Main

A variety of experimental studies have demonstrated nicotinic effects in cognition. Nicotine and nicotine agonists have been shown to improve aspects of cognition in both experimental animals and humans (Buccafusco et al. 1996; Levin 1992; Levin et al. 1996b; Levin and Simon 1998). In particular, nicotine has been shown to improve working memory performance. In previous work, acute and chronic nicotine administration led to working memory improvements in rats tested on the radial-arm maze. In chronic administration studies, these effects persisted for at least two weeks after drug administration ended. The nicotinic antagonist, mecamylamine, blocked the memory improvements of both chronic and acute nicotine administration (Levin et al. 1993; Levin and Russell 1992). Nicotinic systems play an important role in the neural basis of memory function.

Nicotinic systems interact with a variety of other neural systems, which may be important in the neural basis of memory function. Activation of nicotinic receptors leads to the downstream release of many neurotransmitters including acetylcholine, dopamine (DA), norepinephrine, serotonin, and glutamate (Wonnacott et al. 1989). DA in particular has been shown to be important for memory function (Levin 1988). Several studies have examined the role of DA in memory function as well as the interactive effects of nicotine and DA. Previous work in our laboratory has illustrated the importance of DA in cognitive function by showing that both nicotinic and DA antagonists cause working memory deficits in rats tested on the radial arm maze. Nicotinic and DA agonists, on the other hand, induced working memory improvements in rats (Levin and Eisner 1994; Levin et al. 1990; Levin and Rose 1992; McGurk et al. 1989a,b). In addition, nicotine and nicotinic agonists have been shown to increase DA release in the striatum and the nucleus accumbens (Brazell et al. 1990; el-Bizri and Clarke 1994; Grady et al. 1992). Thus, the data illustrate that nicotinic and DA interactions are important for memory function (Levin et al. 1996a). However, the mechanisms of these interactions and their effects on memory are still unclear (Levin et al. 1996a).

The aim of the current study was to further explore the memory effects of interactions between the nicotinic and DA systems. While our previous studies involved the use of selective DA receptor drugs, this study employed the use of three antipsychotic medications: haloperidol, clozapine and risperidone. Many previous studies have explored DA effects on memory by looking at the memory effects of antipsychotic medications, which often act as DA antagonists at specific DA receptors. Haloperidol, a typical antipsychotic, is characterized by its antagonistic binding at the D2 DA receptors. Clozapine, an atypical antipsychotic, binds to the D4 DA receptor subtype 10 times as strongly as it binds to D2 receptors. It also differs from haloperidol in that it shows a high affinity for 5-HT2 sites and a much lower affinity for D2 sites in the cerebral cortex and striatum (Matsubara et al. 1993). Risperidone, another atypical antipsychotic, also has a higher affinity for the 5-HT2 receptor than for the D2 receptor (Hertel et al. 1996; Sumiyoshi et al. 1994). In fact, risperidone binds to the 5HT2 receptor 20 times as strongly as it binds to D2, though its binding to the D2 receptor is comparable to that of haloperidol (Leysen et al. 1988).

While these drugs affect multiple neurotransmitter systems and are not as specific as other DA antagonists, it is still important to understand the memory effects of these antipsychotic medications in their interaction with nicotine. Furthermore, the interaction of nicotine with DA antagonists is one that occurs frequently in certain human populations. Statistics indicate that almost 90% of schizophrenics smoke cigarettes (Hughes et al. 1986). Thus, these individuals who are taking antipsychotic medications are co-administering nicotine as well. Through this study, we seek to gain a better understanding of the memory effects of these drug interactions.

We hypothesize that nicotine and antipsychotic medications are interacting, via the nicotinic and DA systems, to induce downstream effects on memory. In line with our previous findings, we hypothesize that acute nicotine administration can improve memory performance. Furthermore, we expect that acute haloperidol will decrease memory performance while acute clozapine and acute risperidone will decrease memory performance to a smaller extent or not at all. Third, we hypothesize that acute nicotine can reverse cognitive impairments caused by these antipsychotic medications when co-administered with them. We tested these hypotheses through drug challenge studies in rats on the radial-arm maze.

