Main

This study sought to determine the effect of ketamine on eye movements to test the potential of ketamine as a human model for psychosis and assess the effect of ketamine on a measure of working memory. Ketamine is a non-competitive antagonist of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors and is in current medical use as a dissociative anesthetic.

Agents that block the NMDA receptors, especially phencyclidine in animals and its congener ketamine in humans have been of interest to researchers seeking a pharmacologic model for schizophrenia over several decades (Luby et al. 1959; Javitt and Zukin 1991; Tamminga et al. 1995). Recent studies of ketamine in normal volunteers suggests ketamine reproduces several positive and possibly negative symptoms seen in schizophrenia and simulates cognitive dysfunctions often seen in schizophrenia (Ghoneim et al. 1994; Krystal et al. 1994; Malhotra et al. 1996; LaPorte et al. 1996; Newcomer et al. 1998). In a study of persons with schizophrenia, Lahti et al. (1995b) reported that ketamine briefly exacerbated specific symptoms experienced during prior episodes of psychosis. These were accompanied by changes in limbic cerebral blood flow. These and other studies raised the question of whether ketamine's effect on smooth pursuit eye movement would mimic abnormalities that are frequently observed in schizophrenia, and/or exacerbate abnormalities already present in schizophrenic volunteers.

Eye tracking dysfunction (ETD) in schizophrenia was first reported in 1908 by Diefendorf & Dodge (Diefendorf 1908) and has been replicated numerous times since physiologic recording of the phenomena by Holzman et al. (1973). ETD is stable during changes in clinical state, is unimproved by antipsychotic treatment, and is observed in unaffected as well as schizophrenia spectrum affected family members (Levy et al. 1993). These findings suggest that ETD has properties of a genetic trait marker for schizophrenia.

Generally, previous studies in schizophrenia have used measures of smooth pursuit such as closed-loop gain, which examine the final output of a highly complex and distributed system. However, it was of interest to examine the effects of ketamine on the individual components of the smooth pursuit system in order to identify specifically individual ocular motor processes and the underlying neural circuitry affected by the drug. The ocular motor system is unique in that sustained smooth pursuit eye movements are not generated in the absence of motion information. During the initial phase of smooth pursuit, i.e., pursuit initiation, the motion information is provided by a slippage of the image of the target of interest on the retina (henceforth called retinal motion signals). Measures such as eye velocity and acceleration during the initiation examine ability to process retinal motion information and integrate this into an appropriate smooth pursuit motor response. Data from only the first 100 msec of initiation are used for these purposes to assure that response is not modified based on the visual feedback. Such pursuit response, independent of the visual feedback, is called the open-loop response.

Once the image of a moving target is captured on the fovea and the eye velocity approximates the target velocity, there is only a minimum slippage of the target image on the retina. Smooth pursuit during this so called maintenance phase is mostly driven by extraretinal motion signals with corrections based on the visual feedback (i.e., retinal error) (van den Berg 1988; Barnes and Asselman 1992). Such pursuit response, which also integrates retinal error information obtained through visual feedback, is called closed-loop response. The sources of the extraretinal motion signals include a copy of the motor command (so-called efference copy) and an internal representation of the retinal slip velocity. The measures obtained under the closed-loop condition evaluate the final output of the smooth pursuit system and are not informative about the individual component ocular motor processes. Thus, deficits in processing retinal motion signals, maintaining extraretinal motion information, integrating these into a motor command, and/or generating adequate smooth pursuit motor output can affect the closed-loop pursuit.

