Main

Serotonin (5-HT)1A receptors are located both on the cell body of 5-HT neurons and on postsynaptic neurons throughout the central nervous system. On 5-HT neurons, 5-HT1A receptors mediate a negative feedback influence on firing activity. When overactivated by an excess amount of 5-HT, or by an exogenous agonist, they slow down the pacemaker activity leading to a decreased firing rate of 5-HT neurons (Aghajanian and Lakoski 1984; Blier and de Montigny 1987). As 5-HT release is proportional to firing rate, the excess activation of these 5-HT1A autoreceptors generally results in a decrease of 5-HT release in projecting structures (see Rueter et al. 1997). The postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors, particularly abundant in the limbic system, commonly exert an inhibitory function on neuronal activity as well. Therefore, the net effect of the systemic administration of a 5-HT1A receptor agonist on 5-HT1A signal transfer in postsynaptic areas represents a composite of decreased 5-HT release, resulting from 5-HT1A autoreceptor activation, and direct occupation of the postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors.

Although it is well established that neuroendocrine responses, such as growth hormone and adenocorticotropic hormone release, produced by systemic administration of 5-HT1A agonists are mediated by postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors (Van de Kar et al. 1985; Gilbert et al. 1988; Pan and Gilbert 1992), there has been controversy with respect to the hypothermia resulting from this pharmacological challenge. The very first study examining the pre- versus postsynaptic mediation of the hypothermia clearly indicated that this response was produced by postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors (Hjörth 1985). Indeed, this initial study clearly showed that the hypothermic response to the prototypical 5-HT1A agonist 8-OH-DPAT was not attenuated, but even enhanced in rats pretreated daily for 3 days with the 5-HT synthesis inhibitor para-chlorophenylalanine (PCPA). Subsequently, identical results were obtained by another group of investigators using 2-day injections of a higher dose of PCPA (Goodwin et al. 1987). When their PCPA treatment was prolonged, however, for 14 days, despite producing its maximal effect on 5-HT levels after the first 3 days, the 8-OH-DPAT-induced hypothermia was dampened and the conclusion of a presynaptic mediation of this phenomenon was made. From then on, several studies carried out in laboratory animals and humans used the 5-HT1A-induced hypothermia to assess the responsiveness of the 5-HT1A autoreceptor in different disease states and following administration of various types of psychotropic drugs. Although the majority of studies (Hutson et al. 1987; O'Connell et al. 1992; Hillegaart 1991; Millan et al. 1993; Blier and Bouchard 1992; Stockmeier et al. 1992) now indicate that this response in rats results from the activation of postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors, it is still claimed that it is presynaptically mediated in mice (Goodwin et al. 1985). In humans, this issue had not been addressed.

The present study was aimed at examining the 5-HT1A-mediated hypothermia in humans in a 5-HT depleted state. The hypothesis was that if it is mediated by a decreased 5-HT1A transmission at a postsynaptic site, resulting from dampened 5-HT release as a consequence of 5-HT1A autoreceptor activation, the hypothermic response to a 5-HT1A agonist should be attenuated or no longer present when 5-HT availability has been decreased beforehand. As PCPA is no longer used in humans, a dietary tryptophan depletion paradigm was utilized. This approach rapidly leads to significant decreases in brain 5-HT synthesis in humans (Nishizawa et al. 1997; Williams et al. 1999; Carpenter et al. 1998).

METHODS

Study Subjects

Fourteen healthy male volunteers were recruited through local newspaper advertisements. All potential research subjects underwent physical and psychiatric examinations (using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R, nonpatient version; Spitzer et al. 1992). They had an electrocardiogram and routine laboratory tests, including serum sequential multiple analysis by computer, complete blood count, thyroid function tests, HIV test, hepatitis testing, urine analysis, and a urine toxicology screen for drugs of abuse. Only physically healthy subjects without a personal history of psychiatric illness or a family history of mood disorders or alcoholism in first-degree relatives were invited to participate. Subjects scoring 2 or more on at least 3 symptoms on the Hopkins Symptom checklist (HSCL-90) and more than 3 on the Beck Inventory for Depression were excluded. All subjects were medication free for a minimum of 2 weeks prior to testing, smoked less than 10 cigarettes per day, and ingested no more than the equivalent of 5 beers per week and 3 cups of coffee per day. A total of 12 subjects completed the study. Their average age was 29.3 ± 1.6 years (range 22–44). They weighed on average 75.5 ± 2.0 kg (range: 63.5–86.2), and their average height was 178.0 ± 1.7 cm (range 170.2–188.0). Their mean body mass index, calculated by dividing the weight in kg by the square of the height in meters, was 23.9 ± 0.7 (range: 20.3–27.7). This study was approved by the Research Ethics Board of the Department of Psychiatry of McGill University. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects before enrollment in the study. They received $140 (CND) to compensate for loss of time related to their participation.

