Main

During the past several years, research from our laboratory has focused on the potential involvement of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis in cocaine reinforcement. The HPA axis consists of a complex, well-regulated interaction between the brain, the anterior pituitary gland, and the adrenal cortex. The initial step in the activation of the HPA axis is the neuronally regulated secretion of the peptide corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) from the parvocellular division of the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) of the hypothalamus. CRH is released by the hypothalamus into the adenohypophyseal portal circulation to act on the pituitary to induce the secretion of adrenocorticotropin hormone (ACTH). ACTH diffuses through the general circulation until it reaches the adrenal glands. There it stimulates the biosynthesis of adrenocorticosteroids, most notably the glucocorticoids, cortisol (in humans) and corticosterone (in rats), which results in their secretion from the adrenal cortex.

Cocaine has been reported to stimulate HPA axis activity in a manner analogous to various stressors, which indicates that this system has the potential to influence many of the neurochemical and behavioral effects of the drug. For example, the acute administration of cocaine dose-dependently increases plasma concentrations of corticosterone, ACTH, and β-endorphin in rats (Moldow and Fischman 1987; Rivier and Vale 1987; Borowsky and Kuhn 1991). Cocaine also stimulates the release of ACTH and cortisol in humans (Mendelson et al. 1989; Baumann et al. 1995) and non-human primates (Sarnyai et al. 1996) by increasing the peak amplitude of secretory pulses of these hormones without altering pulse frequency, suggesting that these increases are driven by hypothalamic CRH (Mendelson et al. 1989; Teoh et al. 1994; Sarnyai et al. 1996). Cocaine-induced increases in ACTH and corticosterone in rats can be blocked by pretreatment with the CRH antagonist α-helical CRF9-41 (Sarnyai et al. 1992a), by immunoneutralization of CRH with an anti-CRH antibody (Rivier and Vale 1987; Sarnyai et al. 1992a), or by bilateral electrolytic lesions of the PVN (Rivier and Lee 1994), indicating that these increases are also mediated by the cocaine-induced release of CRH from parvocellular neurons in the PVN. Accordingly, it has been reported that cocaine administration also results in increases in hypothalamic CRH mRNA (Zhou et al. 1996; Rivier and Lee 1994) and alters CRH receptor binding measured autoradiographically in various regions of the rat brain (Goeders et al. 1990). These data suggest that the complex relationship between cocaine reinforcement and corticosterone secretion we have previously reported (Goeders and Guerin 1996; Goeders 1997) may ultimately result from actions on hypothalamic CRH. The following experiments were therefore designed to determine the effects of pretreatment with CP-154,526, a centrally active, small molecule CRH1 receptor subtype antagonist (Mansbach et al. 1997), on intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats. This compound was selected for study in these experiments because, unlike peptide antagonists such as D-Phe CRF12-41, CP-154,526 enters the brain following systemic administration (Schulz et al. 1996; McCarthy et al. 1999), which makes it a potentially more useful tool for investigating the effects of CRH in cocaine reinforcement. In fact, this compound has recently been reported to attenuate the electric footshock-induced reinstatement of cocaine- and heroin-seeking behavior (Shaham et al. 1998), suggesting the involvement of CRH in the relapse to drug seeking. We report here that CRH is involved in the maintenance of ongoing cocaine self-administration as well.

METHODS AND MATERIALS

Adult male Wistar rats (n = 58; Harlan Sprague Dawley) 80–100 days old at the start of the experiments were used. The rats were housed singly in cages equipped with a laminar flow unit and air filter in a temperature- and humidity-controlled, AAALAC-accredited animal care facility on a reversed 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 1800 h) with free access to water. Rats were also allowed free access to food until their free-feeding body weights increased to approximately 380–400 g. These rats were subsequently maintained at 85–90% of their free-feeding body weights by presentations of food pellets (P.J. Noyes, 45 mg) during the behavioral sessions and/or by supplemental post-session feeding (Purina Rat Chow) throughout the course of the experiments. All procedures were carried out in accordance with the NIH “Principles of Laboratory Animal Care” (NIH publication No. 85-23).

