Main

Psychiatric research suggests that a reduced turnover rate of serotonin in the central nervous system (CNS), as measured by cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA), is a risk factor for impaired impulse control, violent behavior, and premature mortality from suicide and violence (Faustman et al. 1990; Kruesi et al. 1990; Lidberg et al. 1985; Linnoila et al. 1983; Virkkunen et al. 1994). Research using nonhuman primates has replicated and extended many of these findings (Higley et al. 1996a; Higley et al. 1992a; Higley et al. 1996b; Kaplan et al. 1994; Mehlman et al. 1995; Mehlman et al. 1994; Mehlman et al. 1997; Raleigh 1987; Raleigh et al. 1983a; Raleigh et al. 1983b; Raleigh et al. 1980; Raleigh and McGuire 1994). Further, this research has demonstrated that inter-individual differences in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations are stable across time and experimental settings and can be attributed to genetic and early environmental influences that produce lasting modifications in the CNS serotonin response (Higley et al. 1996b; Higley et al. 1994; Higley et al. 1996a).

Serotonin-mediated behavioral deficits are thought to result in part from an inability to maintain a homeostatic response in a number of physiological systems (Virkkunen et al. 1989a; Virkkunen and Narvanen 1987; Virkkunen et al. 1994). Various lines of evidence indicate that central serotonin neurotransmission contributes to regulation of sleep-wake cycles and circadian activity patterns. For example, several decades of research has shown that the sleep-wake cycle is partially modulated by serotonin release from projections of the ralphe nuclei (Jacobs 1991; Jouvet 1974; Jouvet 1967; Koella 1985; Koella and Czicman 1966; Maeda et al. 1989; Puizillout et al. 1981). Further, administration of p-chlorophenylalanine (PCPA), which inhibits the synthesis of serotonin, causes a reduction in total sleep in the cat (Jouvet 1974), rat (Torda 1967), and monkey (Weitzman et al. 1968). Finally, experimentally induced lesions of the ralphe system lead to nerve terminal decreases in 5-HT and 5-HIAA which are directly proportional to the degree of insomnia produced by the lesions (Jouvet 1974). These findings support the view that increases in CNS serotonin turnover rate promote sleep, in particular NREM sleep, and that deficits in serotonergic transmission are associated with insomnia, high levels of nighttime activity and/or other sleep disturbances (Jouvet 1974; Koella 1985).

Recent evidence from studies of rodents also highlights the role of serotonin in modulating circadian activity patterns through serotonergic projections to the endogenous circadian pacemaker (Klein et al. 1991; Miller et al. 1996), the suprachiasmatic nucleus (Meyer-Bernstein et al. 1997; Meyer-Bernstein and Morin 1996). For example, 5-HT is believed to regulate the phasic effects of light on circadian rhythms (Kennaway et al. 1996; Meyer-Bernstein and Morin 1996; Prosser et al. 1993; Selim et al. 1993; Smale et al. 1990). Also, fluoxetine-induced increases in serotonergic transmission decrease the duration of circadian periods (Possidente et al. 1996), and serotonergic afferents to the suprachiasmatic nucleus are necessary to synchronize nonphotic activity cues into circadian patterns (Edgar et al. 1997). These findings suggest that a reduced central serotonin turnover rate may be conducive to disruptions in circadian patterns of activity, and that these disruptions may lead to increased rates of nighttime activity and/or concomitant sleep disturbances.

Few human studies have directly investigated the relationship between impaired CNS serotonin turnover rate and aberrant diurnal sleep-wake cycles. However, in a recent study of male alcoholic offenders, Virkkunen and colleagues (Virkkunen et al. 1994) found that offenders grouped by antisocial personality disorder (ASP) and intermittent explosive disorder (IED) have low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations and exhibit higher levels of nighttime activity than do healthy volunteers. However, ASP offenders exhibit higher levels of daytime activity than do healthy volunteers, pointing to a general disruption of diurnal activity patterns. In contrast, IED offenders show levels of daytime activity that cannot be distinguished from those of normal controls. A recent laboratory study suggested that a similar relationship between impaired CNS serotonin and aberrant circadian activity may also exist in nonhuman primates, as socially housed juvenile and adolescent macaques with low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations failed to fall asleep readily and were more likely to exhibit excessive activity during the daytime hours than were macaques with high CSF 5-HIAA concentrations (Zajicek et al. 1997). However, in this study 24-hour assessments were not made and the high number of monkeys that were confined together in a cage may have confounded the results.

