM404 (d) on THC seeking during sessions in which the brief stimulus was not presented and end-of-session THC was not delivered. Each bar represents the mean±SEM of the number of responses per session from three monkeys. *P<0.05, **P<0.01, post hoc comparisons with vehicle extinction (dose, 0 mg/kg) (ANOVA, Dunnett's test).

DISCUSSION

Monkeys' THC-seeking behavior occurred at a high rate even though the drug was not delivered until the end of the session. This behavior depended on both delivery of THC and response-contingent presentations of the drug-paired stimulus, which is intended to model the effects of environmental cues in the human drug-abuse environment. When the stimulus was not presented, THC seeking decreased abruptly and continued to occur at a low rate even when THC was still delivered at the end of each session. Moreover, in the relapse procedure, when stimulus and THC delivery were both discontinued, restoring just the stimulus caused the monkeys to immediately resume THC seeking. Thus, like re-exposure to the drug, re-exposure to THC-associated cues represents an important trigger for relapse following a period of abstinence.

However, high levels of THC seeking were only maintained over repeated sessions when the cues were presented and a reinforcing dose of THC was delivered at the end of the previous session. Neither vehicle nor a low dose (10 μg/kg) of THC maintained response over repeated days, even when the cues continued to be presented. Although response-contingent cues can sometimes maintain drug-seeking behavior for many sessions in the absence of drug delivery (Schindler et al, 2002), this is not always the case, particularly in subjects with previous exposure to extinction procedures. Thus, the decrease in drug seeking when end-of-session THC delivery was discontinued but the cue was still presented, is not a unique effect associated with cannabinoid seeking; this effect was similar to that observed under similar schedules of cocaine or food seeking (Goldberg and Tang, 1977), and can be attributed to the monkeys in the present study having had extensive experience with extinction.

When the THC-seeking procedure was used to evaluate the effects of potential therapeutic treatments, it was found that the cannabinoid CB1-receptor antagonist, rimonabant, was highly effective in reducing the drug-seeking response. Importantly, treatment with rimonabant produced an immediate decrease in THC seeking, indicating that rimonabant blocked the ability of the stimulus to maintain THC seeking. This effect cannot be attributed to non-specific reductions of response rate, since rimonabant dose of 0.3 mg/kg was well below doses that disrupt behavior in squirrel monkeys (3.2 mg/kg; Nakamura-Palacios et al, 2000). This finding is consistent with a number of studies showing that rimonabant can reduce the behavioral effects of stimuli associated with other drugs of abuse, including nicotine, alcohol, cocaine, and heroin (Cohen et al, 2005; De Vries and Schoffelmeer, 2005; Fattore et al, 2007a; Le Foll and Goldberg, 2005; Maldonado et al, 2006), as well as the effects of similar cues under second-order food-seeking procedures (Evenden and Ko, 2007; Thornton-Jones et al, 2005). Thus, this effect of rimonabant on responding maintained by drug-paired cues appears to be a general effect, unlike its ability to reduce drug-taking behavior, which seems to be limited to specific drugs (De Vries and Schoffelmeer, 2005). This suggests that the ability to block both drug seeking (behavior reinforced by drug-related cues) and drug taking (behavior reinforced directly by the drug) (Tanda et al, 2000) might make rimonabant and similar drugs especially useful for treating cannabinoid use disorders.

In contrast with rimonabant, treatment with the opioid antagonist, naltrexone, had a more limited effect. In a previous study by Justinova et al (2004), naltrexone produced partial reduction (about 40%) in THC taking over most of a 5-day course of treatment. However, in the present study, naltrexone only decreased THC seeking during the first 2 days of treatment. These results might suggest that, like rimonabant, naltrexone can alter both THC seeking and THC taking, but that naltrexone only partially blocks the reinforcing effects of THC. This finding is consistent with the many studies showing functional interactions between the cannabinoid and opioid systems, but it appears that an opioid antagonist alone might not provide significant protection against drug seeking induced by THC-related environmental cues.

The results of this study demonstrate that this second-order schedule can be used to study not only THC seeking behavior but also drug-induced as well as cue-induced reinstatement of THC seeking. In this model of relapse, we found that drug seeking was reinstated when the monkeys were passively exposed to THC, the endocannabinoid anandamide, or its longer-acting congener, methanandamide. Reinstatement was also produced by the anandamide-transport inhibitor, AM404, that has been shown to increase brain levels of endogenous anandamide in rodents (Bortolato et al, 2006). Also, consistent with evidence for functional links between the cannabinoid and opioid systems (De Vries et al, 2003; Fattore et al, 2007b), passive exposure to morphine increased THC seeking after a period of abstinence. Although it has been shown that passive cannabinoid exposure can reinstate cocaine seeking in rats (De Vries et al, 2001; Xi et al, 2006), cocaine did not reinstate THC seeking, even though these monkeys had a history of psychostimulant self-administration. This finding is consistent with those of Spano et al (2004) who found that the cannabinoid agonist, WIN 55,212-2, or heroin reinstated seeking of WIN 55,212-2 in rats, but cocaine did not.

When we tested the ability of rimonabant and naltrexone to block the reinstating effects of passive exposure to THC or morphine, we found that the cannabinoid antagonist only blocked the effects of the cannabinoid agonist, and the opioid antagonist only blocked the effects of the opioid agonist. These findings contrast with those of Spano et al (2004) that rimonabant and the opioid antagonist, naloxone, were both capable of preventing WIN 55,212-2-induced as well as heroin-induced reinstatement of WIN 55,212-2 seeking in rats. This discrepancy could be due to differences between rats and monkeys, or due to differences between THC and WIN 55,212-2, which show different profiles of non-cannabinoid receptor binding.

In conclusion, THC, the main psychoactive ingredient in cannabis, has been shown to have strong reinforcing effects in a non-human primate, the squirrel monkey, under procedures that now include fixed-ratio drug-taking and second-order drug seeking. Extended sequences of drug-seeking behavior can be maintained by THC-associated environmental cues, even when THC is not delivered until the end of the experimental session. This allows to study THC-seeking behavior that occurs in the absence of direct effects of THC. The maintenance of high rates of THC seeking depended on both presentation of cues and delivery of THC at the end of recent sessions. When both cue presentation and THC delivery were discontinued, THC seeking occurred only at very low levels. After such a period of abstinence, THC seeking could be reinstated either by presentation of the stimulus or by passive exposure to THC, other cannabinoid receptor agonists, the endocannabinoid uptake inhibitor AM404, or morphine, but not by cocaine. This represents the first demonstration of cue- and drug-induced reinstatement of cannabinoid seeking in non-human primates. The cannabinoid antagonist, rimonabant, produced decreases in THC seeking for as long as the rimonabant treatment continued. In contrast, the opioid antagonist, naltrexone, decreased THC seeking only during the first two sessions of treatment. Rimonabant has received attention as a candidate medication for preventing the behavioral effects of drug-related and feeding-related cues in general. The ability of rimonabant to block both cue-induced THC seeking and the direct reinforcing effects of THC in a non-human primate model of cannabinoid dependence suggest that rimonabant, or other cannabinoid antagonists, may be particularly efficacious for the treatment of cannabinoid abuse, helping to both achieve and maintain abstinence.