Each rat was implanted with a chronic indwelling jugular catheter under pentobarbital anesthesia (50 mg/kg, i.p.) with methylatropine nitrate pretreatment (10 mg/kg, i.p.) using previously reported procedures (Koob and Goeders 1989; Goeders et al. 1998). The catheter (0.012 in i.d. × 0.025 in o.d., silicone tubing) was inserted into the right posterior facial vein and pushed down into the jugular vein until it terminated outside the right atrium. The catheter was anchored to tissue in the area and continued subcutaneously to the back where it exited just posterior to the scapulae through a Marlex mesh®/dental acrylic/22-gauge guide cannula (Plastic Products) assembly that was implanted under the skin for attachment of a leash. The stainless steel spring leash (Plastic Products) was attached to the guide cannula assembly and to a leak-proof fluid swivel suspended above the cage. Tubing connected the swivel to a 20-ml syringe in a motor-driven pump (Razel) located outside the chamber. The swivel and leash assembly was counter-balanced to permit relatively unrestrained movement of the animal. The animals were injected with sterile penicillin G procaine suspension (75,000 units, i.m.) immediately before surgery, and they were allowed a minimum of 4 days to recover following surgery. The swivel and leash assembly was always connected during the experimental sessions. At the end of each session, the leash was disconnected and a dummy cannula was inserted into the guide before the rats were returned to their home cages. The patency of the catheters was tested immediately after the end of the session each Wednesday. If blood could be obtained via the catheter, then it was judged to be patent. If not, then the rat was injected via the catheter with methohexital sodium (1.5 mg, i.v.). An immediate light anesthesia indicated that the catheter was functional.

Standard plastic and stainless steel sound-attenuating operant conditioning chambers (Med-Associates, Inc.) were used to run the behavioral experiments. Each experimental chamber was equipped with two response levers (Med-Associates, Inc.) mounted on either side of a food pellet dispenser located on one wall of the chamber, and a stimulus light was located above each lever. The chambers were also equipped with an exhaust fan that supplied ventilation and white noise to mask extraneous sounds. An IBM-compatible personal computer and interface system (Med-Associates, Inc.) was used to program the procedures and collect the experimental data.

Rats were trained to respond under a multiple, alternating schedule of food reinforcement and cocaine self-administration as described previously (Goeders et al. 1998). During the food component of the schedule, a stimulus light located directly above the food response lever was illuminated to indicate the availability of food reinforcement. Initially, each depression of the food response lever resulted in a brief darkening of the food stimulus light (0.6 s) and the delivery of a food pellet (45 mg). A 25-s timeout followed the delivery of each food pellet. During this timeout, the stimulus light was darkened and responses on the food lever were counted but had no scheduled consequences. Responding on the other (i.e., cocaine) lever during the food component also had no scheduled consequences. The response requirement for the food lever was gradually increased over several sessions from continuous reinforcement to a fixed-ratio 10 (FR10) schedule whereby 10 responses were required for food presentation. Following 15 min of access to food, all stimulus lights in the chamber were darkened for a 1-min timeout. Following the timeout, the stimulus light above the cocaine response lever was illuminated to indicate the availability of cocaine. Initially, each depression of the cocaine response lever resulted in a brief darkening of the stimulus light and an infusion of cocaine (0.125, 0.25 or 0.5 mg/kg/infusion in 200 μl 0.9% NaCl delivered over 5.6 s). A 20-s timeout period followed each infusion. The response requirement for cocaine was gradually increased to a FR4 schedule of reinforcement. After 15 min of access to cocaine and another 1-min timeout, the rats were again allowed 15 min of access to the food component of the schedule. Access to food and cocaine alternated in this manner every 15 min during the 2-h behavioral sessions so that each rat was exposed to food and cocaine for four 15-min periods each. Each behavioral session began with 15 min of access to either food or cocaine, and this alternated daily. Sessions were conducted at the same time each day, Monday through Friday. Stable baselines of responding occurred when the total number of cocaine and food presentations, as well as the number of presentations during each of the four exposures each session, varied less than 10% for three consecutive sessions.

