Main

Self-injurious behavior (SIB) occurs in humans (reviewed by Winchel and Stanely 1991; Favazza and Rosenthal 1993) and other species (see Meyer-Holzapfel 1968; Jones and Barraclough 1978). In humans, SIB has been described as representing a continuum of behavior ranging from occasional noninjurious biting, scratching, and head banging to persistent self-biting and cutting associated with severe physical injuries (Harris 1995; Walsh and Rosen 1988). SIB also occurs in rhesus monkeys, most often as a result of self-biting (Tinklepaugh 1928; Erwin et al. 1973; Capitanio 1986). A retrospective study documenting the incidence of wounding in three primate colonies (Bayne et al. 1995) showed that, of 253 monkeys studied, 0.8% engaged in self-injurious behavior that resulted in wounds requiring veterinary treatment. Although severe self-biting in nonhuman primates is a relatively uncommon phenomenon, it nevertheless remains a serious concern, because it can lead to severe and sometimes fatal injuries.

Neuroleptics, anxiolytics, sedative-hypnotics, stimulants, antidepressants and mood stabilizers, anticonvulsants, behavior modification, antihypertensives and opiate antagonists have all been used as treatments for SIB with varying degrees of success. Recent reviews conclude that there is no single, universally accepted treatment for SIB (Harris 1995; Luchins 1990). In addition, there is evidence that effective doses of most medications used to treat SIB have unacceptable side effects (see review by Osman and Loschen 1992).

Serotonin (5-HT), is believed to be involved in the regulation of many behaviors including aggression, arousal, pain sensitivity, and irritability (Brown et al. 1979; Valzelli and Bernasconi 1979; Cleare and Bond 1995), and could theoretically play a role in SIB. If SIB, indeed, results from misdirected aggression, hyper- or hypoarousal, and/or reduced pain sensitivity, modifying the functioning of the serotonergic system might be expected to alter such SIB-related behaviors as selfdirected aggression. Low central nervous system (CNS) 5-HT turnover rate, as measured by low cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) concentrations of the major metabolite of 5-HT, 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) in CSF, has been observed to be correlated with high levels of aggression in both humans and nonhuman primates (Brown et al. 1979; Linnoila et al. 1983; Roy and Linnoila 1988; Roy et al. 1988; Higley et al. 1996). Pharmacological reduction of the availability of 5-HT and its precursors increases aggressive behaviors (Chamberlain et al. 1987; Young et al. 1989). Conversely, pharmacologically increasing availability of 5-HT and its precursors results in reduced rates of aggression (Lindgren and Kantak 1987; Raleigh 1987; Shea et al. 1990; see review by Olivier et al. 1991). Some patients who exhibit selfaggressivity have low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations (Lopez-Ibor et al. 1985). If SIB is thought of as aggression turned toward oneself, it is possible that increasing CNS 5-HT function could decrease or eliminate SIB.

One method for increasing CNS 5-HT functioning is to increase the availability of its precursor, tryptophan (TRP). TRP has been used successfully to increase 5-HT synthesis in a wide range of species under a variety of conditions (Pardridge 1977; Gillman et al. 1981; Chamberlain et al. 1987; Young et al. 1989; Shea et al. 1991; Mench and Shea-Moore 1995). Given these findings, we hypothesized that TRP supplementation would: (1) increase central 5-HT turnover and decrease self-biting in rhesus monkeys with a history of SIB; and (2) in normal (nonSIB) individually housed rhesus monkeys, increased central 5-HT turnover would lead to changes in their behavioral repertoires. To the best of our knowledge, no one to date has specifically examined the physiological and behavioral responses of SIB and normal individually housed rhesus monkeys to oral TRP treatment.

Two studies were performed to test these hypotheses. In both studies, the amount of TRP in the normal and SIB monkeys’ diets was increased by offering them flavored treats supplemented with TRP. This method for administering TRP is both unobtrusive and provokes minimal stress responses in the subject. CSF 5-HIAA and behaviors of the monkeys were measured at regular intervals throughout the studies. Because TRP loading has been associated with a reduction in availability of the catecholamines’ precursor (Huether et al. 1992), and because 5-HT interacts with CNS norepinephrine system (Raleigh 1987), changes in CNS concentrations of the major metabolites of norepinephrine and dopamine were also monitored to determine any possible effect that TRP might have on the catecholamine turnover rates in the SIB and normal monkeys.

