Main

Disruptions in measures of attention and cognition have been observed in patients diagnosed with schizophrenia. These deficits may be linked to an impairment in sensorimotor gating, such that intrusive stimuli are not properly filtered out (Grillon et al. 1990; McGhie and Chapman 1961; Venables 1960). In the acoustic startle paradigm, startle reactivity (i.e., startle magnitude) in response to high-intensity acoustic stimuli is measured and prepulse inhibition (PPI) is assessed. PPI refers to the reduction in the startle response produced when a low-intensity, nonstartling stimulus (the prepulse) is presented shortly before the startle stimulus (Hoffman and Ison 1980). PPI provides an operational measure of sensorimotor gating that appears to be present in all mammals. Disruptions in PPI have been demonstrated in schizophrenic patients compared with healthy controls and patients diagnosed with mood disorders such as depression (Bolino et al. 1994; Braff et al. 1978, 1992; Cadenhead et al. 1997; Grillon et al. 1992; Weike et al. 1996). Deficits in schizophrenia patients do not simply result from generalized behavioral impairments or medications, because schizotypical patients who are not grossly psychotic or receiving antipsychotic medications exhibit similar PPI deficits (Cadenhead et al. 1993). Thus, patients with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders demonstrate dysfunctions in sensorimotor gating, as measured by disruptions in PPI.

A variety of pharmacologic (serotonergic, glutamatergic, and dopaminergic) and nonpharmacologic (rearing in isolation) treatments disrupt PPI in animals and are used as models to characterize and identify antipsychotic drugs (Geyer et al. 1990). For example, the loss of sensorimotor gating observed in patients with schizophrenia has been mimicked in rats treated with the hallucinogenic 5-HT2-receptor agonist DOI (Padich et al. 1996; Sipes and Geyer 1994, 1995). Typical and atypical antipsychotics reduce the disruptive effect of DOI on PPI to varying degrees in different strains of rats (Padich et al. 1996; Sipes and Geyer 1994; Varty and Higgins 1995b). For example, the 5-HT2A/C antagonist ketanserin, reduced the disruptive effects of DOI on PPI (Sipes and Geyer 1994). Further studies have found that the activity at 5-HT2A receptors, but not 5-HT2C receptors, is important for the effect of ketanserin; the selective 5-HT2A antagonist M100907, but not the 5-HT2C antagonist SDZ-SER-082, was effective in reversing the effects of DOI on PPI (Padich et al. 1996; Sipes and Geyer 1995). Additionally, M100907 reduced the effects of serotonin-releasing agents (MDMA and fenfluramine) on PPI (Padich et al. 1996). The effects of DOI and M100907 on PPI appear to be mediated by 5-HT2A receptors in the ventral pallidum (Sipes and Geyer 1997).

In addition to serotonergic systems, there is evidence that glutamatergic systems influence PPI. Phencyclidine (PCP) and other noncompetitive NMDA antagonists, such as ketamine and dizocilpine, produce effects in healthy subjects that are similar to the spectrum of symptoms and cognitive impairments associated with schizophrenia (Domino and Luby 1981; Javitt and Zukin 1991; Luby et al. 1959; Mansbach and Geyer 1991; Moghaddam 1994), and have been used to model psychosis in animals. NMDA antagonists also worsen symptoms in schizophrenic patients (Lahti et al. 1995a,b; Malhotra et al. 1997). PCP and dizocilpine have been shown to disrupt PPI in rats (Bakshi et al. 1994; Keith et al. 1991; Mansbach and Geyer 1989; Varty and Higgins 1995a; Swerdlow et al. 1996), and ketamine has been shown to disrupt PPI in humans (Karper et al. 1994). Thus, NMDA-receptor antagonist-induced disruption of PPI can be used as a model of schizophrenia-like deficits in sensorimotor gating.

The disruptive effects of noncompetitive NMDA antagonists on PPI are not blocked by selective dopamine-receptor antagonists or most typical antipsychotics, but are reduced by atypical antipsychotics (e.g., clozapine, olanzapine, quetiapine), which have antagonistic activity at multiple receptor sites (Bakshi and Geyer 1995; Geyer et al. 1990; Hoffman et al. 1993; Hollister 1969; Keith et al. 1991; Swerdlow et al. 1996; Varty and Higgins 1995b). Several recent studies have tried to determine the specific receptors that underlie the ability of atypical antipsychotics to reduce the disruptive effects of NMDA antagonists on PPI.

