Spontaneous sign systems created by deaf children in two cultures

Abstract

Deaf children whose access to usable conventional linguistic input, signed or spoken, is severely limited nevertheless use gesture to communicate1,2,3. These gestures resemble natural language in that they are structured at the level both of sentence4 and of word5. Although the inclination to use gesture may be traceable to the fact that the deaf children's hearing parents, like all speakers, gesture as they talk6, the children themselves are responsible for introducing language-like structure into their gestures7. We have explored the robustness of this phenomenon by observing deaf children of hearing parents in two cultures, an American and a Chinese culture, that differ in their child-rearing practices8,9,10,11,12 and in the way gesture is used in relation to speech13. The spontaneous sign systems developed in these cultures shared a number of structural similarities: patterned production and deletion of semantic elements in the surface structure of a sentence; patterned ordering of those elements within the sentence; and concatenation of propositions within a sentence. These striking similarities offer critical empirical input towards resolving the ongoing debate about the ‘innateness’ of language in human infants14,15,16.

Access options

Rent or Buy article

Get time limited or full article access on ReadCube.

from$8.99

All prices are NET prices.

Figure 1: Probability that a gesture will be produced for transitive actors (TA), patients (P), and intransitive actors (IA) in a two-element gesture sentence.
Figure 2: Mean number of complex gesture sentences as a proportion of total gesture sentences in Chinese and American deaf children and their mothers.
Figure 3: Development of complex gesture sentences in American deaf children and their mothers (proportion of total gesture sentences).

References

  1. 1

    Moores, D. F. in Language Perspectives (eds Schiefelbusch, R. L. & Lloyd, L. L.) 377–878 (University Park, Baltimore, (1974)).

    Google Scholar 

  2. 2

    Tervoort, B. T. Esoteric symbolism in the communication behavior of young deaf children. Am. Ann. Deaf 106, 436–480 (1961).

    Google Scholar 

  3. 3

    Fant, L. Ameslan (National Association of the Deaf, Silver Springs, MD, (1972)).

    Google Scholar 

  4. 4

    Goldin-Meadow, S. & Feldman, H. The development of language-like communication without a language model. Science 197, 401–403 (1977).

    ADS  CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  5. 5

    Goldin-Meadow, S., Mylander, C. & Butcher, C. The resilience of combinatorial structure at the word level: morphology in self-styled gesture systems. Cognition 56, 195–262 (1995).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  6. 6

    Feyereisen, P. & deLannoy, J.-D. Gesture and speech (Cambridge Univ. Press, New York, (1991)).

    Google Scholar 

  7. 7

    Goldin-Meadow, S. & Mylander, C. Gestural communication in deaf children: the non-effects of parental input on language development. Science 221, 372–374 (1983).

    ADS  CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  8. 8

    Miller, P. J., Wiley, A., Fung, H. & Liang, C.-H. Personal storytelling as a medium of socialization in Chinese and American families. Child Development 68, 557–568 (1997).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  9. 9

    Stevenson, H. W. et al. Contexts of achievement. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 55 1–119 ((1990)).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  10. 10

    Young, N. F. Socialization patterns among the Chinese of Hawaii. Amerasia J. 1, 31–51 (1972).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. 11

    Chen, C. & Uttal, D. H. Cultural values, parents' beliefs, and children's achievement in the United States and China. Hum. Dev. 31, 351–358 (1988).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. 12

    Wang, X-l., Mylander, C. & Goldin-Meadow, S. in Research on Family Resources and Needs Across the World (ed. Cusinato, M.) 363–374 (Edizioni Universitarie di Lettere Economia, Diritto, Italy, (1996)).

    Google Scholar 

  13. 13

    McNeill, D. & Duncan, S. D. in Language and Gesture: Window into Thought and Action (ed. McNeill, D.) (New Lexington, San Francisco, submitted).

  14. 14

    Chomsky, N. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, (1965)).

    Google Scholar 

  15. 15

    Pinker, S. The Language Instinct (Harper Collins, New York, (1995)).

    Google Scholar 

  16. 16

    Gleitman, L. R. & Newport, E. L. in Language, Vol. 1, Invitation to Cognitive Science Series (eds Gleitman, L. R. & Liberman, M.) 1–24 (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, (1995)).

    Google Scholar 

  17. 17

    Petitto, L. A. in The Development of Language and Language Researchers (ed. Kessel, F.) 187–221 (Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, (1988)).

    Google Scholar 

  18. 18

    McNeill, D. Hand and Mind (Univ. Chicago Press, Chicago, (1992)).

    Google Scholar 

  19. 19

    Dixon, R. M. W. Ergativity (Cambridge Univ. Press, New York, (1994)).

    Google Scholar 

  20. 20

    Silverstein, M. in Grammatical Categories in Australian Languages (ed. Dixon, R. M. W.) 112–171 (Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies, Canberra, (1976)).

    Google Scholar 

  21. 21

    Zar, J. H. Biostatistical Analysis 2nd edn (Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, (1984)).

    Google Scholar 

  22. 22

    Goldin-Meadow, S., Butcher, C., Mylander, C. & Dodge, M. Nouns and verbs in a self-styled gesture system: what's in a name? Cogn. Psychol. 27, 259–319 (1994).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  23. 23

    Goldin-Meadow, S. & Mylander, C. Gestural communication in deaf children: the effects and non-effects of parental input on early language development. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 49, 1–151 ((1984)).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  24. 24

    Siegel, S. Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences (McGraw-Hill, New York, (1956)).

    Google Scholar 

  25. 25

    Goldin-Meadow, S. Book review of Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind by Savage-Rumbaugh, S. & Lewin, R. Int. J. Primatol. 17, 145–148 (1996).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. 26

    Wimsatt, W. in Integrating Scientific Disciplines (ed. Bechtel, W.) 185–208 (Martinus-Nijhoff, Dordrecht, (1986)).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank L. Barsalou, J. Huttenlocher, B. Keysar, M. K. McClintock, P. Miller, D.McNeill and W. Wimsatt for comments; S. Duncan for help in collecting the Taiwanese data; and F. Bechter and M.-Y. Zheng for help in transcribing the videotapes. Supported by grants from the National Institute on Deafness and other Communciation Disorders and the NSF.

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Susan Goldin-Meadow.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Goldin-Meadow, S., Mylander, C. Spontaneous sign systems created by deaf children in two cultures. Nature 391, 279–281 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1038/34646

Download citation

Further reading

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.