Main

Elevated plus-mazes exploit rodents’ innate fear of novel, open areas; that is, animals spend significantly less time exploring the open and exposed elevated arm of a maze, compared with the enclosed elevated arm (Pellow et al. 1985; Pellow and File 1986; Lister 1987). These characteristic patterns of behavior have been used extensively to highlight the anti-anxiety effects of both traditional and non-traditional anxiolytic agents (for reviews see Hogg 1996; Rodgers 1997). Generally, animals treated with anxiolytic compounds such as the benzodiazepines (e.g., diazepam, chlordiazepoxide) spend significantly more time exploring the open arms compared with vehicle controls (Pellow and File 1986). Furthermore, anxiolytic drugs have also been shown to reduce ethologically based defensive withdrawal behaviors that are commonly exhibited by rodents when they are exposed to potentially threatening situations (Blanchard et al. 1990). Specifically, in the elevated plus-maze, anxiolytic drugs reduce the number of stretch-attend postures (defined as exploration of the open arm with the front part of the body, while the hind region remains in the closed arm, see Cruz et al. (1994)). Further, anxiolytic drugs have also been shown to increase the number of exploratory head dips on the open arms (Griebel et al. 1997; Dalvi and Rodgers 1999; Silva and Brandao 2000). Conversely, known anxiogenic compounds such as the benzodiazepine receptor inverse agonist FG-7142, and pentylenetetrazol, have been shown to produce pro-anxiety, or anxiogenic effects in the mouse and rat elevated plus-mazes, including reduced time spent exploring the open arm and reduced open arm entries (Pellow and File 1986; Lister 1987; Cruz et al. 1994; Cole et al. 1995; Rodgers et al. 1995).

There are pharmacological agents that have reduced affinity for rat and mouse receptors, compared with their human homologs, for example, 5-HT1D receptor ligands, adrenergic β3 agonists, and neurokinin NK1 receptor antagonists (Beresford et al. 1991; Gitter et al. 1991; Limberger et al. 1991; Bruinvels et al. 1992; Liggett 1992; Arch and Wilson 1996). In these cases, preclinical investigation is most appropriate in species with similar receptor and ligand binding characteristics to human. Of current interest, selective NK1 receptor antagonists are being developed for the treatment of anxiety and depression (Kramer et al. 1998). The majority of these NK1 antagonists have reduced affinity for the rat and mouse NK1 receptor (Beresford et al. 1991; Gitter et al. 1991; Saria 1999), and therefore, preclinical efficacy has been difficult to establish in traditional rodent models of anxiety and depression. There are NK1 antagonists that have comparable affinity at rat and human NK1 receptors and have been tested in traditional models of anxiety and depression. Examples are NKP-608, which has been shown to have anxiolytic-like and antidepressant-like properties in social interaction and chronic mild stress models (File 2000; Papp et al. 2000; Vassout et al. 2000), and FK-888 and WIN51,708, which produced anxiolytic-like effects in mouse and rat elevated plus-mazes, respectively (Teixeira et al. 1996; Nikolaus et al. 1999). Furthermore, research using NK1 receptor knockout mice have shown that the deletion of the NK1 receptor results in a profile of reduced anxiety-like behaviors in a number of models including elevated plus-maze, novelty suppressed feeding, and maternal separation-induced vocalizations (Rupniak et al. 2000; Santarelli et al. 2001). However, in general, preclinical research with NK1 antagonists has, by pharmacological necessity, focused on species such as guinea pig and gerbil. These species have NK1 receptors closer in homology to human NK1 receptors, and therefore bind NK1 receptor antagonists with a similar affinity to human (Beresford et al. 1991; Gitter et al. 1991).

Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus) are small, easily handled rodents that are used routinely in a hind leg foot-tapping model, a pharmacodynamic assay designed to assess CNS penetration of NK1 antagonists (Graham et al. 1993; Bristow and Young 1994; Rupniak and Williams 1994). However, to date, there are few reports describing the behavior of gerbils in traditional models of anxiety. Early work by Margaret Cutler and colleagues, using gerbils, described the anxiolytic-like effects of selective serotonin 5-HT3 receptor antagonists in social interaction and light-dark box assays (Cutler 1990; Cutler and Piper 1990). Of the more recent publications, Ballard et al. (2001) used a fear-conditioning paradigm to demonstrate anxiolytic-like effects of diazepam, and the NK1 antagonists MK-869 and CP-99,994. Additionally, Sandra File and colleagues demonstrated the anxiolytic-like effects of diazepam and the NK1 antagonist L-760,735 in a gerbil social interaction assay (File et al. 2001; Cheeta et al. 2001). Interestingly, the foot-tapping response is an innate behavioral response exhibited when gerbils are exposed to stressful, frightening situations, such as a threat from a predator (Clark and Galef 1982; Ellard et al. 1990; Field and Sibold 1999). Gerbils may use the foot-tapping response to warn of potentially dangerous situations. From personal observations, hind foot tapping can be observed in the laboratory during stressful procedures such as injection or being placed into an aversion environment (see also Clark and Galef 1981). Furthermore, foot tapping can be induced non-pharmacologically using aversive stimuli such as foot shock (Routtenberg and Kramis 1967; Ballard et al. 2001).