In the present study we investigated the relationship between nighttime activity and CSF 5-HIAA concentrations in free ranging rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Rhesus macaques are well suited for making such assessments because CSF can be removed from the cisterna magna, where measurements of 5-HIAA probably better reflect CNS serotonin activity than do lumbar punctures. Studies show that while serotonin turnover is relatively rapid, 5-HIAA concentrations are more slowly changing, requiring 30 minutes or longer to reflect the upstream changes (Bacopoulos et al. 1979; Brammer et al. 1987; Higley et al. 1991), thus allowing one to assess naturally occurring serotonin activity of a subject. As noted, this assessment shows that subjects exhibit unique and stable interindividual differences that persist across time and setting (Higley et al. 1996a; Higley et al. 1994; Higley et al. 1996b). A natural setting provides a unique opportunity for studying nighttime activity, which is highly influenced by social and environmental factors. For example, monkeys at our study site find a new sleep area in their home range each night, and enter sleeping trees up to 15 m in height before sunset and remain until daybreak. These conditions are quite different than those found in standard laboratory settings where animals may be singly caged and subject to unnatural disturbances from laboratory personnel and colony management practices. Based on animal and human studies showing a relationship among serotonin, total sleep duration, and circadian activity patterns, we tested the hypothesis that rhesus macaques with low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations show higher levels of nighttime activity than do rhesus macaques with high CSF 5-HIAA concentrations. Second, we evaluated whether subjects with high levels of nighttime activity and low concentrations of CSF 5-HIAA have daytime activity patterns that more closely resemble human offenders diagnosed with antisocial personality disorder (ASP) or intermitent explosive disorder (IED).

METHOD

Subjects

The subjects were 16 adult male rhesus macaques with a mean age of 86 months at the onset of this study. The animals are part of a free-ranging colony of approximately 4500 monkeys that reside on a 475-acre South Carolina sea island. At the time of sampling the population was organized into 35 social groups with an adult sex ratio of 2.5 females to 1 male. Subjects were selected when they were juveniles, 18–24 months of age. To assure a normative sample of CSF 5-HIAA and aggressive subjects, eight of the subjects in the pre-established age range were selected on a random basis. Four more were selected on the basis of observations of aggressive behavior in the corral and/or by their fight wounds, and the remaining four were selected on the basis of their display of submissive and/or fear-related behaviors in the corral. Preliminary assessments of CSF 5-HIAA concentrations showed that these subjects were representative of the overall population sample. More details on the study population can be found in previous publications (Higley et al. 1992a, b; Higley et al. 1996b; Higley et al. 1996c; Mehlman et al. 1995; Mehlman et al. 1994; Mehlman et al. 1997; Taub and Mehlman 1989).

Methods of Capture

We trapped animals by concealing technicians in blinds near the entrances of ten capture corrals. Corn, fruit, and commercial monkey chow were placed in the corral, and as each subject entered the corral to forage a technician closed the door. The animal was then netted, anesthetized (ketamine hydrochloride, 10 mg/kg, IM), and driven to a clinic area where a CSF sample was obtained. After the CSF sample was obtained, the animal was weighed, given a physical examination, and fitted with a motion sensing radio telemetry transmitter. Upon full recovery from anesthesia, the animal was released near its social group.

Physiological Data Collection

Physiological samples were obtained between 0900 and 1600 hrs over a four-day period. We used a 22-gauge needle and a 5cc syringe to obtain 3-ml CSF samples from the cisterna magna of each male within 35 minutes of ketamine injection. Previous studies have shown that there is no significant capture effect on CSF monoamine concentrations if samples are obtained within 30 to 35 minutes of ketamine injection (Bacopoulos et al. 1979; Brammer et al. 1987; Higley et al. 1991). As in our earlier study (Higley et al. 1991), preliminary analyses indicated that neither time to capture nor time from ketamine injection to the CSF sample was correlated with monoamine concentrations. The samples were quick frozen on dry ice, stored at −70°, and later assayed for 5-HIAA, homovanillic acid (HVA), and 3-methoxy-hydroxyphenylglycol (MHPG) using high performance liquid chromatography with electrochemical detection (Scheinin et al. 1983). All intra and interassay coefficients of variation were less than 10% (Higley et al. 1992a,b; Mehlman et al. 1995; Mehlman et al. 1994).