METHODS

Subjects

A total of 47 adult female Sprague-Dawley rats were used in three experimental groups. The haloperidol group consisted of 12 animals, the clozapine group consisted of 24 animals, and the risperidone group consisted of 11 animals. The rats were housed three per cage and were on a reverse 12 h light/12 h dark cycle (lights off at 7:00 A.M.). The subjects were on ad lib feeding for one week and then kept at approximately 85% of ad lib levels. Testing took place during the dark cycle and the rats were fed daily after testing. These studies were conducted under protocols approved by the Duke University Animal Care and Use Committee conforming to internationally accepted standards of animal welfare.

Radial-Arm Maze

Cognitive tests were performed using a black, wooden 8-arm radial maze. The maze was elevated 30 cm off the ground, with a central platform 35 cm in diameter and 8 arms each 10 × 80 cm. Each arm contained a food cup, at its terminal end, which was baited during testing with one half piece of Kellogg's Froot Loops® cereal. At the beginning of each trial, a 30-cm opaque ring was placed on the central platform and a rat was placed inside the ring for 10 s. Following this interval, the ring was removed and timing began. The rat was allowed to run on the maze until all eight arms were entered or until 300 s had passed. An arm entry was recorded when all four of the animal's legs had crossed the threshold of the arm. Choice accuracy was measured by entries to repeat (ETR), which was the number of arms entered until a repeat entry was made into an arm previously entered during the trial.

Haloperidol-Nicotine Interactions

After 18 training sessions on the radial arm maze, rats (n = 12) began the drug phase of the experiment. In all the studies the drugs were dissolved in saline and injected in a volume of 1 ml/kg. Acute administration was carried out via subcutaneous injection 20 min prior to the maze trial. Nicotine and haloperidol were co-administered in doses containing 0, 0.2, or 0.4 mg/kg of nicotine combined with 0 or 0.04 mg/kg of haloperidol. The six drug combinations are shown in Table 1. Each animal was tested once on the radial arm maze with each of the six drug combinations, which were given as cocktails. Animals were tested every other day to allow for the drug to be broken down and eliminated and they were not run on the maze during washout days. In this experiment, drug treatments were counterbalanced. Drug doses were based on salt weight for both nicotine ditartrate (Sigma) and haloperidol HCl (Ortho-McNeil).

Table 1 Nicotine-Haloperidol Dose Combinations

Clozapine-Nicotine Interactions

After 18 training sessions on the radial arm maze, rats (n = 24) began the drug phase of the experiment in the same manner as described above for the haloperidol-treated animals. Nicotine and clozapine were co-administered in doses containing 0, 0.2, or 0.4 mg/kg of nicotine with 0, 1.25, or 2.50 mg/kg of clozapine. The nine drug combinations are shown in Table 2.

Table 2 Nicotine-Clozapine Dose Combinations

The animals were tested in the same manner as those in the nicotine-haloperidol group, with a washout day in between testing. Once again, doses were based on salt weight for nicotine ditartrate (Sigma) and clozapine (Research Biochemical International). The drugs were administered as cocktails and the treatments were counterbalanced.

Risperidone-Nicotine Interactions

After 18 training sessions on the radial arm maze, these rats (n = 11) began the drug phase of the experiment in the same manner as mentioned above. Nicotine and risperidone were co-administered in doses of 0, 0.2, or 0.4 mg/kg of nicotine with 0 or 0.05 mg/kg of risperidone. The six drug combinations are shown in Table 3.

Table 3 Nicotine-Risperidone Dose Combinations

As in the two previous groups, doses for nicotine ditartrate (Sigma) and risperidone (Research Biochemical International) were based on salt weights. Drugs were administered in a cocktail and treatments were counterbalanced with a washout day between testing.

Data Analysis

The choice accuracy and response latency measures were assessed by a within-subjects design ANOVA. The factors were Nicotine and Antipsychotic dose. A p-value of less than .05 was considered significant. The tests were two-tailed except to test a one-tailed hypothesis to verify the previously demonstrated nicotine-induced memory improvement (for review see Levin and Simon (1998)). A control between-subjects factor of high or low pretest performance accuracy was used to reduce variability and test whether there were selective drug effects on good or poor performers. In analyzing data from each of the three studies, planned comparisons were made between vehicle only administration (saline) and each of the antipsychotic doses alone. Planned comparisons were also made between each of the antipsychotic doses alone with the two doses of nicotine.