The aim of the present study was to expand the development of anxiety models for the gerbil further, by determining whether the elevated plus-maze could be adapted for the gerbil. A specially designed elevated plus-maze was built, testing conditions were optimized, and it was determined that gerbils exhibited an anxiety-like behavioral profile similar to that seen in rats and mice. Following behavioral characterization, the gerbil elevated plus-maze was validated using a range of pharmacologically active compounds. Specifically, the standard anxiolytic drugs, diazepam, chlordiazepoxide, and buspirone, were tested as positive controls, along with the tricyclic antidepressant, imipramine, the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) antidepressants, fluoxetine, and paroxetine, as well as the antipsychotic, haloperidol, and the psychostimulant, amphetamine. Furthermore, the β-carboline, FG-7142, was tested for anxiogenic activity. Finally, to address any non-specific effects arising from motoric stimulation, the effects of specific drugs on exploration and ambulation were tested using a locomotor activity paradigm. Some of this work has been presented previously in abstract form (Varty et al. 2000).

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals and Housing

Female Mongolian gerbils (Charles River Laboratories, Kingston, NY) weighing 30–50 g were used for all studies. On arrival at the holding facility, gerbils were housed three per cage, with food and water available ad libitum, in a room maintained under constant temperature (22 ± 1°C) and humidity (50%). Gerbils were maintained on a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on 7:00 A.M.) and all behavioral testing was conducted during the light phase (between the hours of 10 A.M. and 4 P.M.). Gerbils were allowed one week to acclimate to the change in environment before any testing began. Studies were conducted across all phases of the estrous cycle. All studies were carried out in accordance with the National Institute of Health ‘Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals’ and were conducted at AAALAC (Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care)-accredited facilities under guidelines established by the Schering-Plough Animal Care and Use Committee.

Gerbil Elevated Plus-Maze

Testing was carried out on a Plexiglas plus-maze elevated to a height of 35 cm. The apparatus consisted of two open arms (30 × 8 cm), and two closed arms of the same dimensions with clear Plexiglas walls 20 cm high, arranged so that both open and closed arms faced each other. The walls of the closed arms were clear to allow for constant illumination in all parts of the maze. The floor of the maze was constructed of black Plexiglas with 6.5 mm diameter holes incorporated into the floor, approximately 10 mm apart. These holes allowed the gerbils to grip the surface, as their movement appeared to be impaired on the slick Plexiglas surfaces traditionally used to construct rat and mouse elevated plus-mazes.

Testing Procedure

Gerbils were placed into the center area of the maze facing an open arm. An observer, situated approximately 2 m from the maze, recorded the amount of time each gerbil spent exploring the open and closed arms, and the number of entries into the open and closed arms, for the duration of a 5-min test session. An entry into a specific arm was scored when the gerbil placed all four paws into the arm. Risk-assessment behaviors were also measured. Specifically, the number of head dips on the open arm and the number of protected stretch-attend postures (exploration of the open arm with the front part of the body, while the hind region remains in the closed arm) were recorded. The maze was thoroughly cleaned with dilute soap solution before testing each gerbil.

Initial studies were designed to determine the optimal testing conditions for subsequent drug studies. As light intensity is an important factor when using the elevated plus-maze in rats and mice (Griebel et al. 1993; Hogg 1996), the optimal light level for testing gerbils was first determined. The above behaviors were scored in separate groups of gerbils tested under low light conditions (5-Lux), and high light conditions (500-Lux).