Prior to testing with CP-154,526, the rats were repeatedly exposed to cocaine extinction (Goeders et al. 1998). On extinction test days, a saline vehicle syringe was substituted for the cocaine syringe normally present, and responses on the “cocaine” lever only resulted in infusions of saline. The rats were presented with these “extinction probes” on drug test days (i.e., Tuesdays and Fridays) until stable, reproducible, “extinction-like” behavior was observed. Extinction-like behavior was deemed to occur when the rates and patterns of responding did not vary more than 10% during at least two consecutive extinction probes. Cocaine extinction probes continued to be run approximately every 2 weeks until the conclusion of all of the experiments to ensure that consistent extinction-like behavior was reliably produced whenever saline was substituted for cocaine. Similar behavior during cocaine self-administration would be expected following pretreatment with CP-154,526 if cocaine reinforcement was significantly reduced.

Once consistent and reproducible behavior during cocaine extinction was obtained, CP-154,526 testing during cocaine self-administration commenced. For the dose-response experiments, 18 rats were pretreated with CP-154,526 (10–40 mg/kg, i.p.) or vehicle (5% emulphor in 0.9% saline) 30 min prior to the start of the behavioral session. Each rat was tested with each dose of CP-154,526 at least twice. CP-154,526 was tested on Tuesdays and Fridays provided that responding returned to baseline between test days. In order to determine the effects of CP-154,526 across several doses of cocaine, separate groups of rats were trained to self-administer 0.125 (n = 14), 0.25 (n = 16) or 0.5 (n = 10) mg/kg/infusion as described above. Once stable baselines of responding with cocaine and during extinction were obtained, the rats were pretreated with vehicle or CP-154,526 (20 mg/kg, i.p.) 30 min prior to the start of the behavioral session. Data collected included the total number of infusions and food pellets delivered per session as well as the number delivered during each of the four presentations of each component of the alternating schedule. For the cocaine extinction probes, data analyses only included those data obtained once the criteria for successful extinction-like behavior were met. Significance of the differences between the various treatments was determined with a one-way or two-way analysis of variance as appropriate. Tukey's all pairwise multiple comparison procedures were then used to isolate differences between groups.

RESULTS

Stable baselines of cocaine- and food-reinforced responding were obtained following approximately 15 to 20 experimental sessions with 0.25 mg/kg/infusion cocaine. The effects of CP-154,526 on the number of drug infusions at this dose are depicted in Figure 1. A one-way analysis of variance revealed a significant effect of CP-154,526 dose on self-administration [F(5,99) = 23.176, p < 0.001], with responding on the cocaine lever significantly reduced during cocaine extinction and following pretreatment with 20–40 mg/kg CP-154,526. Since there were no statistically significant differences between the effects of 20 or 40 mg/kg CP-154,526 on cocaine self-administration, the lower dose (i.e., 20 mg/kg, i.p.) was used in all subsequent testing. The effects of CP-154,526 on the total number of infusions and food presentations delivered per session with the three different doses of cocaine (i.e., 0.125, 0.25 and 0.5 mg/kg/infusion) are shown in Figures 2 and 3, respectively. A two-way analysis of variance demonstrated a significant effect of cocaine dose [F(2,230) = 32.399, p < .001] and treatment condition [F(3,230) = 109.379, p < .001] on the total number of infusions self-administered. There was also a significant dose × treatment interaction [F(6,230) = 10.356, p < .001]. The number of infusions was significantly reduced during cocaine extinction and following pretreatment with CP-154,526 in rats trained to self-administer cocaine at each of the three doses tested (i.e., 0.125, 0.25 or 0.5 mg/kg/infusion). There was also a significant effect of cocaine dose [F(2,230) = 7.746, p < .001] and treatment condition [F(3,230) = 4.774, p = .003] on the number of food presentations per session, but no treatment × dose interaction [F(6,230) = 1.394]. Although the number of food presentations delivered during cocaine extinction (i.e., saline substitution) was significantly greater than that delivered during baseline (q = 4.825) or following pretreatment with CP-154,526 (q = 4.909), there were no other statistically significant effects, which simply indicates that the self-administered cocaine-induced intoxication produced a very modest, albeit significant, decrement in the ability of the rats to obtain as many food reinforcers as when not intoxicated (i.e., during saline substitution for cocaine).