RESULTS

Haloperidol-Nicotine Interactions

There was a significant main effect of haloperidol (F1,10 = 9.97, p < .025). However, as can be seen in Figure 1, there was no effect of haloperidol on working memory performance detected in the absence of nicotine. Nicotine administration without haloperidol produced a significant (p < .05) improvement in working memory performance as has been seen many times previously (for review see Levin and Simon (1998)). The haloperidol-induced deficit was seen when it was given in combination with nicotine. With the higher nicotine dose (0.4 mg/kg), haloperidol co-treatment caused a significant (p < .05) decrease in working memory performance compared with 0.4 mg/kg of nicotine alone. A similar but not quite significant (p < .10) effect was seen with haloperidol reducing performance accuracy with 0.2 mg/kg of nicotine.

Figure 1
figure 1

Haloperidol interactions with nicotine and radial-arm maze working memory performance, Entries to Repeat (mean ± SEM).

Latency was significantly increased by haloperidol as evidenced by the significant main effect of haloperidol (F1,11) = 5.71, p < .05). The main effect of nicotine and the haloperidol X nicotine interaction were not significant. Interestingly, despite the fact that there was a significant main effect of haloperidol, haloperidol treatment alone did not significantly increase latency relative to the saline control condition. Rather, haloperidol co-treatment reversed the significant (p < .05) reductions in latency caused by 0.2 mg/kg and 0.4 mg/kg of nicotine.

Clozapine-Nicotine Interactions

Both doses of clozapine significantly impaired choice accuracy when administered alone (p < .025). The 0.4 dose of nicotine significantly reversed the memory impairment caused by 1.25 mg/kg of clozapine (p < .025) and 2.5 mg/kg of clozapine (p < .05), returning choice accuracy to nearly control levels. The lower dose of 0.2 mg/kg of nicotine was not effective in reversing the clozapine-induced impairment (Figure 2). A higher acute injection dose of 5 mg/kg of clozapine was tested in pilot studies, but it caused excessive sedation and was not used in further studies.

Figure 2
figure 2

Clozapine interactions with nicotine and radial-arm maze working memory performance, Entries to Repeat (mean ± SEM).

Latency was significantly increased by clozapine as detected by a significant clozapine main effect (F2,46 = 6.08, p < .005). The main effect of nicotine and the clozapine X nicotine interaction were not significant. However, neither dose of clozapine by itself significantly affected latency relative to performance after saline injection. The combination of the 2.5 mg/kg clozapine dose with either 0.2 mg/kg (p < .01) or 0.4 mg/kg nicotine (p < .05) significantly increased latency compared with these doses on nicotine alone.

Nicotine-Risperidone Interactions

In the nicotine-risperidone experiment (Figure 3) there was a significant nicotine main effect (F2,18 = 8.85, p < .005) with regard to choice accuracy (n = 11). The interaction of nicotine X risperidone was significant (F2,18 = 4.06, p < .05). The planned comparisons showed that the 0.4 mg/kg nicotine dose caused a significant (p < .001) improvement in working memory performance compared with the saline treatment. The rats averaged 5.20 ± 0.52 entries to repeat after saline injections and 7.27 ± 0.03 entries to repeat after 0.4 mg/kg of nicotine. The rats averaged an intermediate 6.30 ± 0.41 entries to repeat after the lower dose of 0.2 mg/kg of nicotine, however this improvement was not quite significant (p < .071). Risperidone (0.05 mg/kg) did not impair performance relative to saline. In fact there was a slight, though nonsignificant, increase in the entries to repeat measure to 5.50 ± 0.29. However this dose of risperidone did significantly attenuate the memory improvement caused by nicotine (0.4 mg/kg). The entries to repeat measure dropped from 7.45 ± 0.21 with 0.4 mg/kg of nicotine alone to 6.00 ± 0.30 with 0.4 mg/kg of nicotine + 0.05 mg/kg of risperidone, a significant difference (p < .025). A higher acute risperidone injection dose of 0.1 mg/kg was tested in pilot studies, but it caused excessive sedation, so that choice accuracy could not be reliably determined.

Figure 3
figure 3

Risperidone interactions with nicotine and radial-arm maze working memory performance, Entries to Repeat (mean ± SEM).

There was a significant main effect of risperidone on latency (F1,10 = 7.62, p < .025). The nicotine main effect and the nicotine X risperidone interaction were not significant. The 0.05 mg/kg risperidone dose caused a significant (p < .05) increase in latency that was reversed by co-administration of 0.2 mg/kg of nicotine (p < .025). The 0.4 mg/kg nicotine dose modestly decreased the risperidone-induced sedation, but this was not significant.