For drug studies, gerbils were administered the drug, and following a specific pretreatment time, the gerbils were placed onto the maze. The observer was blind to the treatment groups and treatments were tested in a counter-balanced, random order. One person observed all the studies, eliminating the need for inter-rater comparison. All studies testing for anxiolytic-like effects were conducted under high light conditions (500-Lux). The drugs tested for anxiolytic-like activity were the benzodiazepines, diazepam (0.03–3 mg/kg), and chlordiazepoxide (0.3–10 mg/kg), the serotonin 5-HT1A partial agonist, buspirone (1–30 mg/kg), the tricyclic antidepressant, imipramine (1–30 mg/kg), the SSRI antidepressants, fluoxetine (1–30 mg/kg), and paroxetine (0.3–10 mg/kg), the antipsychotic, haloperidol (1–10 mg/kg), and the psychostimulant, amphetamine (1–10 mg/kg). The study testing the anxiogenic compound, FG-7142 (1-30 mg/kg), was conducted under low light conditions (5-Lux).

Based on the factor analyses described by Cruz et al. (1994) and Rodgers and Johnson (1995), anxiolytic-like effects were loaded on the behavioral measures of time spent in the open arm (expressed as percentage of total test time), the number of open arm entries (expressed as percentage of total arm entries), the number of stretch-attend postures, and the number of open arm head dips. For example, an anxiolytic drug may be expected to have any combination of the following effects: Increased percentage of time spent on the open arm, increased percentage of entries into the open arm, reduced number of stretch-attend postures, and/or increased number of head dips. Effects of locomotion and exploration of the maze were loaded on the number of closed arm entries, that is, a drug that causes sedation and reduces locomotion should reduce the number of closed arm entries, whereas a drug that causes stimulation and increases locomotion should increase the number of closed arm entries.

Measurement of Locomotor Activity in the Gerbil

Locomotor activity (LMA) was measured using an automated photobeam system (Digiscan, Accuscan Instruments, Columbus, OH). Briefly, gerbils were treated with vehicle or a dose of drug, and following an appropriate pretreatment time (5 min for amphetamine, 30 min for other drugs), were placed into the corner of one of eight Plexiglas LMA chambers (42 × 42 × 30 cm; L × W × H). Drug doses were balanced across the eight LMA chambers to allow for any differences in sensitivity between the chambers. Horizontal activity was recorded as consecutive breaks in a series of 16 photobeams, spaced 25 mm apart and 15 mm above the floor. Horizontal activity (measure of activity detected as breaks in beams) and total distance traveled (measure of ambulation) were recorded by a computer. Activity was measured for a 60 min test period.

Drugs

Buspirone hydrochloride and fluoxetine hydrochloride were purchased from Tocris Cookson Inc. (Baldwin, MO). Haloperidol, amphetamine sulfate, diazepam, chlordiazepoxide hydrochloride, and FG-7142 (N-methyl-β-carboline-3-carboxamide) HBC complex were purchased from RBI/Sigma (Natick, MA). Imipramine and paroxetine were synthesized by the Chemical Research Department at the Schering-Plough Research Institute (Kenilworth, NJ). FG-7142 was administered in distilled water. All other drugs were administered in 0.4% methylcellulose at a volume of 5–10 ml/kg (depending on solubility), 30 (chlordiazepoxide, diazepam, buspirone, haloperidol, amphetamine) or 60 (fluoxetine, paroxetine, imipramine) min before testing in the elevated plus-maze. Drugs were administered via subcutaneous (s.c. - buspirone, haloperidol, amphetamine), intraperitoneal (i.p. - chlordiazepoxide, diazepam, FG-7142), or oral (p.o. - fluoxetine, paroxetine, imipramine) routes. Doses are expressed as free base.

Statistical Analysis

Data from the study of the 5-Lux and 500-Lux light conditions were directly compared using unpaired t-tests. For the elevated plus-maze drug studies, arm time, arm entries, and stretch-attend postures were analyzed using one-way analyses of variance (ANOVA) with drug treatment as a between-subjects factor. The head dips measure was analyzed using a Kruskal-Wallis non-parametric test. In the locomotor activity studies, horizontal activity and total distance traveled data were totaled for the 60 min test and analyzed using one-way ANOVAs with drug treatment as a between-subjects factor. The accepted level of significance for all statistical tests was p < .05 and where appropriate, post-hoc analyses were carried out using Dunnett's t-test. All statistical analyses were conducted using GraphPad Instat (San Diego, CA).