The contributions of 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors to the interaction between atypical antipsychotics and noncompetitive NMDA antagonists have recently been explored. Nonselective 5-HT2 receptor antagonists (e.g., risperidone) have been shown to block the effects of noncompetitive NMDA antagonists in Wistar rats (Varty and Higgins 1995a,b), but not Sprague-Dawley rats (Bakshi et al. 1994; Swerdlow et al. 1996). Thus, there appears to be a strain difference in the ability of nonselective 5-HT2-receptor antagonists to attenuate the effects of NMDA antagonists; previous studies have shown differences in PPI and its pharmacological manipulation among strains of rats (Rigdon 1990; Swerdlow et al. 1998a; Varty and Higgins 1994). M100907 significantly reduced the effects of dizocilpine on PPI in both strains of rats (Varty et al. 1999). In contrast, the 5-HT2C antagonist SDZ-SER-082 completely failed to reduce the effects of dizocilpine (Varty et al. 1999). These results support the hypothesis that antagonism of 5-HT2C receptors, produced by nonselective 5-HT2 receptor antagonists and SDZ-SER-082, interferes with a reversal of NMDA antagonist-induced disruptions of PPI in the Sprague-Dawley strain of rat (Bakshi et al. 1994). Together with previous reports (Varty and Higgins 1995a), these findings indicate that activity at 5-HT2A receptors contributes to the efficacy of atypical antipsychotics in reducing the disruptive effects of NMDA antagonists on PPI.

Increasing dopaminergic transmission, such as with the direct dopamine agonist apomorphine, also disrupts PPI (Davis et al. 1990; Mansbach et al. 1988; Swerdlow et al. 1994). Apomorphine-induced disruption of PPI has been demonstrated to be due to the activation of D2-like dopaminergic receptors within the nucleus accumbens (Swerdlow et al. 1992). The effect of apomorphine on PPI can be reduced by the administration of both typical and atypical antipsychotics (Mansbach et al. 1988, 1998; Rasmussen et al. 1997; Rigdon and Viik 1991; Swerdlow et al. 1991, 1998a; Swerdlow and Geyer 1993). In contrast, several nondopaminergic compounds have proven ineffective in reducing the effects of apomorphine on PPI (Bakshi and Geyer 1997; Furuya et al. 1998; Rasmussen et al. 1997; Rigdon and Viik 1991). Therefore, the effects of apomorphine on PPI appear to be reduced, almost exclusively, by selective dopamine antagonists and multi-receptor antagonists (atypical antipsychotics). However, a serotonergic mechanism of action in the ability of the atypical antipsychotics to reverse apomorphine disruption of PPI has not been ruled out. Accordingly, in the present paper, we examined the effects of M100907 on apomorphine-induced disruption of PPI. We hypothesized that 5-HT2A-receptor antagonism would have no effect on the disruption of PPI produced by apomorphine.

We also have used developmental manipulations to produce a nonpharmacological model of the sensorimotor gating deficits of schizophrenia (Geyer et al. 1993; Varty and Higgins 1995b). Rat pups reared in isolation for 8 weeks from the time of weaning display deficits in PPI when tested in adulthood compared to socially reared rats (Geyer et al. 1993). This effect is developmentally specific because similar isolation of adult rats does not alter PPI (Wilkinson et al. 1994). Of potential therapeutic importance is the fact that PPI deficits produced by isolation rearing are reversed by both typical and atypical antipsychotics (Bakshi et al. 1998; Geyer et al. 1993; Varty and Higgins 1995b). This nonpharmacological method of inducing schizophrenia-like behavioral deficits has potential utility in the screening of novel antipsychotic drugs. Two experiments were conducted to examine the effects of M100907 on the disruption of PPI produced by isolation rearing in rats.

METHODS

Subjects

Apomorphine Challenge Experiment

Forty naive male Sprague-Dawley rats (Harlan Industries, San Diego, CA), weighing between 250 and 400 g, were housed two to three per cage upon arrival. Animals were allowed to acclimate to housing conditions for approximately 1 week before the start of the experiment.