Procedure

The study was a double-blind crossover design. The day before testing, all subjects ate a low protein diet provided by the investigators and fasted from midnight before the experimental day (see Benkelfat et al. 1994). On the test day, subjects arrived at the laboratory at 8:30 A.M. and had blood samples drawn for the measurement of baseline plasma concentrations of tryptophan and baseline hormone levels using an IV catheter that was left in place with a heparin lock for the rest of the day. They then ingested one of two amino acid mixtures: (1) a 202-g nutritionally balanced control amino acid mixture containing 2.3 g of tryptophan, or (2) a similar mixture without tryptophan (Benkelfat et al. 1994). The drinks were prepared within a few minutes of oral administration by mixing the powdered amino acids with either: (1) 150 ml water, 45 ml chocolate syrup, and 0.6 g of sodium cyclamate; or (2) 180 ml of orange juice plus sodium cyclamate, according to the preference of the subjects. Because of the unpleasant taste of sulfur-containing amino acid methionine, cysteine, and arginine, these were encapsulated and administered separately.

Following ingestion of the amino acid mixture, the subjects remained awake in a room with affectively neutral videos and reading materials available to them. The 5-hour interval was chosen on the basis of data showing that, after ATD in humans, plasma tryptophan reaches its lowest level after about 5 h (Young et al. 1985), and CSF 5-hydroxyindole acetic acid levels are lowered at this time (Carpenter et al. 1998; Williams et al. 1999). Four hours after ingesting the amino acid mixture, the subjects lay down in a reclined bed in a comfortable position with the head elevated. They were not allowed to eat, sleep, or watch television until the procedure was completed. Following a 45-min rest period, baseline blood samples were obtained and oral temperature was measured. Immediately following collection of these data, subjects ingested three 10-mg tablets of buspirone (Buspar®) with a few sips of water. Additional blood samples were collected 60 and 120 min later. Oral temperature was measured every 15 min using a thermistor probe (Electro-Therm, Model TM99A), and digital readings were obtained at the end of a 2-min recording period.

At the end of each test day, before returning home, all subjects ate a high-protein snack and ingested a 1-g L-tryptophan tablet (Tryptan®). This tryptophan preparation, available only on prescription in Canada, has not been associated with any cases of eosinophilia myalgia syndrome (Wilkins 1990).

The samples for determination of growth hormone (GH) and prolactin were collected in ice-cold plastic tubes containing 10 μl/ml of 0.5 M Na2EDTA. They were centrifuged (20 min at 2,500 g at 4°C) within 1 h of sampling. Plasma was aliquoted into plastic tubes and stored at −70°C until time of assay. Plasma prolactin and GH levels were determined by standard immunoradiometric assays using commercially available kits (Immunocorp, Montréal, PQ; Nichols Institute Diagnostics, San Juan Capistrano, CA, USA). The inter- and intra-assay coefficients of variation were as follows: prolactin = 3.9 and 2.5%; growth hormone = 12.1 and 12.3%. Plasma tryptophan concentrations were measured by HPLC with fluometric detection as previously described (Benkelfat et al. 1994). At the end of the study, all measurements were carried out in one assay by a technician blind to the order and nature of drug treatments.

Statistical Analyses

All the results are expressed as means ± SEM. The data were submitted to analysis of covariance for repeated measures. There were no significant treatment X time interactions for any of the data sets, and therefore the two treatments were compared using a multivariate repeated analysis measure. All statistical analyses were performed with the Systat software package (Macintosh version, 1989). The level of statistical significance was taken as p < .05.