Figure 1
figure 1

Effects of pretreatment (30 min) with CP-154,626 (0–40 mg/kg, i.p.) on intravenous cocaine self-administration (0.25 mg/kg/infusion) in rats (n = 18). The open circle represents the effects of the vehicle (5% emulphor in 0.9% saline), the open square represents responding during extinction (saline substitution) and the closed circles represent the various doses of CP-154,526. Responding was significantly decreased following 20–40 mg/kg CP-154,526. *p < .05 CP-154,526 compared to vehicle pretreatment.

Figure 2
figure 2

Effects of CP-154,526 (20 mg/kg, i.p.) on the number of infusions delivered by rats trained to self-administer three doses of cocaine (0.125, 0.25 and 0.5 mg/kg/infusion). Open bars represent baseline cocaine self-administration, striped bars represent saline infusions delivered during extinction, gray bars represent cocaine infusions delivered following pretreatment with vehicle, and closed bars represent infusions delivered following pretreatment with CP-154,526. The number of infusions was significantly reduced during extinction and following pretreatment with CP-154,526 at each of the cocaine doses tested. *p < 0.05 extinction vs. baseline and CP-154,526 vs vehicle pretreatment.

Figure 3
figure 3

Effects of CP-154,526 (20 mg/kg, i.p.) on the number of food presentations obtained by rats trained to self-administer three doses of cocaine (0.125, 0.25 and 0.5 mg/kg/infusion). Open bars represent baseline food deliveries, striped bars represent food presentations delivered during cocaine extinction, gray bars represent food presentations delivered following pretreatment with vehicle, and closed bars represent food pellets obtained following pretreatment with CP-154,526. The number of food presentations was significantly increased during extinction (saline substitution) compared to each of the other treatment conditions. *p < .05 extinction vs. baseline and CP-154,526 pretreatment.

The number of infusions delivered during each of the four 15-min self-administration bins per session for each of the three doses of cocaine is depicted in Figure 4. With 0.125 mg/kg/infusion as the self-administration dose (Figure 4, upper panel), a two-way analysis of variance revealed a significant effect of treatment condition [F(3,363) = 2.680, p < .05] and 15-min self-administration bin [F(3,363) = 154.313, p < .001] as well as a treatment × bin interaction [F(9,363) = 3.848, p < .001]. The number of infusions delivered following pretreatment with CP-154,526 was significantly decreased compared to vehicle pretreatment during each of the four 15-min self-administration bins, and there were no differences between the number of infusions delivered during cocaine extinction and following pretreatment with CP-154,526 during the second, third and fourth 15-min bin. Similar results were obtained from rats trained to self-administer 0.25 mg/kg/infusion cocaine (Figure 4, middle panel). A two-way analysis of variance indicated a significant effect of treatment condition [F(3.335) = 7.936, p < .001] and 15-min self-administration bin [F(3,335) = 90.607, p < .001] with a significant treatment × bin interaction [F(9,335) = 4.286, p < .001] as well. While the number of infusions delivered during the first 15-min bin was less than all other treatment conditions following pretreatment with CP-154,526, the number of infusions obtained during cocaine extinction and following pretreatment with CP-154,526 was significantly reduced compared to vehicle pretreatment during the second, third and fourth self-administration bins. This same trend was even observed when the cocaine dose was increased to 0.5 mg/kg/infusion (Figure 4, bottom panel). A two-way analysis of variance indicated a significant effect of treatment condition [F(3.223) = 2.917, p < .05] and 15-min self-administration bin [F(3,223) = 40.751, p < .001] with a significant treatment × bin interaction [F(9,223) = 4.124, p < .001] as well. Once again, the number of infusions obtained during cocaine extinction and following pretreatment with CP-154,526 was significantly reduced compared to vehicle pretreatment during the second, third and fourth self-administration bins. During the first 15 min, however, the number of infusions delivered following pretreatment with CP-154,526 was also significantly less than that observed following each of the other treatment conditions.