Recent studies have examined smooth pursuit responses to only extraretinal motion signals under experimental conditions in which there is a brief target extinction and reappearance at the appropriate location on the trajectory. Neurophysiological studies in monkey suggest that neurons in the medial superior temporal (MST) region continue to keep the motion information online after target extinction (Newsome et al. 1988; Boussaoud et al. 1990). This is dependent on the smooth pursuit eye movements and thus thought to encode eye velocity or the efference copy (Newsome et al. 1988; Boussaoud et al. 1990). In contrast to the MST region, posterior parietal cortex in monkey encodes previously presented retinal motion information in the absence of eye movements (Ferrera et al. 1994; Assad and Maunsell 1995). Studies indicate that patients with schizophrenia and their non-psychotic relatives have poor smooth pursuit response to only extraretinal motion signals (Thaker et al. 1998, 1999).

The current study examined effects of ketamine on the closed-loop pursuit, pursuit response to only retinal motion signals during initiation, and responses to only extraretinal motion. The latter was of particular interest for examining the effects of ketamine and other NMDA antagonists because the concept of extraretinal motion parallels the working memory construct. Aside from its relevance to schizophrenia, the effects of ketamine on eye movements can help to clarify the role of NMDAsensitive glutamatergic transmission plays in eye movements and aspects of working memory. The effects of ketamine on working memory components of our tasks would help to clarify how ketamine affects frontal lobe function, an action already implicated in several neurochemical and imaging studies (Verma and Moghaddam 1996; Javitt et al. 1996; Moghaddam et al. 1997; Lahti et al. 1995a; Breier et al. 1997; Krystal et al. 1994).

A previous study has already assessed eye tracking during ketamine infusion using a prolonged, escalating dosage infusion (Radant et al. 1998). The results of that study showed that ketamine effects on eye movement in normal volunteers are similar in some respects to the eye movement abnormalities in schizophrenia in showing disruption in gain, and increased catch-up saccade frequency early in the infusion before significant nystagmus was noted later in the infusion and at higher doses. In this study we have used a bolus infusion of ketamine at a low dose (0.1 mg/kg) that does not produce nystagmus.

METHODS

Volunteers

Subjects were recruited from the community by newspaper advertisement and screened in a telephone interview. All subjects gave written informed consent and were paid $10 per hour for their time. Subjects were evaluated with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM III-R (SCID-NP) (Spitzer et al. 1990), Structured Interview for DSMIII-R Personality Disorders (SIDP-R) (Pfohl et al. 1989), Chapman Scales for Perceptual Aberration (Chapman and Chapman 1984), medical history, physical exam, and blood and urine laboratory studies including drug screen. Subjects were excluded for current or lifetime Axis I or II disorders, medical conditions that would render ketamine infusions unsafe and any neurological disorders. Subjects with elevated Chapman scores were excluded. Current or lifetime abuse of any substances as defined by DSMIII-R criteria or any substances found in the urine drug screen would also exclude subjects. Subjects were on no medication other than vitamins. Procedures were approved by the University of Maryland, Baltimore Internal Review Board.

Subjects included 10 males and two females, mean age 34.0 with mean socioeconomic status 2.7. All subjects had prior exposure to ketamine in bolus infusion in a laboratory setting that did not involve eye tracking tasks.

Laboratory Procedures

Eye Tracking Procedures

Targets were presented on a CRT screen 28” from the subjects eyes. After stepping to the left or right of center, the target (a cross inside a .25 × .25 degree box) began moving from the center across the screen at 9.4 or 18.7 degrees per second of constant target speed over a 24 degree span of the 15″ monitor. After several cycles of motion, the target was unpredictably blanked out (masked) for 500 msec, after which the target re-appeared at the same position and velocity as cycles without a mask. The subjects were instructed to “ … follow the target as it moves. Occasionally the target will become invisible for very brief periods. During these periods, the target will keep on moving, so continue moving your eyes to follow the invisible but moving target.”

In half the trials, the mask occurred at the beginning of the ramp (Figure 1) requiring the subject to change directions during the mask. In the other half of the trials, the mask occurred in the middle of the ramp (Figure 2) requiring the subject to continue in the same direction. One block of trials consisted of 12 presentations lasting 3.5 minutes. Half of the trials used 9.4 and the other half 18.7 degree/second target speeds.