RESULTS

Tryptophan Levels

The total plasma tryptophan levels were nearly identical for the two test periods 24 h after a low protein diet. Five hours after ingestion of the drink containing tryptophan, total tryptophan was doubled whereas it was decreased by nearly 90% when the amino acid drink did not contain tryptophan. Two hours after the oral administration of buspirone, the total plasma tryptophan was back to the baseline value in the condition when the amino acid drink ingested contained tryptophan, but still decreased by more than 80% when the drink did not contain tryptophan (Figure 1 ). The difference in tryptophan levels between times 0 and 120 in the depleted state, however, did not quite reach statistical significance (t=1.91, p = .08).

Figure 1
figure 1

Total plasma tryptophan (TRP) levels in healthy male volunteers prior to and following the injection of the amino acid mixture containing tryptophan (TRP balanced) or not (TRP depletion) at time –285 min. The subjects were on a low protein disk for 24 h prior to the first determination. At time 0, buspirone (30 mg) was taken orally. *Indicates a p < .05 using analysis of variance (ANOVA) for repeated measures when compared with the corresponding value obtained at baseline (time: −285 min.).

Neuroendocrine Assessments

Prolactin

During the initial depletion phase, prior to buspirone administration, there was a highly significant time effect [F1,22 = 20.6, p < .001]. There were, however, no significant order [F1,22=0.1, p = .8], subject [F11,12 = 0.8, p = .6] or time X treatment [F1,22 = 1.0, p = .3] effects. During the buspirone phase, there were no significant order [F2,21 = 0.2, p = .8], subject [F22,22 = 2.0, p = .06], or time X treatment effects [F2,20 = 0.40, p = .7]. In contrast, there was a highly significant time effect [F2,20 = 10.3, p < .001]. Although there was a significant elevation of prolactin levels 1 and 2 h after buspirone ingestion, the degree of enhancement was pronounced at time 60 [F1,22 = 14.1, p = .001], when usually a peak of prolactin is achieved following oral administration of a 5-HT1A agonist. These results indicate that, although prolactin levels fell during the day, they were then markedly enhanced by buspirone, independent of whether individuals were in a tryptophan depletion state or not (Figure 2 ).

Figure 2
figure 2

Prolactin plasma levels in 12 healthy male volunteers prior to and following the ingestion of the amino acid mixture containing tryptophan (TRP balanced) or not (TRP depletion) at time –285 min. At time 0, buspirone (30 mg) was taken orally. *Indicates a p < .05 using ANOVA for repeated measures when compared with the corresponding value obtained at baseline (time: −285 min.) prior to buspirone administration, and afterward the baseline values were those obtained at time 0. There were no statistically significant differences between the values obtained in the TRP balanced and the TRP depletion conditions.

Growth Hormone

During the initial depletion phase, prior to buspirone administration, there was no significant order [F1,22 = 0.6, p = .4], subject [F1,11 = 1.2, p = .4], treatment [F1,22 = 2.1, p = .2], or time X treatment effects [F1,22 = 0.3, p = .6]. During the buspirone phase, there was a significant treatment effect [F2,21 = 3.6, p = 0.05], but no order [F1,21 = 0.5, p = .6] or subject effects [F2,21 = 0.2, p = .8]. These results indicate that there was no significant modification of growth hormone levels following the ingestion of amino acid drink, contrary to the results obtained with prolactin, and that buspirone significantly enhanced the levels of growth hormone regardless of whether the subjects were in a tryptophan depleted state or not (Figure 3 ).

Figure 3
figure 3

Growth hormone plasma levels in 12 healthy male volunteers prior to and following the ingestion of the amino acid mixture containing tryptophan (TRP balanced) or not (TRP depletion) at time –285 min. At time 0, buspirone (30 mg) was taken orally. *Indicates a p < .05 using ANOVA for repeated measures when compared with the corresponding value obtained at time 0. There were no statistically significant differences between the values obtained in the TRP balanced and the TRP depletion conditions.