DISCUSSION

The data presented in this study points to the interaction of nicotine and DA in working memory function. We found that acute clozapine, at 1.25 mg/kg and 2.50 mg/kg, caused a significant working memory deficit in rats on the radial-arm maze. In contrast, acute haloperidol administration at 0.04 mg/kg and acute risperidone, at 0.05 mg/kg did not cause a significant impairment. Both acute haloperidol and acute risperidone, however, blocked any nicotine-induced improvement in working memory performance. Nicotine, however, reversed the working memory impairment induced by acute clozapine administration of 1.25 or 2.5 mg/kg. Together, these results illustrate the different memory effects of antipsychotic administration either in isolation or in combination with nicotine.

While we hypothesized that acute nicotine administration would induce improvement in memory performance, we observed variation in the nicotinic effect in the three experiments we performed. Nicotine induced improvement was most clearly observed in the nicotine-risperidone experiment. As illustrated in the results, animals in the nicotine-risperidone experiment receiving nicotine at a dose of 0.4 mg/kg showed a significant increase in memory performance compared with saline administration. In the nicotine-haloperidol experiment, nicotine at 0.4 mg/kg produced a smaller increase in memory performance. The difference in nicotine's effect may be due to the fact that the saline administration trials in these two experiments resulted in different baseline levels. In the nicotine-risperidone experiment, the ETR value following saline administration was lower than the same value in the nicotine-haloperidol group. Thus, in the nicotine-haloperidol study, the animals were performing at higher levels with saline treatment and had less room for improvement. In the nicotine-clozapine experiment, we also did not observe a nicotine-induced memory improvement. This may have been due to negative carry-over effects of the clozapine treatment in the repeated measures design.

We also hypothesized that acute haloperidol would cause a decrease in memory performance, but this effect was not observed in the study. Previous work in our laboratory has shown that chronic haloperidol decreases memory performance in both rats and humans (Levin et al. 1996a, 1987; Levin 1997). Thus, the memory effects of acute haloperidol administration in the current study differ from chronic haloperidol administration studies previously carried out in our lab (Levin 1997). In the current study, we also hypothesized that acute nicotine would reverse memory impairments caused by any of the three antipsychotics tested. In the nicotine-haloperidol experiment, however, we observed that haloperidol blocked responsiveness to the 0.4 mg/kg dose of nicotine (see Figure 1).

Given prior memory related research carried out with risperidone, we hypothesized that acute risperidone would decrease memory performance to a lesser extent than haloperidol or not at all. Acute risperidone administration has been shown to induce both improvements in a working memory task in rats and increased frontal cortical activity in schizophrenics during a working memory task (Honey et al. 1999; Nowakowska et al. 1999). In the current nicotine-risperidone experiment, acute risperidone administration in isolation did not induce a significant change in memory performance in rats on the radial-arm maze. Acute risperidone, however, did block the nicotine induced memory improvement.

The results from the nicotine-haloperidol and nicotine-risperidone experiments suggest that blockade of the D2 receptor affects nicotinic and DA interactions in memory performance on the radial arm maze. In interpreting the downstream memory effects of nicotinic and DA interactions in these two experiments, it is important to consider the binding properties of haloperidol and risperidone. Though both drugs have affinity for the D2 receptor, haloperidol's affinity for the receptor is two to three times stronger than that of risperidone in vitro. In addition, haloperidol administration results in quick D2 occupancy while risperidone administration leads to gradual D2 occupancy (Schotte et al. 1996). In light of these differences in D2 affinity and their effects on DA, one would expect that haloperidol would be more likely to induce memory impairment than risperidone. In addition, if nicotine exerts its memory effects through downstream DA activity, one could hypothesize that D2 antagonism by haloperidol would block the nicotinic improvement effect more than antagonism with risperidone. It is also important to note that the memory effects of risperidone may also be due to its antagonism at other receptors, namely the 5HT2 receptor. Risperidone binds to the 5HT2A receptor 20 times as strongly as it binds to D2 in vivo (Schotte et al. 1996). Thus, this 5HT2A antagonism may play a role in the downstream release of neurotransmitters that affect memory. From the results in the current study, however, we conclude that there is an important interaction between nicotinic and DA systems in memory function that appears to be partially mediated through the D2 receptor.