Figure 1
figure 1

The upper left panel displays the target velocity of a single ramp-mask-ramp trial. The expanded area shows an example of measures during a mask at the beginning of the ramp with eye velocity and eye position graphed

Figure 2
figure 2

The upper left panel displays the target velocity of a single ramp-mask-ramp trial with the mask in the middle of the ramp. The expanded area shows the eye velocity continuing through E and then drops during ER. The residual predictive pursuit gain is the ratio of eye velocity during ER to the target velocity before the mask

Ketamine Infusions

On each of two study days, subjects had an intravenous catheter placed with a drip of normal saline to keep open the vein. Subject's pulse and oxygen saturation were continuously monitored. After two baseline eye tracking blocks, subjects were injected with ketamine 0.1 mg/kg or a placebo bolus injection over 1 minute. Subjects were then tested on two eye tracking blocks lasting a total of 10 minutes. Subjects were then assessed for symptoms and recordings of blood pressure were made. Half of the subjects received ketamine on the first study day and placebo on the second in a double-blind fashion. The rest of the subjects had placebo on the first study day and ketamine the second.

The dose level of 0.1 mg/kg was chosen based on a previous dose ranging study. At this dose, mild analgesia is produced with very mild visual and bodily distortions, but no psychotic symptoms, no blood pressures or pulse changes and no optical nystagmus. The peak effect of the ketamine is approximately 3 minutes after injection and the effect lasts approximately 8 minutes.

Oculomotor Data Acquisition

Eye movement data were collected by infrared oculometry (sampling rate of 333 Hz with a time constraint of 4 milliseconds), filtered with 75 Hz low-pass filter, and digitized with a 16 bit A to D converter. Calibration and scoring were done with waveform analysis software that allowed semi-automated analysis and active monitoring by a trained scorer.

Eye Movement Measures

Eye blinks and saccades were removed from the records. Measures of performance were obtained for closed loop pursuit gain, velocity during the mask condition and during pursuit initiation.

  1. 1

    Closed loop pursuit gain was measured from the ramp preceding the mask. Smooth pursuit eye velocity values were divided by target velocity values for each 3 msec section and averaged for each trial.

  2. 2

    Three sets of measures of performance during the mask were obtained: 1) Peak gain during masks occurring at the beginning of the ramp (eye velocity/predicted target velocity). In such trials, subjects changed direction of pursuit and initiated pursuit in the anticipated direction (Figure 1). Latency: The timing of the change of direction of the eye in relation to the expected direction change of the target was measured. This change of direction latency was evaluated as an absolute value; 2) Mean gain during 101–400 msec interval of the mask occurring during a ramp (eye velocity/predicted target velocity; Figure 2); and 3) Residual predictive gain for trials with the mask occurring during the ramp. In such trials, subjects followed the target trajectory at the same velocity as before the mask for about 130 to 190 msec, presumably still partly responding to retinal motion information available just prior to the onset of the mask period. After this initial period, there was a 35–50% drop in the eye velocity and then the velocity stabilized at this new level until the end of the mask. This residual velocity is thought to reflect pursuit responses based on only extraretinal signals (Becker and Fuchs 1985) and was measured to obtain residual predictive gain (Thaker et al. 1998).

  3. 3

    Three measures of performance during initiation of each ramp was obtained using methods described elsewhere (Ross et al. 1996) (Figure 3): 1) Latency to onset of smooth pursuit; 2) Open loop acceleration from the first 100 msec of smooth pursuit, where no visual feedback is available to the oculomotor system; and 3) Open loop velocity during the first 100 msec after onset of smooth pursuit.