METHODS

Study One: Self-Injuring Monkeys

Subjects

The subjects were selected by requesting rhesus monkeys with a history of spontaneous SIB from 14 primate veterinarians with access to large nonhuman primate populations. Over a 2-year period, nine individually housed male rhesus monkeys were identified as having a recent history of developing SIB of sufficient severity to require veterinary attention (i.e., suturing). The monkey's background information (surgical and/or research history) was screened to determine if any procedures had been conducted that would preclude using the monkey in the study (e.g., brain lesioning or castration). Two subjects were rejected as a result of this criterion leaving a sample size of seven individuals. One goal of the study was to treat the SIB monkeys without adding such a stressor as relocation to unfamiliar surroundings. Therefore, TRP was administered to each individual in its familiar home-cage setting. All research was carried out under NIH guidelines, using an approved animal study protocol.

Study Design

There were three phases to the study: (1) a 1-week placebo baseline; (2) a 21-day treatment period; and (3) a 1-week placebo posttest. During baseline and posttest, placebo was administered twice a day in the same fashion as during the treatment phase. During the treatment period (phase 2), the monkeys received TRP orally twice a day at a dose of 100 mg/kg in flavored, commercially available primate treats. The morning TRP/placebo dose was administered between 0630 and 0800 hours and the afternoon dose was given between 1400 and 1600 hours. A pilot study of SIB monkeys in which 50 and 100 mg/kg doses were used showed that the 100 mg/kg was the lowest effective dose that increased CSF 5-HIAA concentrations and decreased self-biting. The TRP was added to the treat, according to our specifications, by BioServe®. Each 5-gram treat contained 200 mg of L-tryptophan and was scored into quarters for easy dosing.

Behavioral Observations

Behavioral sampling was conducted by one researcher who had more than 10 years of experience collecting nonhuman primate behavioral data. Data were obtained using the Observer® software package on an IBM laptop computer, which allowed the recording of the frequency and duration of each of 13 behaviors (see below). Each monkey was observed five times a week during the pretest and posttest periods and 10 times during the treatment period. Observations were randomized across the mornings and afternoons. Morning scoring occurred between 0700 and 1200 hours. Afternoon scoring sessions were completed between 1200 noon and 1600 hours. Each scoring session lasted 20 minutes, producing a total of 6 hours and 40 minutes of data for each subject. The following behaviors were recorded using an objectively defined scoring system: autoerotic, tactile/oral exploration of the environment, self-groom, passive, stereotyped locomotion, stereotypies (i.e., self-clasp, eye poke/salute, etc.), threats (i.e., directed toward the observer or other animals in the room), social behavior (i.e., rump or neck presenting, lipsmacking directed toward the observer or other animals in the room), fear (i.e., fear grimace toward observer or other animals in the room), contact enrichment, manipulate enrichment, self-biting, and SIB (i.e., spontaneous severe self-biting resulting in serious wounds that require clinical treatment). The veterinary care staff made daily rounds at regularly scheduled intervals to assess the physical condition of each animal in their facility, which ensured that injuries would be treated immediately if self-injury did occur.

Physiological Data

CSF was collected at the end of the 3rd week of TRP treatment. To allow sufficient time for TRP to be metabolized and act centrally, cisternal CSF samples were obtained 3 to 6 hours after dosing, between the hours of 1100 and 1300. The animals were anesthetized with ketamine hydrochloride (15 mg/kg) for these procedures. We obtained 3 milliliters of CSF once each during the baseline, treatment, and posttest periods from each subject, using a 5-cc syringe with a 22-gauge needle following the procedures described by Higley et al. (1996). Samples were obtained in less than 30 minutes following anesthesia induction and immediately frozen in liquid nitrogen. Previous studies have shown that CSF monoamine concentrations are unaffected by ketamine if samples are collected within 30 minutes of ketamine injection (Brammer et al. 1987; Bacopoulos et al. 1979). CSF was stored at −80 C° until assayed for 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA), homovanillic acid (HVA), and 3-methoxy 5-hydroxyphenylethyleneglycol (MHPG) by gas chromatography-mass spectrometry using deuterated internal standards (Polinsky et al. 1988). The coefficients of variation for the assays of the metabolites were 6, 2, and 5%, respectively.

Statistical Analyses

Statistical analyses were computed using analysis of variance (ANOVA) and, where appropriate, Friedman's nonparametric analyses. Because there were an unequal number of behavioral samples taken during the three phases of the study (e.g., five scores during pre- and posttest and 10 during the TRP treatment), data were standardized by calculating the mean total duration (in seconds) per 20 minute scoring session for each of the three phases of the study. Preliminary analyses indicated that the variances for self-biting data were not homogeneous; thus, the nonparametric Friedman's test (Sokal and Rohlf 1987) was used to compare the means of self-biting across the study. A one-way repeated measure ANOVA was used to analyze all other data.