Isolation-Rearing Experiments

A total of 94 naive male Sprague-Dawley rats (Harlan, San Diego, CA), brought in as weaned 21-day-old pups, were used in the isolation-rearing studies; 31 rats were used in Experiment 1 and 63 rats were used in Experiment 2. In each experiment, pups were randomized to two groups: a socially reared group and an isolation-reared group. Socially reared pups were housed under normal social conditions (three per cage), whereas isolation-reared pups were housed under isolated conditions (one per cage) for 8 weeks prior to the start of testing. All rats were housed in the same room and could hear, see, and smell other rats. Isolated rats received minimal handling; rats were handled once a week for cage cleaning.

All rats were housed in a temperature- and humidity-controlled room on a 12 h reversed light/dark cycle (lights off period 07.30–19.30 h). All testing occurred during the dark phase between 09.00 and 17.00 h (see Geyer and Swerdlow 1998 for a detailed description of experimental methods). Food and water were provided ad libitum.

Drugs

M100907, (R)-(+)-α-(2,3-dimethoxyphenyl)-1-[2-(fluorophenyl)ethyl]-4-piperidinemethanol (Hoechst Marion Roussel, Frankfurt, Germany), was dissolved in a solution of Tween 80 and distilled water. Apomorphine hydrochloride (Sigma Chemicals, St. Louis, MO) was dissolved in 0.1% ascorbic acid. All drugs were administered subcutaneously (SC) in a volume of 1 ml/kg. Control animals were treated with the corresponding vehicle.

Apparatus

Four chambers were used to measure startle reactivity (SR-LAB, San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA). Each chamber housed a Plexiglas cylinder (9 cm in diameter) into which the rats were placed during each test session. Vibrations of the cylinder, caused by the whole-body startle response of the animal, were transduced into analog signals by a piezoelectric accelerometer attached to the platform. Stabilimeter readings were then rectified, digitized (0–4095), and stored by a microcomputer and interface assembly. A loudspeaker mounted 24 cm above the cylinder provided broadband background noise and auditory stimuli. The whole apparatus was housed within a ventilated, sound-attenuating chamber (39 × 38 × 58 cm). Presentations of acoustic stimuli were controlled by SR-LAB software and interface system. As previously described (Geyer and Swerdlow 1998; Mansbach et al. 1988), sound levels [dB(A) scale] and accelerometer sensitivities within each chamber were calibrated regularly and found to remain constant over the test period.

Procedure

Apomorphine Challenge Experiment

Four groups of rats (n = 10) were pretreated with either vehicle or 1.0 mg/kg M100907. Twenty-five minutes later, rats were treated with either vehicle or 0.5 mg/kg apomorphine. Five minutes after the second injection, rats were placed in startle chambers for a 25-min PPI test session.

PPI test session consisted of a 5-min acclimatization period to a 65 dB background noise (continuous throughout the session), followed by a 20-min acoustic PPI test session. Five trial types were presented in this block: four 20 ms prepulse + pulse combinations that consisted of a 6 dB or 12 dB (above background) prepulse, each followed 100 ms or 300 ms later by a 40 ms, 120 dB startle pulse (6 dB, 100 ms; 12 dB, 100 ms; 6 dB, 300 ms; and 12 dB, 300 ms) and a 40 ms, 120 dB startle pulse alone (P120). Both the 100 ms and 300 ms inter-stimulus intervals (ISI) have been used to demonstrate drug and housing effects on PPI (Varty and Higgins 1995b; Varty et al. 1999). The trial types were presented in a pseudorandom order with an average inter-trial interval (ITI) of 15 s. In addition, 5 P120 trials were presented at both the beginning and the end of the PPI test session, but these P120 trials were not used in the calculation of PPI.

Isolation-Rearing Experiments

During Experiments 1 and 2, rat pups were housed either socially (three per cage) or in isolation (one per cage) for eight weeks before testing. In Experiment 1, four groups of rats (n = 7–8) were administered either vehicle or 1.0 mg/kg M100907, 30 min before a 25-min PPI test session. One week later, rats were retested with the alternative treatment in a within-subjects design. As in the apomorphine experiment, PPI test sessions consisted of a 5-min acclimatization period to a 65-dB background noise (continuous throughout the session), followed by a 20-min acoustic PPI test session. During the acoustic PPI session, five trial types were presented: a 40 ms, 120 dB startle pulse (P120); and one of three 20 ms prepulses of 3, 6, or 12 dB above background, followed 100 ms later by the pulse (3 dB, 100 ms; 6 dB, 100 ms; and 12 dB, 100 ms). Again, trial types were presented in a pseudorandom order with an average ITI of 15 s. In addition, 5 P120 trials were presented at both the beginning and the end of the acoustic test session.