RESULTS

Optimization of Testing Conditions

Gerbils spent significantly more time in the closed arm of the elevated plus-maze under both the low and high light conditions (70% under the 5-Lux light, 90% under the 500-Lux). Comparing the 5 and 500-Lux conditions, gerbils spent less time on the open arm under the 500-Lux condition (Figure 1, panel A). Percent open arm entries was also reduced under the 500-Lux condition (Figure 1, panel B), but this was not significant. Additionally, gerbils made fewer closed arm entries under the 500-Lux light, and the total number of arm entries was lower at 500-Lux. Furthermore, gerbils made more stretch-attend postures under the 500-Lux condition (Figure 1, panel E). There was no difference in the number of head dips between the light conditions (Figure 1, panel F).

Figure 1
figure 1

Effects of light intensity on (A) percentage of open arm time, (B) percentage of open arm entries, (C) closed arm entries, (D) total arm entries, (E) stretch-attend postures, and (F) head dips in the gerbil elevated plus-maze. X-axes: Light Intensity (Lux); Y-axes: Behavioral Measures. Values are mean ± SEM. *** p < .01, * p < .05 vs. low light (5 Lux) condition. n = 10 per group.

Therefore, the overall behavioral profile exhibited under high level 500-Lux light appeared optimal for detecting anxiolytic-like drug effects as the gerbils spent less time in the open arm, made fewer open arm entries and more stretch-attend postures, thus providing the largest window for measuring anxiolytic-like activity. Interestingly, the 5-Lux light condition produced a behavioral profile suitable for testing anxiogenic drugs, i.e., high level of open arm exploration.

Anxiolytic Drugs

The benzodiazepines diazepam and chlordiazepoxide increased the percentage of time spent exploring the open arm (Figure 2). Specifically, diazepam produced a dose-related increase in the percentage of time spent on the open arm (F5,56 = 16.2, p < .0001) with significant effects at 0.3–3 mg/kg i.p. doses. Diazepam also increased the percentage of open arm entries (F5,56 = 2.7, p < .05, Figure 2), with no change in closed arm entries (see Table 1). This effect of diazepam to selectively increase the percentage of entries into the open arm without affecting closed arm entries is indicative of anxiolytic-like activity. Diazepam (1 and 3 mg/kg i.p.) increased total arm entries (F5,56 = 6.7, p < .0001, Table 1) and this increase simply reflects the anxiolytic-like effect of diazepam to increase open arm entries.

Figure 2
figure 2

Effects of the anxiolytic drugs diazepam (0.03–3 mg/kg i.p.), chlordiazepoxide (0.3–10 mg/kg i.p.), and buspirone (0.3–30 mg/kg s.c.) on percentage of open arm time and percentage of open arm entries on the gerbil elevated plus-maze. X-axes: Dose (mg/kg); Y-axes: percentage of Open Arm Time (upper panels), percentage of Open Arm Entries (lower panels). Values are mean ± SEM. ** p < .01, * p < .05 vs. vehicle. n = 9–15 per dose group.

Table 1 Effects of the Anxiolytic Drugs Diazepam, Chlordiazepoxide, and Buspirone on Behavioral Measures from the Gerbil Elevated Plus-maze

Chlordiazepoxide produced a significant increase in the percentage of time spent in the open arm (F4,64 = 5.2, p < .01) at i.p. doses from 0.3–3 mg/kg. Similar to diazepam, chlordiazepoxide also increased percentage of open arm entries (F4,64 = 3.0, p < .05) with significant increases at the 1 and 3 mg/kg doses. These anxiolytic-like effects of chlordiazepoxide were not confounded by locomotor effects as there was no effect of chlordiazepoxide on closed arm entries. The 10 mg/kg dose of chlordiazepoxide increased percentage of open arm time but the effect was not significant. The reason for the decrease in activity at the 10 mg/kg dose of chlordiazepoxide is not clear, but it was certainly not due to sedative effects, because there was no decrease in closed or total arm entries at this dose.

Diazepam and chlordiazepoxide also produced anxiolytic-like effects on the ethological measures of anxiety. Diazepam dose-dependently increased the number of head dips (p < .0001), and reduced the number of stretch-attend postures (F5,55 = 6.3, p < .001) with significant effects on both measures at i.p. doses of 1 and 3 mg/kg (Table 1). Chlordiazepoxide reduced stretch-attend postures at i.p. doses of 0.3 and 1 mg/kg (F4,64 = 4.3, p < .01). Chlordiazepoxide did not increase head dips, and instead there was a significant decrease at the 0.3 mg/kg dose (p < .05)