Nighttime Behavioral Data Collection

Each radio telemetry collar contained a motion detector that altered the rate of signal emission depending on the orientation and change in orientation of the radio transmitter. These signals were monitored with a radio telemetry receiver and fed into an electronic activity recorder (MPC-100) that averaged these signals over 10-sec intervals. The numerical output of the EAR was loaded into a commercial database program so that each subject's nighttime activity output could be graphically represented as mean milliseconds per 10-sec interval. Periods of flat output or only very small oscillations were scored as inactive periods, alternating with active periods. Spikes of activity within each flat-line output were counted as activity if they exceeded 20 seconds in duration. Activity was thus measured as frequency (number of periods per sample), duration (number of total minutes spent in activity and inactivity), and a derived variable, the mean length of activity periods, created by dividing total duration by the frequency of active periods. Before beginning the formal phase of the study, the collars were tested on single-caged and group housed males. Behavioral observations showed a high degree of correspondence between data collected with using direct observations and data collected using the motion detectors.

Nighttime data collection occurred over a 45-day period that began within 30 days after physiological data had been collected. A technician located each subject between 1930 and 2030 hrs and settled in near the sleeping site to allow the group time to habituate to the technician's presence. The MPC-100 was used to collect nighttime data. Recording sessions began at 2100 hours. The MPC-100 collected and stored data for two periods of five and four hours each, representing the time between 2100 hrs each night and 0600 hrs the next morning. During the study we discovered that one subject had received a severe wound as the result of inter-male aggression. His nighttime and daytime behavior was biased towards inactivity (presumably because his wound was healing). As a result we omitted these data from further consideration.

Accuracy of the Electronic Nighttime Recordings

Laboratory tests taken prior to field use showed the MPC-100 to be sensitive enough to detect subtle body movements, but less sensitive to movements that were limited to head-only or leg-only movements that did not involve changes in body posture. To verify the relationship between activity periods measured by the MPC-100 and real-time patterns of activity, we collected simultaneous sampling using the MPC-100 and a field observer for a 16-minute period for each individual. During this time, the observer recorded all frequencies and durations of lying and sitting without significant head movement. The majority of the inactive periods were spent in trees (85%) and consisted of the animal sitting or lying quietly. The observations of lying and quiet sitting were thus pooled together to represent periods of inactivity, measured by frequency and total duration. Activity periods were derived from these data, as well as mean duration of active periods, and we then correlated the MPC-100 output with the simultaneous field observations. For duration and frequency data there were significant positive correlations between the two methods (for duration data, r(13) = .68, p = .004; for frequency data, r(13) = .52, p = .04).

Daytime Behavioral Data Collection

Daytime behavioral data collection occurred over a 45-day period that began with the onset of nighttime data collection. We collected daytime data using focal animal sampling randomized for subject order and representational by time of day. A total of 70 hours of observations were made during ten sessions. All subjects were followed in the field with radio telemetry. Quantitative behavioral data were collected on frequency and duration of lying or sleeping, and the behaviors were combined to represent periods of inactivity. Non-sleeping data were found to be heterogeneous with respect to the simultaneous occurrence of foraging, grooming, head movements, etc. Duration and frequency of daytime activity were then calculated from the frequency and duration of inactivity data. Observations were also collected on frequencies of yawning. An event was considered to have ended if it stopped and did not resume within 10 seconds. Both observers were blind to the physiological data and the hypotheses being tested. To establish inter-observer reliability, simultaneous observation sessions were conducted at the beginning, midpoint, and end of the study. During these sessions, both observers scored the same subjects for concurrent 15-minute periods. All inter-observer reliabilities on the various categories of behavior were determined by Cohen's kappa test values to be greater than 0.85.

Preliminary Data Analyses

All correlation coefficients reported herein are Pearson product-moment with an alpha level of 0.05. We partialled out subject age and weight by use of standardized residuals for physiological variables affected by these potential confounding variables. Measures related to conditions of capture (time of day, elapsed time between capture and sampling, etc.), as well as age and weight, were then correlated with physiological measures to determine whether these factors could be considered confounds. Time of Day and Time to CSF were not significantly correlated with CSF metabolite levels. As expected, weight and age were positively correlated with each other (r(13) = .53, p < .04), and with CSF 5-HIAA (r(13) = −.58 and −.68, respectively, p < .03) and HVA (r(13) = −.72 and −.55, respectively, p < .04).