In Experiment 2, a separate cohort of four groups of rats (n = 14–16) were administered either vehicle or 1.0 mg/kg M100907 30 min before a 25-min PPI test session in a between-subjects design. The PPI session of Experiment 2 was the same type of session employed in the apomorphine experiment described above.

Statistical Analysis

The magnitude of PPI (% PPI) was determined according to the following formula: % PPI = [100 - ({startle magnitude on prepulse + pulse trials/startle magnitude on pulse-alone trials} × 100)], such that a 0% value indicated no difference between the responses to prepulse + pulse trials and pulse alone trials (i.e., no PPI) (Geyer and Swerdlow 1998).

Apomorphine Challenge Experiment

Percent PPI data were analyzed using a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with prepulse intensity and ISI as within-subjects factors, and pretreatment and treatment as the between-subjects factors. Mean % PPI was analyzed using two-way ANOVA with pretreatment and treatment as between-subjects factors.

Isolation-Rearing Experiments

Our a priori hypothesis that M100907 would reverse isolation-induced PPI deficits, based on past evidence that isolation rearing disrupts PPI and that M100907 does not increase PPI on its own, led us to perform separate post hoc ANOVAs of both data sets. Isolation-rearing PPI deficits were tested by performing a one-way ANOVA for housing effect in vehicle-treated or M100907-treated rats. M100907 antagonism of isolation rearing-induced PPI deficits was tested by performing a one-way ANOVA for drug effect in Isolated or Social rats. F-values are described only for significant effects. When main effects were significant, post hoc analyses were conducted using Tukey's protected-t with a 0.05 level of significance.

RESULTS

Apomorphine Challenge Experiment

Apomorphine produced a disruption of PPI in both vehicle- and M100907-pretreated rats as revealed by a significant main effect of apomorphine treatment [F(1,36) = 34.57]. Pretreatment with M100907 had no effect on disruption of PPI by apomorphine; there was neither a main effect of M100907 pretreatment nor an interaction between M100907 and apomorphine. There was a significant interaction of apomorphine treatment with ISI level [F(1,36) = 4.81], but no other interactions. Because of the significant interaction with ISI, two-way ANOVAs for each ISI/Intensity combination were performed. While each combination demonstrated a significant main effect of apomorphine treatment (data not shown), no significant interactions were found. There was no effect of drug on startle magnitude.

Mean % PPI data for the apomorphine challenge experiment are presented in Figure 1 . Post hoc comparisons confirmed that the Vehicle/Apomorphine and the M100907/Apomorphine groups were significantly different from the Vehicle/Vehicle group.

Figure 1
figure 1

Effect of M100907 pretreatment and apomorphine treatment on prepulse inhibition (PPI) in rats. Apomorphine (0.5 mg/kg, SC) treatment significantly disrupted mean % PPI. Pretreatment with 1.0 mg/kg M100907 (SC) had no effect on PPI by itself and failed to reduce the effects of apomorphine on PPI. Data are presented as group means ± SEM for the entire startle session. * Significantly different from Vehicle/Vehicle group (Tukey post hocs)

Isolation-Rearing Experiments

Isolation rearing disrupted PPI in most prepulse conditions, as revealed by a significant effect of housing in vehicle-treated rats in Experiment 1 [F(1,28) = 7.19] and Experiment 2 [F(1,29) = 4.92]. There was a significant effect of housing in vehicle-treated rats in Experiment 1 [F(1,14) = 7.37] on startle magnitude, but there was no effect on startle in Experiment 2. Thus, the PPI deficits produced by isolation rearing are independent of any effects on startle magnitude. Specific comparisons revealed that the Social/Vehicle group differed from the Isolated/Vehicle group for all conditions in Experiment 1 and in the 6 dB 300 ms condition in Experiment 2 (Table 1).