    Figure 3
    figure 3

    The middle panel shows initiation of eye movements at the beginning of a trial with eye velocity shown in the lower panel. Point b shows the latency of eye movements from the target onset. Following b for 100 msec is the open loop acceleration and velocity of eye movements

Data Analysis

Before final data analysis, dependent variables were plotted to insure the data corresponded to a normal distribution. Preliminary analysis was also performed for order effects (i.e., whether the subject received ketamine versus placebo first) and no differences were found. We also compared the 10 males to the two females and found no differences, so both sexes were combined for the final analysis. We also compared the two blocks of trials subsequent to ketamine injection for differences but none were found, thus allowing combining these blocks for subsequent analysis.

For each subject, data were collapsed across trials to obtain mean values on the two baseline blocks and two blocks subsequent to placebo and ketamine injection. Repeated measures analysis of variance was performed using within-subject factors of phase (baseline vs. injection), drug (placebo vs. ketamine), and target velocity (9.4 vs. 18.7 degrees per second) on the dependent variables. For statistical significance, the α level was set at 0.05. Post hoc comparisons used Tukey's honestly significant difference tests. Results are presented as mean ± standard deviation.

Results

The means, standard deviations and analysis of variance values for the measures are presented in Table 1

  1. 1

    Ketamine significantly decreased closed loop gain as determined by phase by drug interactions (F1,11 = 13.51, p < .004). Post hoc comparison suggested no effect of baseline (0.873 ± 0.088) versus placebo injection (0.849 ± 0.097) but a significant change with ketamine infusion (0.845 ± 0.082 versus 0.715 ± 0.116). Next the effects of ketamine on individual components were evaluated.

  2. 2

    Eye acceleration during initiation was significantly decreased with ketamine but not with placebo as suggested by drug by phase interaction (F1,11 = 5.66, p < .037). Probing the interaction showed no effect of placebo and significant effect of ketamine on eye acceleration (F1,11 = 7.52, p < .02). This suggests impairment in processing retinal motion information and generating a motor response. Measures of open loop pursuit latency and velocity were not significantly affected by ketamine.

  3. 3

    There was no effect of ketamine on predictive pursuit in response to extraretinal motion information. The peak gain for masks at the beginning of the ramp (F1,11 = .01, p = .941) and changes in direction latency were not significantly affected by ketamine.

  4. 4

    Ketamine decreased mean mask gain. Significant effects of ketamine as determined by phase by drug interactions were noted for mean mask gain (F1,11 = 9.48, p < .011) for masks during the ramp. There was no significant effect of placebo challenge but ketamine significantly worsened mean gain during the mask (F1,11 = 10.046, p < .009). The reduced mean mask gain with ketamine, however, may be secondary to the low pre-mask closed-loop gain and/or reflect the effects of retinal motion signals in the first 130 to 190 msec of the mask. Therefore, we examined the effects of the drug on residual predictive gain. Ketamine had no significant effect on the residual predictive gain (F1,11 = 2.70, p = .128). This suggests the decreased mean mask gain was probably secondary to the decreased closed-loop gain (No. 1 above).

  5. 5

    Total BPRS scores showed a small increase from baseline (18.36 ± 0.50) to ketamine infusion (20 ± 2.46), reflecting mild increases in withdrawal and thought disorder subscales.

Table 1 Means, Standard Deviations and Analysis of Variance Results for Eye Movement Measures

DISCUSSION

The purpose of this investigation was to determine if ketamine in low doses would produce abnormalities in the eye movements of normal volunteers that are observed in subjects with schizophrenia. Ketamine appears to produce some abnormalities in eye movements common to schizophrenia, but not all abnormalities. A traditional measure of smooth pursuit, closed loop gain showed a significant worsening during ketamine that resembles the smooth pursuit deficits seen in schizophrenia (Friedman et al. 1991). This result is similar to the findings of Radant et al. (1998). Ketamine also produced deficits in open loop acceleration that has been observed in schizophrenia (Clementz and McDowell 1994; Ross et al. 1996).