RESULTS

Physiological Measures

The mean CSF 5-HIAA concentration per subject was 152 pmols/ml (range = 98–226 pmols/ml; s.d. = 42 pmols/ml). The mean CSF HVA concentration per subject was 760 pmols/ml (range = 465–1394 pmols/ml; s.d. = 223 pmols/ml). The mean CSF MHPH concentration per subject was 129 pmols/ml (range = 68–159 pmols/ml; s.d. = 27 pmols/ml). We noted significant positive correlations between CSF 5-HIAA and HVA concentrations (r(13) = .63, p < .01). We noted a significant negative correlation between CSF MHPG and HVA (r(13) = −.52, p < .05). The correlation between CSF MHPG and 5-HIAA was not statistically significant (r(13) = −.07).

Nighttime Activity

The relationships between the three measures of nighttime activity are displayed in Table 1. Total duration of nighttime activity was positively correlated with mean duration of nighttime activity (r(13) = .72, p < .002), as was frequency and total duration of nighttime activity (r(13) = .53, p < .04). We noted an inverse correlation between total duration and frequency of nighttime inactivity periods (r(13) = −.53, p < .04) indicating that as total time spent in nighttime activity decreased there were fewer inactive events with increasingly longer duration.

Table 1 Descriptive Statistics for Nighttime Activity Measures and Pearson r Correlations Between the Three Measures. For all Measures n = 15

Nighttime Activity and Physiological Measures

CSF 5-HIAA concentrations (with weight and age statistically partialled out) were inversely correlated with the frequency of active periods (t(14) = −2.65, p < .03) and with the total duration of nighttime activity (t(14) = −2.21, p < .05). Further analysis revealed that CSF 5-HIAA concentrations were positively correlated with the mean duration of nighttime periods of inactivity (t(14) = 2.22, p < .05). CSF HVA concentrations (with weight and age statistically partialled out) were inversely correlated with mean duration of nighttime activity periods (t(14) = −4.59, p < .001). CSF MHPG concentrations (with Time of Day statistically partialled out) were positively correlated with mean duration of nighttime activity (t(14) = 2.56, p < .03).

Daytime Activity and Physiological Measures

We failed to detect any associations between daytime measures of activity and CSF 5-HIAA and HVA concentrations (age and weight statistically partialled out). In contrast, CSF MHPG concentrations (with Time of Day residualized out) were positively correlated with daytime sleep frequency (t(14) = 2.69, p < .02) and daytime yawning (t(14) = 2.41, p < .04), and inversely correlated with mean duration of activity periods (t(14) = −2.68, p < .02). See Figure 1 and Figure 2.

Figure 1
figure 1

Scattergram plot of CSF 5-HIAA z scores (with age and weight statistically partialled out) correlated with total duration of nighttime activity in minutes

Figure 2
figure 2

Scattergram plot of CSF 5-HIAA z scores (with age and weight statistically partialled out) correlated with total daytime frequency of lie and sleep combined

Daytime and Nighttime Measures of Activity

There were no significant correlations between measures of nighttime and daytime activity. However, when extreme values of nighttime activity were removed from the distribution (< −1.6 or>1.6 standard deviations), total nighttime activity was inversely correlated with total daytime activity (r(10) = −.63, p < .03). We also noted a positive correlation between nighttime activity and daytime yawning (r(10) = .81, p < .001).

DISCUSSION

We found support for the primary hypothesis that time spent inactive, presumably asleep, during the nighttime hours was positively correlated with serotonin turnover rate. Similarly, subjects with low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations had higher frequencies and a higher total duration of nighttime activity than did subjects with high CSF 5-HIAA concentrations. Thus, it appears that free-ranging rhesus males with low serotonergic neurotransmission are more prone to sleep difficulties such as frequent waking, high levels of nighttime activity, and possibly more tiredness during the day. These findings are consistent with research in impulsive male alcoholics with low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations showing that they exhibit aberrant diurnal activity rhythms with daytime naps, and frequent nighttime waking, and higher than normal overall daytime activity (Roy et al. 1986; Virkkunen et al. 1994). They are also consistent with a recent laboratory study of rhesus macaques showing that onset of nighttime sleep is inversely correlated with CSF 5-HIAA concentrations (Zajicek et al. 1997).

HVA and MHPG concentrations were not correlated with time spent inactive, but they were correlated with time spent active, suggesting that the catecholamines were not related to sleep, but instead associated with waking activity during the nighttime hours. This relationship between HVA and nocturnal activity remained significant even when 5-HIAA and HVA were simultaneously entered in a multiple regression analysis (t(14) = −4.48, p < .002). Similarly the relationship between nocturnal activity and MHPG remained significant even when 5-HIAA and MHPG were simultaneously entered in a multiple regression analysis (t(14) = 2.67, p < .03). These findings are intriguing, and suggest the possibility that the serotonin system is more strongly involved in sleep regulation, but when the subjects are awake, the serotonin and catecholamine systems are both involved in modulating activity.