Buspirone produced a dose-dependent increase in percentage of open arm time (F5,61 = 8.4, p < .0001) and percentage of open arm entries (F5,61 = 15.3, p < .0001). These effects were significant at s.c. doses of 3, 10, and 30 mg/kg (Figure 2). However, buspirone also reduced the number of open (F5,61 = 3.5, p < .01), closed (F5,61 = 8.8, p < .0001), and total (F5,61 = 8.3, p < .0001) arm entries (see Table 1). The reductions in closed and total arm entries were significant for the 10 and 30 mg/kg doses suggesting that these doses may be reducing locomotor activity and exploration. Therefore, only the 3 mg/kg dose of buspirone produced an anxiolytic-like effect on percentage of open arm time without reducing indices of exploration and locomotion, i.e. entries into the closed arm. Buspirone reduced stretch-attend postures at the 10 and 30 mg/kg s.c. doses (Table 1); however, these effects may also reflect the reduced locomotor activity at these doses. There was a trend for the 3 mg/kg dose of buspirone to reduce stretch-attend postures and increase head dips (see Table 1).

Antidepressant Drugs

The tricyclic antidepressant imipramine had no effect on percentage of open arm time (Figure 3), percentage of open arm entries or head dips (Table 2), and increased the total number of arm entries (F4,49 = 2.7, p < .05) with a significant increase at 10 mg/kg p.o. (Table 2). However, imipramine reduced the number of stretch-attend postures (F4,49 = 3.7, p = .01) with significant anxiolytic-like reductions at the 10 and 30 mg/kg doses (Table 2). This effect did not appear to be due to motoric effects as there was no change in closed arm entries.

Figure 3
figure 3

Effects of the antidepressant drugs imipramine (1–30 mg/kg p.o.), fluoxetine (1–30 mg/kg, p.o.), and paroxetine (0.3–10 mg/kg p.o.) on percentage of open arm time and percentage of open arm entries on the gerbil elevated plus-maze. n = 10–16 per dose group. ** p < .01, * p < .05 vs. vehicle.

Table 2 Effects of the Antidepressant Drugs Imipramine, Fluoxetine, and Paroxetine on Behavioral Measures from the Gerbil Elevated Plus-maze

There was a significant overall effect of the SSRI, fluoxetine, to increase the percentage of time in the open arm (F4,49 = 2.8, p < .05), but no effect on the percentage of open arm entries (Figure 3). Fluoxetine increased closed (F4,49 = 5.5, p < .01) and total (F4,49 = 5.4, p < .01) arm entries (Table 2). Fluoxetine had no effect on stretch-attend postures or head dips.

The SSRI, paroxetine, produced anxiolytic-like effects on a number of measures. Specifically, paroxetine increased percentage of time in the open arm (F4,79 = 9.7, p < .0001) and percentage of open arm entries (F4,79 = 3.1, p < .05), with significant increases at the 10 mg/kg p.o. dose (Table 2 and Figure 3). Furthermore, paroxetine increased head dips significantly at the 3 and 10 mg/kg doses (p < .0001). There was also a trend for paroxetine to reduce stretch-attend postures. Closed arm entries were increased (F4,79 = 8.1, p < .0001), with a significant increase at the 10 mg/kg dose, and total arm entries were increased (F4,79 = 20.7, p < .0001) at the 1, 3 and 10 mg/kg doses (Table 2).

Haloperidol and Amphetamine

Haloperidol and amphetamine had no anxiolytic-like effect in the gerbil elevated plus-maze (Figure 4 and Table 3). Haloperidol dose-dependently reduced percentage of open arm time (F3,36 = 10.6, p < .0001) with significant effects at the 3 and 10 mg/kg s.c. doses (Figure 4). There was a trend for haloperidol to reduce percentage of open arm entries (p = .07). All doses of haloperidol reduced the number of closed arm entries (F3,36 = 16.5, p < .0001), and total arm entries (F3,36 = 25.8, p < .0001). Haloperidol reduced stretch-attend postures at doses of 3 and 10 mg/kg (F3,36 = 10.8, p < .0001), and reduced head dips at 1, 3, and 10 mg/kg doses (p < .01, Table 3). Collectively, these findings suggest that haloperidol is reducing locomotion and exploration of the maze, probably due to sedating effects.

Figure 4
figure 4

Effects of the antipsychotic haloperidol (1–10 mg/kg s.c), the psychostimulant amphetamine (1–10 mg/kg s.c), and the benzodiazepine inverse agonist FG-7142 (1–30 mg/kg, i.p.) on percentage of open arm time and percentage of open arm entries on the gerbil elevated plus-maze. n = 10–15 per dose group. Please note higher level of percentage of open arm time in the FG-7142 study due to testing gerbils under low light conditions (5-Lux). ** p < .01, * p < .05 vs. vehicle.