Table 1 Effect of Pretreatment with either Vehicle or 1.0 mg/kg M100907 on % PPI in Rats Reared Socially or in Isolation

M100907 partially reversed the isolation rearing-induced disruption of PPI, as revealed by a significant effect of drug in isolated rats in Experiment 1 [F(1,15) = 5.67] and Experiment 2 [F(1,30) = 8.42], but not in socially reared rats in either experiment. Additionally, there was no significant effect of housing in M100907-treated rats in either Experiment 1 or 2. Specific comparisons revealed that there was a significant difference between the Isolated/Vehicle and Isolated/M100907 groups for the 12 dB 100 ms, 6 dB 300 ms, and the 12 dB 300 ms conditions in Experiment 2, but not in any specific condition in Experiment 1. There was no significant main effect of M100907 in socially reared rats in either experiment. Nevertheless, post hoc comparisons revealed a significant difference between the Social/Vehicle and Social/M100907 groups for the 6 dB 100 ms condition in Experiment 1 (Table 1). As shown in Figure 2 , these effects were replicated in the mean % PPI data.

Figure 2
figure 2

Effect of pretreatment with M100907 on mean % PPI in rats reared socially (three per cage) or in isolation (one per cage). Eight weeks later, rats were treated with either saline or 1.0 mg/kg M100907 (SC) and tested for PPI. Isolation-reared rats show disrupted PPI. M100907 pretreatment had no effect on PPI by itself, but significantly reduced isolation rearing-induced disruption of PPI. The upper panel depicts results from Experiment 1 and the lower panel depicts the results from Experiment 2 (data from a different cohort of animals tested in a different PPI test session). * Significant effect of housing in vehicle-treated rats. † Significant effect of drug in isolated rats

DISCUSSION

The present experiments demonstrated two new findings regarding the effects of the putative antipsychotic M100907 on PPI in rats. First, M100907 is inactive in the primary dopaminergic model of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia. Specifically, M100907 failed to diminish the PPI-disruptive effects of apomorphine and also had no effect on PPI by itself. Second, M100907 partially reversed the disruption of PPI observed in isolation-reared rats, while having no significant effects in socially reared rats.

There are several variants of the PPI paradigm that putatively model the sensorimotor gating deficits exhibited by patients with schizophrenia. Attempts to model certain aspects of psychoses present in the schizophrenia-spectrum disorders include disruption of PPI in rats by the administration of 5-HT2-receptor agonists, NMDA-receptor antagonists, and as shown in the present studies, administration of dopamine agonists and isolation rearing. The validity of these models has been assessed by examining the effects of various antipsychotic agents.

Drugs that increase dopamine transmission in humans produce a psychotic state similar to that observed during acute psychotic episodes in schizophrenic patients. As previously reported (Mansbach et al. 1988) and replicated in the present paper, dopamine agonists such as apomorphine disrupt PPI in rats. Selective dopamine antagonists, as well as typical and atypical antipsychotics, reduce the apomorphine-induced disruption of PPI (Mansbach et al. 1988, 1998; Rasmussen et al. 1997; Swerdlow et al. 1991, 1994; Swerdlow and Geyer 1993; Varty and Higgins 1995b). In contrast, the current experiment demonstrated that the selective 5-HT2A-receptor antagonist M100907 does not attenuate the effects of apomorphine on PPI. Thus, dopamine agonism appears to be a good model of schizophrenia-like disruptions in PPI, but may not be the optimal paradigm for screening the new generation of potential atypical antipsychotics such as M100907, which have little activity at dopamine receptors.

In two of the present studies, isolation rearing produced deficits in PPI, confirming previous reports (Geyer et al. 1993; Varty and Higgins 1995b). These deficits were significant in the vehicle-treated rats in both experiments, but were significantly diminished in the M100907-treated rats. Thus, M100907 partially reversed the deficit in PPI produced by isolation rearing when tested either in a between-subjects or a within-subjects design. Although the trend for M100907 treatment to increase PPI in socially reared rats appears to have prevented the demonstration of a significant interaction between housing and drug treatment, post hoc comparisons based on the a priori hypothesis confirmed that M100907 significantly increased PPI in isolation-reared rats but not in socially reared rats. A similar trend for an M100907-induced increase in PPI has been seen in previous studies, but has never reached significance when M100907 was administered systemically (Sipes and Geyer 1995). Indeed, M100907 alone produced no increase in PPI in the apomorphine experiment, indicating that this tendency is inconsistent. Intra-ventral pallidal administration of M100907, however, has been shown to produce a significant increase in PPI (Sipes and Geyer 1997). The present studies indicate that M100907 treatment reduces the effects of isolation rearing on PPI. Furthermore, as both typical and atypical antipsychotics attenuate the effects of isolation rearing, isolation rearing seems to be a good model of schizophrenia-like behavioral deficits in PPI and may have utility for screening putative atypical antipsychotics.