RESULTS

Study One—SIB Monkeys

During TRP treatment, the duration of self-biting was shorter than in the pre- and posttest periods (see Figure 1; df=2, Chi corrected for ties=6.952, p=.03). SIB did not occur during any phase of the study. TRP supplementation also markedly increased CSF 5- HIAA concentrations (See Figure 2 ) above pre- and posttest concentrations (See Table 1); F=12.149, df=2, p=.0013). CSF concentrations of MHPG and HVA during the treatment phase of the study were unaffected by TRP (p > .3—see Table 1). Except for the duration of self-biting, there were no other behavioral changes attributable to TRP supplementation (p > .3).

Figure 1
figure 1

Mean duration and SD for self-biting during Study One; self-biting decreased during TRP treatment and increased during the posttest period

Figure 2
figure 2

Mean and SD for CSF concentrations of 5-HIAA (picomoles/milliliter) for each of the phases of Study One

Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations for CSF Concentrations of 5-HIAA, MHPG, and HVA (in picomoles/milliliter) from SIB Monkeys (n=7), Study One

Study Two—Normal Monkeys

Based upon the results in Study One, we examined the effect of TRP supplementation on the behavior and physiology of normal rhesus monkeys.

Subjects

Fourteen adult male rhesus monkeys were randomly selected for the study from the NIH Animal Center, Laboratory of Comparative Ethology colony. All monkeys were individually housed in stainless steel quad cages that provided each individual with 0.56 m2 of floor space. Prior values for the physiological measures in the study were available on these individuals from longitudinal data previously gathered (in some cases, since birth). Because appetite can be affected by central serotonin manipulation (Spring 1986), all monkeys were weighed weekly, and the number of biscuits that remained from the previous feeding was recorded prior to dispensing each monkey's next food ration.

Study Design

In Study One, marked changes in behavior occurred by the end of the 1st week of TRP treatment, and there were no differences between weeks 1 and 2. Therefore, the normal monkeys in Study Two were administered TRP for 1 week at each dosage level. There were four randomized treatments: placebo, 50, 100, and 200 mg/kg TRP. Each treatment was administered for 1 week, and there was a 2 week placebo washout period between each treatment. TRP treats were administered orally twice a day, as described in Study One. The placebo treats provided during the placebo phases were identical to the TRP treats, lacking only in the TRP supplementation. Two subjects that refused the TRP treat were administered TRP in such alternative vehicles as pudding, marshmallow creme, yogurt, jam, or jelly. In these instances, the amount of vehicle was minimized, so as not to affect the transport of TRP across the blood–brain barrier due to competition for the large neutral amino acid carrier. Preliminary analyses of these two subjects’ physiological data indicated that there was no differential effect of providing TRP in the alternative vehicles. At the end of the experiment, a dose of 400 mg/kg was administered to determine if a higher dose would result in physiological changes. Because most subjects refused to consume the 400 mg/kg when it was administered in the treat form, an alternative method of administration was manufactured. A grain-based nutritionally adequate diet comparable to the standard chow that the monkeys received was formulated and supplemented with additional TRP to the 400 mg/kg TRP dose. This diet was fed to the monkeys in lieu of their standard primate chow for one week. Of the 13 monkeys, only five consumed the diet.

Behavioral Observations

Each monkey was observed by two trained technicians who were blind to the treatment condition and the subject's CSF 5-HIAA concentrations. Interobserver reliability was tested using the Observer® reliability test, and observers were in agreement greater than 95% of the time. Animals were observed in a random order 4 days per treatment period between the hours of 1100 and 1400. Behaviors were recorded for 10 minutes per session on an IBM laptop equipped with the Observer® program. A total of 2 hours and 40 minutes of behavioral data were collected for each subject. The behaviors recorded for Study Two were the same as those described in Study One with two exceptions. The first was that eat and drink were divided into separate categories rather than being collapsed with tactile/oral exploration. In addition, sleep (defined as motionless with eyes closed for more than 15 seconds) was added because, although none of the SIB subjects slept during any of the observations in Study One, the subjects in Study Two would be receiving twice the dose of TRP used in Study One, and drowsiness has been shown to increase following TRP augmentation (Steinberg et al. 1992; Olsen et al. 1994).

Physiological Data

Physiological data were collected once during each test phase in the same manner as in Study One, with one modification–-in addition to collecting CSF, 7 milliliters of blood was obtained once on the last day of each treatment period. Blood was centrifuged, and the plasma collected from the chilled blood was frozen on liquid nitrogen and stored at −80° C until it could be assayed for TRP concentrations using high-performance liquid chromatography (HPLC) by Nicols Institute through Maryland Medical Metpath (coefficient of variation for the assay was 3.4%).