Body Temperature

A total of 19 temperature deteminations were done, 10 prior to buspirone administration and 9 afterward. Body temperature had a tendency to rise following ingestion of the amino acid drink, which most likely reflected a diurnal change. Nevertheless, body temperatures remained stable in the 3 h preceding buspirone administration (Figure 4 ). Following buspirone administration, and taking the temperature 285 min after ingestion of the amino acid drinks as the baseline values, there was a significant time effect [F1,9 = 21.2, p = .001], but no subject [F11,9 = 0.3, p = .6], treatment [F1,9 = 1.5, p = .3], and treatment X time effects [F7,3 = 0.1, p = 1.0]. Consequently, buspirone exerted a significant hypothermic effect, and although there was a trend for a greater effect in the tryptophan-depleted state that would anyhow go against the hypothesis of a presynaptic mediation, this difference did not reach the predetermined level of statistical significance (Figure 5 ).

Figure 4
figure 4

Evolution of oral temperature prior to and following the ingestion of the amino acid mixture containing tryptophan (TRP balanced) or not (TRP depletion) at time –285 min. The subjects were allowed to move around in the observation room until time –60 min. when they remained lying in bed. There were no statistically significant differences between the values obtained in the two conditions.

Figure 5
figure 5

Hypothermic response obtained with the oral administration of buspirone (30 mg) in the same 12 male healthy volunteers tested 285 min. after the ingestion of an amino acid mixture containing tryptophan (TRP balanced) or not (TRP depletion). The baseline values were those illustrated at time 0 in Figure 4. The difference between the two curves was not statistically significant.

DISCUSSION

The results of the present study indicate that the hypothermia induced by the 5-HT1A agonist buspirone in humans was not attenuated in a 5-HT depleted state (Figure 5), just as for the prolactin and the growth hormone responses (Figures 2 and 3). These data, therefore, imply that all three responses are mediated by an increased activation of postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors, and not by a decreased occupation of such receptors by endogenous 5-HT resulting from the agonistic activity of buspirone at the 5-HT1A autoreceptor.

Given the results obtained in the present experiments, the first possibility to consider is the adequacy of the tryptophan depletion paradigm to lower synaptic 5-HT availability. As depicted in Figure 1, total plasma tryptophan was decreased by about than 90% by the time the 5-HT1A agonist was administered. Although free plasma tryptophan, which is the important parameter for brain uptake, was not measured, previous studies clearly indicate that the two values are affected to the same extent with this dietary challenge (Young et al. 1985; Delgado et al. 1990). In addition, the following observations would support the notion that this tryptophan-dietary depletion paradigm is sufficient to exert a robust decrease in the synaptic availability of 5-HT in the human brain. This challenge has been shown to decrease tryptophan in the cerebrospinal fluid about as much as plasma tryptophan (Carpenter et al. 1998; Williams et al. 1999), but to a lesser extent than that of the metabolite 5-hydroxyindole acetic acid. Furthermore, 5-HT synthesis measured in humans, using α-[11C]methyL-tryptophan and positron emission tomography, is decreased approximately to the same extent as plasma tryptophan availability (Nishizawa et al. 1997). The possibility that the latter method is not merely a reflection of tryptophan plasma levels can tentatively be ruled out by the observations that different patient populations with similar plasma levels of tryptophan present different 5-HT synthesis rates (see Benkelfat et al. 1999; Leyton et al., 2001). Although this procedure does not trigger clear depressive symptoms in normal individuals, it is nevertheless sufficient to produce mild dysphoria, especially if they are subjected to a mild stress (Young et al., 1985; Leyton et al. 2000). Moreover, this paradigm produces, within the same time frame as that used in the present study, a resurgence of depressive symptoms in patients previously ameliorated by their antidepressant drug regimen (Delgado et al. 1990,1999; Bremner et al. 1997). Most importantly, it can produce a recurrence of depressive symptoms in remitted depressed patients who are no longer taking antidepressant drugs (Smith et al. 1997; Moreno et al. 2000; Spillmann et al. 2001). Consequently, the tryptophan depletion was likely sufficient to attenuate 5-HT transmission in the brain of subjects studied in the present experiments.