Table 3 Effects of the Antipsychotic Haloperidol, the Psychostimulant Amphetamine, and the Benzodiazepine Receptor Inverse Agonist FG-7142, on Behavioral Measures from the Gerbil Elevated Plus-maze

Amphetamine had no effect on any of the behavioral measures (Table 3 and Figure 4). These doses of amphetamine are known to increase locomotor activity in laboratory animals but there was no evidence of locomotor stimulation on the elevated plus-maze reflected by no obvious increase in the entry measures. At the 10 mg/kg s.c. dose of amphetamine, there was in fact a trend for amphetamine to decrease the number of arm entries.

FG-7142

Under low light conditions, FG-7142 reduced percentage of time in the open arm (F4,50 = 4.8, p < .01) with significant decreases from vehicle at 3–30 mg/kg (Figure 4). There was a trend for a decrease in percentage of open arm entries but this did not reach significance. There was a main effect of FG-7142 on closed arm entries (F4,50 = 3.1, p < .05), with a significant increase at 30 mg/kg. There was no effect on total arm entries. FG-7142 increased stretch-attend postures at 30 mg/kg (F4,50 = 4.3, p < .01), and decreased open arm head dips at doses of 3 and 30 mg/kg (p < .05).

Locomotor Activity Studies

Data from the LMA studies are described in Table 4. Amphetamine increased both horizontal activity (F4,25 = 8.5, p < .001) and total distance traveled (F4,25 = 3.7, p < .05). Horizontal activity was increased significantly at doses of 1–10 mg/kg, whereas total distance traveled was increased at doses of 3 and 10 mg/kg. Imipramine and fluoxetine had no effect on either measure of locomotor activity. Paroxetine had a significant effect of horizontal activity (F4,25 = 5.7, p < .01); specifically, activity was decreased at 0.3 and 1 mg/kg doses. There was no effect of paroxetine on total distance traveled.

Table 4 Effects of Amphetamine, Imipramine, Fluoxetine, and Paroxetine on Spontaneous Locomotor Activity in the Gerbil

DISCUSSION

The purpose of these studies was to adapt a commonly used anxiety paradigm, the elevated plus-maze, for the gerbil. An initial finding was the observation that the elevated plus-maze needed to be adapted to accommodate the hopping-style locomotion of the gerbil. With this hopping motion, it was clearly difficult for gerbils to maintain adequate traction on the smooth Plexiglas surfaces of rat and mouse elevated plus-mazes. To overcome this problem, a novel plus-maze was constructed with 6.5 mm diameter holes located 10 mm apart over the entire surface of the maze.

Using this adapted plus-maze, optimal light conditions of approximately 500-Lux were established for testing gerbils. Based on findings from our lab, the need for high level light in gerbils is similar to rats, but opposite to mice, which typically exhibit an anxiogenic profile under low level light conditions. However, when comparing the baseline data in the gerbil to the published literature in rats and mice, data from the gerbil appeared similar to that reported in the mouse, with comparable amounts of open arm time, open arm entries, closed arm entries, stretch-attend postures, and head dips (see Cruz et al. 1994; Rodgers and Johnson 1995). It is important to reiterate that the present studies used female gerbils. Although the effects of the estrous cycle have not been examined in a specific study, the described studies were conducted during all stages of the estrous cycle and no obvious behavioral effects of estrous were noted. Further studies in male gerbils are warranted to establish whether there is a similar behavioral profile in the elevated plus-maze across both genders.

Interestingly, the low level light produced a behavioral profile in the gerbil that looked appropriate for testing anxiogenic drugs. Typically, it has been difficult to test anxiogenic drugs in the elevated plus-maze due to the low baseline level of open arm exploration (10–20% open arm time) that is difficult to reduce further. However, the 30–50% level of exploration in the gerbil under low level lighting appeared to be suitable for testing anxiogenic drugs. To test this hypothesis, the anxiogenic, β-carboline, FG-7142, was administered to gerbils under the 5-Lux light condition. FG-7142 produced a behavioral profile consistent with anxiogenesis (i.e., reduced percentage of open arm time and open arm entries, increased stretch-attend postures, and reduced open arm head dips). These findings are in accordance with the published effects of FG-7142 in rat and mouse elevated plus-mazes (Pellow and File 1986; Lister 1987; Cruz et al. 1994; Cole et al. 1995; Rodgers et al. 1995).