Figure 4
figure 4

(Upper panel) Effects of CP-154,526 (20 mg/kg, i.p.) during each of the four 15-min self-administration bins on infusions self-administered by rats trained to self-administer 0.125 mg/kg/infusion cocaine. Open bars represent baseline cocaine self-administration, striped bars represent saline infusions delivered during cocaine extinction, gray bars represent cocaine infusions delivered following pretreatment with vehicle, and closed bars represent cocaine infusions delivered following pretreatment with CP-154,526. The number of infusions delivered during extinction was significantly less than baseline in the second, third and fourth 15-min self-administration bin. The number of cocaine infusions delivered following pretreatment with CP-154,526 was significantly reduced in all four of the 15-min self-administration bins, including the first 15-min bin. (Middle panel) Effects of CP-154,526 (20 mg/kg, i.p.) during each of the four 15-min self-administration bins on infusions self-administered by rats trained to self-administer 0.25 mg/kg/infusion cocaine. The number of infusions delivered during extinction was significantly less than baseline in the second, third and fourth 15-min self-administration bin. The number of cocaine infusions delivered following pretreatment with CP-154,526 was significantly reduced in all four of the 15-min self-administration bins, including the first 15-min bin. (Lower panel) Effects of CP-154,526 (20 mg/kg, i.p.) during each of the four 15-min self-administration bins on infusions self-administered by rats trained to self-administer 0.5 mg/kg/infusion cocaine. The number of infusions delivered during extinction was significantly less than baseline in the second, third and fourth 15-min self-administration bin. The number of cocaine infusions delivered following pretreatment with CP-154,526 was significantly reduced in all four of the 15-min self-administration bins, including the first 15-min bin. *p < .05 extinction vs. baseline and CP-154,526 vs. vehicle pretreatment. **p < .05 extinction vs. CP-154-526 pretreatment.

DISCUSSION

The results of these experiments suggest that CRH receptors are involved in the maintenance of ongoing cocaine self-administration in rats. CP-154,526 binds with a high affinity (Ki < 10 nM) to the CRH1 receptor subtype (Schulz et al. 1996), suggesting that the effects of the compound on cocaine self-administration resulted primarily from interactions at this receptor site. While there is the possibility that this compound may also affect other binding sites, its selectivity and specificity for the CRH1 receptor make this possibility unlikely (Schulz et al. 1996; McCarthy et al. 1999). Cocaine-reinforced responding was decreased across several doses of cocaine, with the dose-response curve for cocaine self-administration shifted downward and flattened (Figure 2), suggesting that CP-154,526 was decreasing cocaine reinforcement. Interestingly, self-administration was decreased even at the highest dose tested (i.e., 0.5 mg/kg/infusion), which was noticeably different from the effects we observed with ketoconazole (Goeders et al. 1998). Ketoconazole was only effective in reducing drug intake at the lower unit doses of cocaine (i.e., 0.25 mg/kg/infusion or lower). No significant effects were observed when the higher dose (i.e., 0.5 mg/kg/infusion) was self-administered, suggesting that pretreatment with CP-154,526 produced a more efficient inhibition of cocaine reinforcement. This decrease in cocaine self-administration was not likely due to nonspecific effects of the antagonist since food-maintained responding during the same session was unaffected by pretreatment with CP-154,526. However, food-maintained responding following CP-154,526 pretreatment was similar to that observed following vehicle pretreatment even though the number of cocaine infusions was significantly reduced. During extinction, when saline was delivered instead of cocaine, there was a small but significant increase in the number of food presentations earned. It is not clear why the number of food presentations delivered following CP-154,526 pretreatment did not increase to levels observed during cocaine extinction, but this may have resulted from the amount of cocaine that was still self-administered. Even though it was less than following vehicle pretreatment, the amount of cocaine delivered may have still been sufficient to slightly reduce food-maintained responding.