In terms of predictive pursuit, where eye movement measures are obtained during periods where the target was masked, the effect of ketamine is more complex. When the mask occurred during a ramp, mean mask gain was affected by the drug. However, this measure may be vulnerable to response based on retinal motion signals. In contrast, residual predictive gain, which is thought to be based on mainly extraretinal signals, was unaffected by ketamine. Similarly, ketamine did not disrupt the peak gain measure obtained from trials with masks at the beginning of the ramp. The latter two findings suggest that ketamine did not significantly disrupt the predictive component of the smooth pursuit function. In an earlier study comparing relatives of patients with schizophrenia and normal groups, measures of predictive pursuit were found to be impaired in relatives (Thaker et al. 1999). This measure, which is similar to working memory of target trajectory, of eye movement control is not similar between a ketamine infusion and schizophrenia.

Brain imaging studies with ketamine have been done in an attempt to delineate areas involved with its psychotomimetic effects. Breier and coworkers (1997) found increases in frontal areas and a small area of decrease in the cerebellum during a prolonged ketamine infusion and 18-F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography (PET). We have reported anterior cingulate and central frontal increases and reduced hippocampus and primary visual cortex in subjects with schizophrenia during a 0.3 mg/kg bolus injection and H215O rCBF PET study (Lahti et al. 1995a).

Our more recent PET, ketamine studies with larger numbers of normal controls confirm cingulate and frontal activations and showed a profound decrease in cerebellum (Lahti et al. 1997). Bolus injected ketamine may block the abundant R2C subtype of NMDA receptors in the granule cell layer of the cerebellum. The ketamine induced eye movement changes reported here may be best explained as mediated by the disruption of sensory motor contributions within the cerebellum to eye movements. A study of Purkinje cell activity in the monkey posterior vermis during smooth pursuit suggested that the posterior vermis is involved in processing retinal image velocity and calculating target velocity in visual space based on the summation of current retinal slip velocity and internal representation of the eye velocity (Suzuki and Keller 1988). Thus, dysfunction in vermis would affect both the initiation response as well as closed-loop pursuit gain. Caution is indicated in this interpretation of ketamine on the cerebellum. Animal studies on the binding of ketamine suggest lower affinity in the cerebellum than in the hippocampus (Yoneda and Ogita 1991) and the forebrain (Porter and Greenamyre 1995). The cerebellum is rich in the NMDAR2C subunit whereas the NMDAR2A and NMDAR2B subunits are expressed more strongly in the cortex and hippocampus (Nakanishi 1992). Also, in a recent study of subjects with cerebellar degeneration, Moschner et al. (1999) found a significant increase in latency and decreased acceleration of eye movement initiation whereas we found only decreases in acceleration. These studies suggest other areas of the brain than the cerebellum may be responsible for ketamine effect.

A second reason to be cautious in interpreting our findings is that the dose of ketamine may have been too low to elicit effects on our memory related measures. Ketamine has been shown to cause impairment in several measures of attention and memory (Malhotra et al. 1996; LaPorte et al. 1996; Newcomer et al. 1998). The study by Newcomer and coworkers (1998) showed dose dependent decreases in verbal and nonverbal declarative memory but not visual spatial delayed response. Our results appear to agree with less impairment in memory related to the visual system. Our intent in using a low dose was to avoid nystagmus and insure the subject could cooperate fully with the testing. Higher doses of ketamine may have produced significant effects on more of our measures. A third reason for caution is that our sample size may be too small to detect smaller effects. The lack of effect on the working memory of target trajectory suggests intact frontal functioning at this dose level. If our interpretation of cerebellar dysfunction is correct, it may be that ketamine disrupts an essential timing or error correction function attributed to the cerebellum in many tasks (Ivry and Keele 1989; Jueptner et al. 1995).

The results of this study seem to fit with other work that implicates the cerebellar dysfunction in schizophrenia (Andreasen et al. 1998; Andreasen 1999; Martin and Albers 1995). Ketamine at this dose level appears to target our measures involving the cerebellum more than our measures that tap cortical functions. The ketamine model of schizophrenia appears not to disrupt the eye movements that have a genetic component.