The main aim of these studies was to pharmacologically validate the gerbil elevated plus-maze by testing standard anxiolytic drugs such as the benzodiazepines and 5-HT1A partial agonists, as well as antidepressants, a stimulant and an antipsychotic drug. The benzodiazepines, diazepam and chlordiazepoxide, and the 5-HT1A partial agonist, buspirone, produced anxiolytic-like activity across a number of measures. However, the anxiolytic-like effects of buspirone at doses of 10 and 30 mg/kg were also accompanied by effects on locomotion, making interpretation of the data difficult, and suggesting that the effects of the 10 and 30 mg/kg doses of buspirone on the anxiolytic measures may be due to sedation, rather than anxiolysis (see Cole and Rodgers 1994). However, the 3 mg/kg dose appeared to selectively increase percentage of open arm time without affecting locomotion, suggesting that, at least in the gerbil, this is an anxiolytic-like dose. These complex findings with buspirone are in accordance with the rat and mouse elevated plus-maze literature, that describes either anxiogenic-like, anxiolytic-like, and/or sedative effects following buspirone treatment (Pellow and File 1986; Moser 1989; Lee and Rodgers 1991; Cole and Rodgers 1994; Cao and Rodgers 1997; Collinson and Dawson 1997). Overall, the gerbil elevated plus-maze appeared to have good predictive validity for detecting the anxiolytic-like effects of the benzodiazepine class of drugs. This preliminary study also suggests that the model may be sensitive to the anxiolytic-like effects of 5-HT1A agonists, although this class of compound produced a different behavioral profile than the benzodiazepines. In particular, there appeared to be a narrow efficacious window with buspirone, compared with the benzodiazepines, in terms of anxiolytic versus sedative effects.

Currently, clinical anxiety disorders are being increasingly treated with agents that are traditionally considered as antidepressants, e.g., fluoxetine and paroxetine (Schatzberg 2000; Zohar and Westenberg 2000), despite these agents having limited anxiolytic-like activity in preclinical models, especially rat and mouse elevated plus-mazes. It is therefore important to identify animal models and/or species that more accurately predict the clinical efficacy of non-traditional anxiolytic agents such as the SSRI antidepressants. In these studies, we tested the tricyclic antidepressant, imipramine, and the SSRIs, fluoxetine and paroxetine, in gerbils to assess their behavioral effects following acute administration. Interestingly, all three antidepressant drugs produced some acute anxiolytic-like effects, albeit to different magnitudes. Imipramine reduced the number of stretch-attend postures, while fluoxetine and paroxetine increased the percentage of time spent exploring the open arm. The three antidepressants did not reduce closed arm entries, suggesting that there were no sedating effects. In fact, the three compounds actually appeared to increase the total number of entries into all arms of the maze suggesting, if anything, a possible simulating effect, and a profile different to the benzodiazepines and buspirone. However, this stimulant-like profile with the antidepressants appeared different from a general psychostimulant effect, because amphetamine, at behaviorally activating doses, did not increase arm entries in these studies. To further clarify these findings, imipramine, fluoxetine, and paroxetine were tested in a gerbil locomotor activity (LMA) paradigm for effects on spontaneous locomotion, and compared with amphetamine, which is known to stimulate locomotor activity in rats and mice (Taylor and Snyder 1970; Glick 1972; Creese and Iverson 1973). The dose ranges tested were identical to those tested in the elevated plus-maze studies. In contrast to amphetamine, that produced a marked increase in LMA, the antidepressant drugs did not increase LMA at doses that were clearly anxiolytic in the elevated-plus maze. These data suggest that the anxiolytic-like effects of these doses of the antidepressant drugs were not due to locomotor stimulation.

Interestingly, the anxiolytic-like profile of the tricyclic antidepressant, imipramine, was different to the profile of the SSRIs, fluoxetine and paroxetine. Imipramine had no effect on the open arm time and entry measures, and specifically reduced stretch-attend postures. Conversely, the SSRIs increased open arm time and entries and had no effect of stretch-attend postures. These effects may suggest some mechanistic difference in the anxiolytic-like properties of tricyclic antidepressants, compared with the SSRIs, but further studies examining the effects of other tricyclic and SSRI antidepressants are warranted to expand the present findings. Furthermore, future studies should also examine other classes of antidepressants, including the monoamine oxidase inhibitors (MAOIs), and serotonin and norepinepherine reuptake inhibitors (SNRIs), to determine, first, whether these classes are anxiolytic-like in the gerbil elevated plus-maze, and second, how any such anxiolytic-like activity compares to the tricyclic and SSRI antidepressants.