In rats, the hypothermia resulting from the systemic administration of 5-HT1A agonists is generally believed to be caused by the action of postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptor activation. The most compelling arguments in favor of this lore are the following. First, subacute 5-HT synthesis inhibition does not attenuate this response (Hjörth 1985; Hutson et al. 1987; O'Connell et al. 1992; Goodwin et al. 1985). Second, the lesioning of 5-HT neurons using the neurotoxin 5,7-dihydroxytryptamine also leaves unaffected the 5-HT1A-induced hypothermia (Millan et al. 1993). The observation, however, that intraraphe injection of 5-HT or of the 5-HT1A agonist 8-OH-DPAT produces hypothermia in rats may shed some doubt as to the postsynaptic mediation of this decrease in core temperature (Hillegaart 1991). It is nevertheless conceivable that if the solution injected diffused to the Sylvius aqueduct, located immediately above the dorsal raphe, a small amount of agonist could find its way to the third ventricle and reach to the hypothalamus located just rostrally to the dorsal raphe. Indeed, the above-mentioned experiments were carried out using 0.5 μL, whereas Bonvento et al. (1992) showed that only a volume of 0.1 μL of [3H]8-OH-DPAT injected in that region of the brainstem remained confined to the dorsal raphe. Furthermore, it was noted in the work of Higgins et al. (1988) that the amount of 8-OH-DPAT (injected directly into the dorsal raphe) necessary to produce hypothermia was 25 times greater than that necessary to trigger behavioral modifications.

Another line of evidence in favor of the hypothermia being mediated by postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors stems from experiments examining the effects of repeated electroconvulsive shocks (ECS) on this parameter. This procedure has repeatedly been shown to attenuate the 8-OH-DPAT-induced hypothermia in rats (Goodwin et al. 1985; Blier and Bouchard 1992; Stockmeier et al. 1992). When the sensitivity of the 5-HT1A autoreceptor in the dorsal raphe, the presumed site of action of 5-HT1A agonists on core temperature, was assessed using direct microiontophoretic applications of 5-HT and 8-OH-DPAT, the response of 5-HT neurons was unaltered in ECS-treated rats (Blier and Bouchard 1992). In contrast, following repeated ECS, [3H]8-OH-PAT binding density was markedly decreased in rat hypothalamus (Stockmeier et al. 1992), a structure being a much more likely candidate to mediate the hypothermic response. Indeed, the neurons ultimately responsible for the decrease in body temperature are probably located in the medial preoptic region of the hypothalamus. This is based, in part, on the observations that increasing 5-HT in the medial preoptic region, either by stimulating the raphe nuclei or directly injecting 5-HT into this region, induces a decrease in core temperature in the rat (Lin et al. 1983). Nevertheless, the possibility that the 8-OH-DPAT-induced hypothermia could in fact be mediated by somatodendritic 5-HT1A autoreceptors of 5-HT neurons in the median raphe has to be considered. The medial preoptic region of the hypothalamus, which is located immediately adjacent to the third ventricle, is innervated, however, to only a small extent by fibers originating from the median raphe (Vertes and Martin 1988). It would therefore be unlikely that the administration of 8-OH-DPAT, which suppresses the firing activity of 5-HT neurons in the median and dorsal raphe (Sinton and Fallon 1988; Blier et al. 1990), and consequently reduces 5-HT release in the hypothalamus (Auerbach et al. 1989), would induce hypothermia by such a presynaptic mechanism.

In mice, the 5-HT1A receptor-induced hypothermia may appear to be mediated through 5-HT1A autoreceptor activation (Goodwin et al. 1985). Not all studies support this contention, however. For instance, Meller et al. (1992) did not observe any attenuation of the 5-HT1A receptor-induced hypothermia following three doses of PCPA to decrease 5-HT synthesis. Had the hypothermia been mediated in mice by the 5-HT1A autoreceptor, it should have been decreased by the 5-HT synthesis inhibitor. In addition, the dorsal raphe is generally considered to be endowed with a significant receptor reserve with respect to 5-HT1A autoreceptor function, which is not what these investigators have found. From a theoretical point of view, it is difficult to imagine that in rats and mice the same biological parameter could be mediated by diametrically opposed changes in 5-HT transmission, i.e., a presynaptic mediation leading to decreased 5-HT transmission and a postsynaptic one producing an increase in 5-HT transmission.

In conclusion, the bulk of the evidence indicates that the 5-HT1A receptor-induced hypothermia is mediated by postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptor activation in laboratory animals, and the present study, presumably carried out under adequate conditions, did not generate evidence to the contrary in humans. One may thus conclude that this physiological response in humans should not be used as a probe for the 5-HT1A autoreceptor. Such an erroneous use of this challenge may lead to false conclusions in disease states on receptor responsiveness, or to misleading information with respect to the effects of psychotropic drug treatments.