The serotonin system has been implicated in the psychopathology of schizophrenia. Early studies demonstrated altered levels of serotonin and its metabolites in the cerebrospinal fluid, and decreased densities of serotonin receptors in postmortem brain tissue, of schizophrenic patients (Abi-Dargham et al. 1997; Breier 1995; Busatto and Kerwin 1997; Gurevich and Joyce 1997; Harrison and Burnet 1997; Issa et al. 1994; Joyce et al. 1993; Nyberg and Farde 1997). Although there have been problems with replication and confounding factors in many of these studies, the hypothesis that alterations of serotonergic transmission could be involved in schizophrenia was supported by the discovery that atypical antipsychotic drugs have 5-HT2A-receptor antagonist activity (Goyer et al. 1996; Meltzer 1995a,b).

Serotonin 5-HT2-receptor agonists (e.g., DOI), known as classical hallucinogenic drugs, produce a limited number of effects in humans that appear similar to the symptoms of schizophrenia. 5-HT2-receptor agonists produce disruptions in PPI in animals (Padich et al. 1996; Sipes and Geyer 1994, 1995). Some typical and atypical antipsychotics reduce these effects, while others do not (Padich et al. 1996; Sipes and Geyer 1994; Varty and Higgins 1995b). Nonselective 5-HT2-receptor antagonists that do not appear to be efficacious as antipsychotics, however, do attenuate these effects (Sipes and Geyer 1994). In addition, the selective 5-HT2A-receptor antagonist M100907 robustly reduced the effects of DOI on PPI (Padich et al. 1996; Sipes and Geyer 1995). Thus, nonselective 5-HT2-receptor agonism produces schizophrenia-like PPI deficits in rats that are sensitive to 5-HT2A antagonists and serotonergic atypical antipsychotics, but less sensitive to dopaminergic typical antipsychotics.

In humans, noncompetitive NMDA-receptor antagonists, such as PCP or ketamine, can produce psychotic symptoms similar to those observed in schizophrenic patients (Javitt and Zukin 1991; Luby et al. 1959). These compounds also disrupt PPI in animals (Bakshi et al. 1994; Keith et al. 1991; Mansbach and Geyer 1989, 1991; Varty and Higgins 1995a) and humans (Karper et al. 1994). A number of atypical antipsychotics, including clozapine, olanzapine, and quetiapine, reduce the effects NMDA antagonists on PPI (Bakshi et al. 1994; Bakshi and Geyer 1995; Swerdlow et al. 1996), whereas most typical antipsychotics are ineffective (Furuya et al. 1998; Geyer et al. 1990; Johansson et al. 1994; Keith et al. 1991; Swerdlow et al. 1996, 1998b; Varty and Higgins 1995b). With regard to serotonin/glutamate interactions, nonselective 5-HT2-receptor antagonists are effective in the NMDA antagonist PPI model only to a limited extent and in certain rat strains (Bakshi et al. 1994; Varty et al. 1999; Varty and Higgins 1995a). In contrast, M100907 pretreatment markedly reduced the PPI-disruptive effects of dizocilpine in two different strains of rats (Varty et al. 1999). Thus, noncompetitive NMDA receptor antagonism is a more promising candidate for modeling symptoms of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia and as a method for screening novel antipsychotics.

In conclusion, based on nondopaminergic models of deficient PPI, the selective 5-HT2A-receptor antagonist M100907 appears to have potential utility as an atypical antipsychotic. Although M100907 proved to be inactive in the apomorphine model of disrupted PPI, it is effective in reducing deficits in PPI produced by hallucinogenic 5-HT2A agonists, psychotomimetic NMDA antagonists, or isolation rearing. If the results of Phase III clinical trials of M100907 support the efficacy of this drug as a treatment for schizophrenia, the pharmacologic and non-pharmacologic PPI models of schizophrenia will be supported in their utility to screen novel antipsychotic drugs.