Of the three antidepressant drugs studied in the present experiments, paroxetine produced the strongest anxiolytic-like effect with increases in open arm exploration, open arm entries, and head dips. Furthermore, paroxetine appeared the most potent with anxiolytic-like effects at a 10 mg/kg dose, rather than the 30 mg/kg doses of imipramine and fluoxetine. Recently, paroxetine has been approved for the treatment of social phobia (Schatzberg 2000; Zohar and Westenberg 2000) and is currently in clinical trials for the treatment of Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD). The data in the present study support the potential anti-anxiety effects of paroxetine by demonstrating the acute anxiolytic-like effects of the drug in an elevated plus-maze that exploits neophobia in animals.

It is important to note that previous studies in rat and mouse elevated plus-mazes have not demonstrated any acute anxiolytic-like effects of antidepressant drugs. For example, Griebel et al. (1997), Lister (1987), and Cole and Rodgers (1994) all reported that imipramine was inactive in rat and mouse elevated plus-maze. Similarly, fluoxetine and paroxetine did not exhibit any anxiolytic-like activity in rat and mouse elevated plus-maze, and in fact produced anxiogenic effects (Silva et al. 1999; Kurt et al. 2000; Silva and Brandao 2000; Koks et al. 2001). However, in contrast to the data generated in rat and mouse elevated plus-mazes, imipramine, fluoxetine, and paroxetine have been shown to produce anxiolytic-like actions in a number of other animal models of anxiety, including rat social interaction (Lightowler et al. 1994), shock- or separation-induced vocalizations in rats and guinea pigs (Molewijk et al. 1995; Molewijk et al. 1996; Kramer et al. 1998; Schreiber et al. 1998; Podhorna and Brown 2000), and the four-plate test in mice (Hascoet et al. 2000). Therefore, it is of interest that the gerbil elevated plus-maze appears to corroborate the anxiolytic findings with antidepressants in other in-vivo anxiety models and the recent findings in human patients. The gerbil elevated plus-maze may therefore be a valuable alternative to the traditional rat and mouse elevated plus-mazes for examining the potential anxiolytic-like properties of novel antidepressant agents.

Of final note, the clinical anxiolytic actions of antidepressant drugs only become apparent following a period of chronic administration. However, while the effects of chronic administration of imipramine, fluoxetine, and paroxetine were beyond the scope of the present studies, those experiments are certainly warranted to determine if chronic antidepressant treatment can produce a similar, reduced, or enhanced anxiolytic-like effect in the gerbil elevated plus-maze.

In further validation studies, the dopamine D2 antagonist and antipsychotic, haloperidol, and the indirect dopamine agonist and psychostimulant, amphetamine, had no effect in the gerbil elevated plus-maze. At high doses, haloperidol produced a behavioral profile consistent with sedation (see Cole and Rodgers 1994). Surprisingly, despite using doses that stimulated locomotion in the gerbil, amphetamine did not appear to increase overall exploration or locomotion in the elevated plus-maze. In fact, the 10 mg/kg dose of amphetamine appeared to reduce entries, which may have been due to the appearance of stereotyped behavior(s), or the known anxiogenic effects of amphetamine (Pellow et al. 1985; Lister 1987; Lapin 1993).

The main aim of these studies was to determine if the elevated plus-maze could be adapted for the gerbil, and whether the paradigm had predictive validity for detecting anxiolytic drugs. These studies demonstrated that the gerbil displays an anxiety-like behavioral profile similar to rats and mice under specific light conditions. Furthermore, the gerbil elevated plus-maze was sensitive to the anxiolytic effects of benzodiazepines and the 5-HT1A partial agonist buspirone. Interestingly, the gerbil elevated plus-maze also detected some anxiolytic-like effects following acute administration of selected antidepressant drugs, particularly with the SSRI paroxetine, which is currently prescribed for the treatment of social phobia. Finally, when tested under appropriate conditions, the gerbil elevated plus-maze was also sensitive to the anxiogenic effects of the benzodiazepine receptor inverse agonist, FG-7142. In summary, the gerbil elevated plus-maze appears to be a valid model of anxiety and neophobia for detecting anxiolytic and anxiogenic drugs, similar to other anxiety models in gerbils (Ballard et al. 2001; File et al. 2001). Furthermore, the gerbil elevated plus-maze may be beneficial as a means to assay the anxiolytic-like effects of pharmacological classes that exhibit a reduced affinity for rat and mouse receptors, such as the neurokinin NK1 receptor antagonists.