In the nicotine-clozapine experiment, acute clozapine administration induced a significant decrease in memory performance, contrasting with our hypothesis that it would induce slight memory impairment or none at all. In line with part of our hypothesis, however, nicotine did reverse the clozapine-induced memory impairment. The results from this experiment with acute clozapine administration contrast with previous studies where chronic clozapine administration led to less of a cognitive impairment effect than chronic haloperidol (Buchanan et al. 1994; Hagger et al. 1993). In addition, clozapine administration in humans, in comparison to typical neuroleptics, has also been shown to cause greater improvement in certain cognitive tests including verbal fluency, psychomotor speed, and attention (Lee et al. 1999; Meltzer and McGurk 1999). These differing memory effects may be due to differing pharmacodynamic characteristics of acute versus chronic administration of these drugs. Previous work in other laboratories also illustrates other possible mechanisms by which clozapine may have induced the memory impairment we observed in the current study. For instance, previous studies showed that clozapine inhibited nicotine binding at nicotine acetylcholine receptors and also inhibited downstream catecholamine secretion (Park et al. 2001). This inhibition of the nicotinic acetylcholine receptors by clozapine and the downstream effects could lead to memory impairment such as we observed. In addition, this interpretation is consistent with previous work in our lab, which showed that acute administration of nicotinic receptor antagonists in the hippocampus caused memory impairment (Levin et al. 2002). In light of this interpretation, our observation that nicotine reverses clozapine-induced impairment suggests that acute clozapine-induced limitation of nicotinic receptor binding is not sufficient to block nicotine-induced improvement.

The results from the nicotine-clozapine experiment also illustrate the importance of DA in working memory function and suggest that DA receptor binding at the D4 receptor is one of contributing factors. Clozapine, in contrast to many of the typical antipsychotic medications, has a higher affinity for the D4 receptor than the D2 receptor (Schotte et al. 1996). This strong affinity for the D4 receptor may also contribute to its therapeutic effectiveness (Liegeois et al. 1998). While acute clozapine antagonism causes memory impairment, which is blocked by nicotine, it is difficult to make direct correlation to specific neurotransmitter systems because clozapine affects so many systems. In fact, clozapine, like many atypical antipsychotic medications, has a higher affinity for the 5HT2A receptor than the D2 receptor in vivo (Schotte et al. 1996). In addition, clozapine also has high binding affinities for specific histamine and muscarinic receptors and both of these systems are involved in memory function (Kathmann et al. 1994; Michal et al. 1999; Roussinov and Yonkov 1976; Wall et al. 2001). Because clozapine does affect other neurotransmitter systems as well, it would be helpful to further explore the involvement of the D4 receptor by using a drug that is more specific for this receptor subtype.

Although we explored the effects of nicotinic interaction with acute clozapine, it would be interesting and helpful to also look at nicotinic interaction with chronic clozapine administration. Our observation that acute clozapine caused a working memory deficit was actually somewhat surprising given that clozapine has been noted to have less cognitive impairment effects than typical antipsychotics (Buchanan et al. 1994; Hagger et al. 1993). As mentioned earlier, this effect may be due to pharmacodynamic differences with acute versus chronic administration. Thus, it would be useful to look at chronic clozapine administration in rats to see whether working memory impairments are observed or not.

This study and studies like it are important as we seek to understand not only memory function, but nicotinic involvement in memory function as well. With the complexity of nicotinic activity in the brain, we are continuing to explore how nicotine is acting to affect memory. This study further supports previous work that nicotinic and DA interaction is important in working memory function. It is clear that altering activity in these systems through nicotinic and antipsychotic administration leads to downstream effects on memory performance. Further studies will allow us to continue to explore how much of a role each system plays in memory function. Furthermore, this allows us to improve our understanding of which DA receptor subtypes are important.

Understanding the interactions of nicotinic and DA systems in memory function carries many important implications. Since such a large proportion of people with schizophrenia smoke cigarettes, it is important for us to understand the memory effects of this co-administration. In addition, looking at nicotinic involvement in memory provides clues to possible treatments for illnesses with cognitive deficit components, such as Alzheimer's and Parkinson's disease. Characterization of nicotinic interactions with DA systems is an important endeavor for improved treatment of cognitive dysfunction and for improvement of the basic understanding of the neural basis of cognitive function.