We did not find an association between measures of daytime activity/inactivity and CSF 5-HIAA and HVA concentrations. This finding is of particular interest in view of research indicating that alcoholics with low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations and antisocial personality disorder (ASP) have higher than normal measures of daytime activity, while alcoholics with low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations and intermittent explosive disorder (IED) do not (Virkkunen et al. 1994). Thus, our macaque subjects appear to be more similar to patients with IED than to patients with ASP. This is consistent with the contention that increased daytime activity in ASP offenders reflects the interaction between ASP and attention deficit disorder (and its attendant hyperactivity), rather than simply lowered CSF 5-HIAA concentrations (Virkkunen et al. 1994). It must be noted that the present results are independent of previous alcohol consumption, which is known to affect peptide concentrations in the suprachiasmatic nucleus for long periods of time. To the degree that our findings generalize to the human condition, they suggest that the aberrant diurnal activity pattern seen in alcoholics with low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations is not simply a result of prolonged exposure to alcohol, but may instead be a primary behavior trait characterizing populations with impaired CNS serotonin functioning.

CSF MHPG concentrations showed a correlation pattern opposite to those of CSF 5-HIAA and HVA concentrations, exhibiting a positive correlation with nighttime activity. This is somewhat consistent with our previous studies (Higley et al. 1992b; Higley et al. 1996a) and some (Brown et al. 1979), but not with all, human studies (Virkkunen et al. 1989b) investigating alcoholism and violent behavior. In these studies, low CSF 5-HIAA and high CSF norepinephrine or MHPG concentrations were associated with violent behavior, wounding, and premature death from violent behavior. Unlike the other metabolites, MHPG exhibited correlations with daytime sleep and awake activity. Subjects with low CSF MHPG concentrations yawned and napped less, and spent more time active than did subjects with high CSF MHPG. Subsequent studies are underway to assess a more complete behavior daytime profile of subjects with low MHPG.

Jacobs, in a comprehensive reviews of CNS serotonin and diurnal activity patterns, showed that diurnal sleep-wake-arousal motor patterns are positively correlated with CNS serotonin firing patterns in many areas of the brain, including the suprachiasmatic nucleus (Jacobs 1987; Jacobs and Fornal 1998). CNS serotonin firing patterns are shown to be most active during patterns of locomotion, and least active during sleep, especially REM sleep (Jacobs 1987; Jacobs and Fornal 1998). While this seems somewhat paradoxical when considering our results (subjects with low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations were the most active, and least likely to sleep), it may be that the two different methods of measurement reflect different variables. Firing patterns of CNS serotonin neurons are phasic, specific to certain stimuli, and time-linked to motor activity or sleep patterns. CSF 5-HIAA concentration, on the other hand, is an integrated measure that has been shown to be trait-like across stimuli and situations, and while individual CSF 5-HIAA concentrations are reflective of changing states and stressors (Higley et al. 1996a; Higley et al. 1991; Higley et al. 1992b), interindividual differences are enduring. To reconcile this apparent discrepancy electrophysiological studies would need to be performed on subjects with high and low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations. These studies would be of considerable interest, since subjects with low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations show numerous impulse control deficits and it is possible that subjects with low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations show a less time-linked phasic link between motor patterns and CNS serotonin activity which may be related to their characteristic behavioral deficits.

This study contributes to the development of a nonhuman primate model that describes the behavioral correlates of naturally occurring variations in central serotonin turnover. In this long-term study trait-like variation in CSF 5-HIAA is correlated with an array of deleterious behavioral patterns which include severe aggression and wounding (inverse), risky, impulsive leaping (inverse), sociality (positive), early death (inverse), and sexual interactions with insemination (positive) (Higley et al. 1996b; Higley et al. 1992a; Higley et al. 1996a; Higley et al. 1996c; Joseph and Kennett 1980; Mehlman et al. 1995; Mehlman et al. 1994; Mehlman et al. 1997). The present study adds to the growing profile of behaviors associated with natural variation in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations by showing an inverse correlation between central serotonin turnover rate and total nighttime activity. We conclude that male rhesus monkeys with low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations have disrupted circadian patterns, are more active at night, and less active during the day.