We have employed food as a non-drug, alternate reinforcer for several years now using the same alternating schedule that we described for these experiments. As stated previously (Goeders et al. 1998), although food extinction probes were not specifically programmed in these experiments, the food pellet dispensers we use do malfunction from time to time. Therefore, these rats received multiple, random food extinction probes throughout the course of these experiments. Under these conditions, the rats rapidly learned to stop responding on the “food” lever whenever the food pellet dispensers malfunctioned (i.e., during a non-programmed food extinction probe), which suggests that the relative lack of effect of CP-154,526 on food-maintained responding was not the result of a lack of experience with food extinction. In addition, while food-maintained responding was maintained under a FR10 schedule of reinforcement, cocaine was self-administered under a FR4 schedule, suggesting that the different rates of responding on the two levers could have contributed to the differential effects of CP-154,526 on the two reinforcers. This is unlikely, however, since a greater effect would be expected against responding maintained at a higher rate (i.e., on the food lever). Nevertheless, preliminary data from our laboratory using equal schedules of reinforcement for both food and cocaine (i.e., FR4) have shown that CP-154,526 produces similar effects under both variations of the alternating reinforcer paradigm, suggesting that this may not have been an important factor in these experiments. However, additional research will be necessary to conclusively answer this question.

Another finding from these experiments was that responding on the cocaine lever following CP-154,526 pretreatment was significantly decreased even during the first 15-min bin. Typically, rats continue to respond during the first few minutes of the behavioral sessions even during extinction (Goeders et al. 1998), probably as a result of conditioning and related events associated with attaching the catheter and leash and the rat being placed into the self-administration environment at the start of the session (Peltier et al. 1999). Preliminary data from our laboratory suggest that such a conditioning effect, mediated at least in part via the HPA axis, occurs even in rats in which cocaine lever pressing has been successfully extinguished. We are currently investigating the role for CRH receptors and CP-154,526 in this conditioning phenomenon during cocaine extinction and using a conditioned cue reinstatement model (Goeders et al. 2000).

CRH has been implicated in many of the behavioral effects of cocaine, which is not surprising since the behavioral profiles of CRH and cocaine are very similar in many respects, such as the production of anxiety (Britton et al. 1984; Yang et al. 1992, Goeders 1997). Furthermore, in contrast to the effects of adrenocorticosteroids, the well-characterized interactions of CRH with membrane receptors make it a more attractive candidate as a mediator of the acute neurobehavioral effects of cocaine. For example, the central administration of α-helical-CRF9-41 or of an anti-CRH antibody results in an attenuation of cocaine-induced locomotor activity (Sarnyai et al. 1992b), while icv CRH augments amphetamine-induced stereotyped behavior (Cole and Koob 1989) as well as cocaine-kindled seizures and lethality (Weiss et al. 1992). CRH may also provide a potential substrate for stressor-cocaine interactions. The central, but not peripheral, administration of α-helical-CRF9-41 blocks the restraint stress-induced cross-sensitization of the behavioral response to amphetamine (Cole et al. 1990), while the central, but not systemic, administration of CRH itself induces behavioral sensitization in a manner analogous to amphetamine or stressors (Cador et al. 1993).

These findings have been extended to include a role for CRH in the effects of stressors on cocaine-seeking behavior. The icv administration of the CRH antagonist, D-Phe CRF12-41 (Erb et al. 1998), or the systemic administration of CP-154,526 (Shaham et al. 1998), attenuates the electric footshock-induced reinstatement of extinguished cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. CRH receptor blockade also reduces the electric footshock-induced reinstatement of extinguished heroin seeking (Shaham et al. 1997, 1998). On the other hand, the icv administration of CRH can reinstate extinguished heroin-seeking behavior (Shaham et al. 1997), although similar effects on extinguished cocaine seeking have not yet been reported. These data suggest that electric footshock reinstates extinguished cocaine- and heroin-seeking behavior, at least in part, through the stress-induced stimulation of CRH secretion from the hypothalamus, with the subsequent activation of the HPA axis. The ability of electric footshock to reinstate cocaine seeking probably also involves corticosterone, since adrenalectomy (Erb et al. 1998) or the inhibition of corticosterone synthesis (Mantsch and Goeders 1999) each attenuates stress-induced reinstatement. Furthermore, replenishing plasma corticosterone to minimal basal concentrations restores the ability of electric footshock to produce reinstatement in adrenalectomized rats (Erb et al. 1998). Electric footshock-induced reinstatement can still be blocked by D-Phe CRF12-41 in these rats, indicating that corticosterone may be playing a “permissive” role in the CRH-mediated reinstating effects of electric footshock, such that corticosterone may actually be required for the actions of the neuropeptide.

Cocaine has also been reported to induce a number of changes in the expression of CRH and its receptors within the central nervous system. Acute cocaine administration reduces CRH-like immunoreactivity in the hypothalamus (Sarnyai et al. 1993), an effect believed to reflect the release of the peptide into the portal circulation from the median eminence where it exerts its effects on neuroendocrine function. Cocaine also affects CRH-like immunoreactivity in a number of other brain regions that are not thought to be directly linked to pituitary-adrenal activity, including the hippocampus, frontal cortex, basal forebrain, and amygdala (Sarnyai et al. 1993; Gardi et al. 1997). Interestingly, in contrast to the reduction in CRH-like immunoreactivity seen in the hypothalamus, frontal cortex, hippocampus, and basal forebrain, cocaine has been reported to dramatically and dose-dependently increase the expression of CRH in the amygdala (Sarnyai et al. 1993; Gardi et al. 1997). This effect appears to reflect cocaine-induced increases in CRH synthesis and release because increases in extracellular CRH have also been characterized in the amygdala following an acute injection of cocaine (Richter et al. 1995) and during withdrawal from cocaine self-administration (Richter and Weiss 1999), and because increases in CRH mRNA are observed with acute “binge-pattern” cocaine administration (Zhou et al. 1996). Further evidence for this premise is provided by our findings that CRH receptors are down-regulated in the amygdala following chronic cocaine delivery (Goeders et al. 1990). These data suggest that the cocaine-induced increase in the synthesis and/or release of CRH in this region may mediate some of the behavioral effects of the drug. In fact, a role for the amygdala in psychomotor stimulant self-administration (Deminiére et al. 1988; Caine et al. 1995; Hurd et al. 1997) and relapse to drug seeking (Baker et al. 1999) has already been established. Interestingly, this role appears to at least partially involve the classical conditioning of drug effects (Whitelaw et al. 1996; Meil and See 1997), a process that is obviously intimately involved in the persistence and relapse of cocaine-seeking behavior and may also be related to the ability of CP-154,526 to reduce cocaine self-administration even during the first few minutes of the behavioral session. Central CRH has been implicated in the conditioned effects of cocaine (De Vries et al. 1998; De Vries and Pert 1998), although it is unclear whether or not these effects are the result of CRH's actions within the amygdala.

Cocaine affects CRH activity within the mesocorticolimbic dopaminergic neuronal system, a system widely regarded to mediate the reinforcing effects of cocaine and other drugs of abuse (Koob and Bloom 1988). Acute cocaine delivery decreases CRH-like immunoreactivity (Sarnyai et al. 1993; Gardi et al. 1997) and increases CRH mRNA (Zhou et al. 1996) in the medial prefrontal cortex. Chronic cocaine administration reduces CRH receptor binding in the medial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens (Goeders et al. 1990; Ambrosio et al. 1997), effects which may be secondary to drug-induced increases in the release of the neuropeptide. In contrast to the effects seen in these terminal regions, CRH receptor binding is actually increased in the ventral tegmental area (VTA). The localization of CRH and its receptors within this system suggests that the peptide may have modulatory effects on dopaminergic neurotransmission. Accordingly, the administration of CRH directly into the VTA results in a time-dependent decrease in prefrontal cortical dopamine metabolism with an increase in dopamine turnover in the nucleus accumbens (Kalivas et al. 1987). This general pattern of dopaminergic neurotransmission has previously been associated with an enhanced vulnerability to engage in psychomotor stimulant self-administration (Piazza et al. 1991; Goeders and Smith 1993). However, the effects of intra-VTA CRH are very different from those reported following the icv administration of the peptide. ICV CRH has little or no effect on dopamine in the nucleus accumbens (Kalivas et al. 1987; Dunn and Berridge 1990) and actually increases dopamine metabolism in the medial prefrontal cortex (Dunn and Berridge 1990). Nevertheless, in consideration of the putative dual role for the medial prefrontal cortex in the neurobiological response to stress as well as in the initiation of cocaine-seeking behavior (Goeders and Smith 1983; Goeders 1997), it is likely that CRH-induced alterations in dopaminergic neurotransmission play an important role in this peptide's effects on cocaine responsiveness.