Statistical Analyses

A two-way repeated measures ANOVA was used to compare each of the behaviors across the four phases. The within-group factors were treatment (placebo, 50, 100, or 200 mg/kg treatment) and day of treatment (Monday through Friday). The physiological data were analyzed using a one-way repeated measures ANOVA comparing the three treatment phases (baseline, TRP, posttest). Pearson product moment correlations were computed to examine correlations between TRP with CSF 5-HIAA, HVA, MHPG concentrations, and behavior. One subject was removed from the study during the second treatment period because of a physical illness unrelated to the experimental treatment. Analyses were completed on the remaining 13 animals. In addition, the baseline CSF 5-HIAA concentrations of the original 14 normal subjects in this study and the seven SIB monkeys from Study One were compared using a one-way ANOVA.

RESULTS

Study Two—Normal Monkeys

When compared to placebo, the subjects demonstrated no significant behavioral or physiological changes (p > .3) during any of the three TRP doses (see Table 2). When plasma was obtained from the five subjects that consumed the 400 mg/kg dose, after 3 days of that treatment, the plasma TRP concentrations were more than six times higher (mean=381.0 mmol/ml) than TRP concentrations obtained during placebo (mean=59.4 mmol/ml) and all other TRP treatment dosages (mean=64 mmol/ml) values.

Table 2 Means and Standard Deviations for CSF Concentrations of 5-HIAA, MHPG, and HVA (in picomoles/milliliter) from Normal Rhesus Monkeys (n=13), Study Two

When 5-HIAA concentrations in CSF obtained from normal and SIB monkeys during the pretest periods of both studies were compared, there was no difference between the concentration of 5-HIAA in SIB monkeys (mean=207.6 pmol/ml) and normal monkeys (mean=191.4 pmol/ml).

DISCUSSION

In Study One, TRP increased CSF concentrations of 5-HIAA and reduced the rate of self-biting in rhesus monkeys that previously exhibited SIB. Although some self-biting was observed in the SIB subjects during the TRP treatment period, the behavior was not of sufficient severity to result in SIB, as measured by wounds requiring clinical treatment from the veterinary staff (i.e., suturing). Our results show that TRP seems to be an effective treatment for self-biting that may lead to SIB.

The SIB monkeys studied had average CSF 5-HIAA concentrations similar to those of normal monkeys, which is contrary to the findings of Kraemer et al. (1989), who reported high CSF 5-HIAA concentrations among monkeys exhibiting SIB that were socially deprived during rearing. Rearing history (mother- or peer-deprived) (Fittinghoff et al. 1974; Kraemer and Clarke 1990) can also affect the development of self-biting and its responsiveness to 5-HT (Kraemer and Clarke 1990). Information on the rearing history of our SIB subjects was not available; thus, it is possible that the discrepancy between the findings presented here and those of Kraemer and Clarke (1990) may be attributable to differences in age or early rearing histories of the subjects. SIB and self-biting are not limited to monkeys that have experienced social isolation early in life (Erwin et al. 1973; see Capitanio 1986 for a review). For example, during the past decade, there have been three cases of spontaneous SIB occurring in monkeys born in our laboratory, and all three had been reared in social groups.

Whereas humans with suicidal tendencies have been reported to have low CSF concentrations of 5-HIAA (Brown et al. 1982a, b; Asberg et al. 1987), the CSF 5-HIAA concentrations of SIB monkeys were not especially low, and some of the normal monkeys in Study Two with no history of SIB had markedly low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations. Therefore, in general, SIB in rhesus monkeys would not seem to provide an appropriate model for the human suicidal behavior related to low CNS serotonin functioning.

The CSF catecholamine concentrations were not related to the severity of SIB and were unaffected by TRP treatment, but the sensitivity of the SIB monkeys to stimulation by means of serotonergic augmentation (TRP) was evident in both their behavior and physiology. Although the long-term outcome for most of the SIB monkeys included in the present study is unknown, we were able to complete physiological follow-ups on two of the monkeys 8 months posttreatment. CSF samples taken at that follow-up were assayed for 5-HIAA concentrations, and the values did not differ from those taken during the pretest period. To date, only one of the seven monkeys in Study One has inflicted self-injury serious enough to require veterinary treatment since the end of the study, and that injury occurred 2 months after completion of the study. One advantage of TRP treatment of self-biting in monkeys is that it can be readily treated in this manner with minimal expense and labor.

Contrary to results from studies of vervet monkeys (Raleigh et al. 1983 and personal communication 1992), most of our subjects readily ate the TRP supplements at doses less than 400 mg/kg. TRP supplementation of normal rhesus monkeys in Study Two did not result in increases in plasma TRP concentrations or 5-HIAA concentrations. Although a dose of 400 mg/kg twice a day did increase plasma TRP concentrations, the dose was voluntarily consumed by only a few of the normal monkeys.

SUMMARY

Oral TRP supplements were effective in the treatment of self-biting in rhesus monkeys. We found that self-biting decreased and CSF 5-HIAA concentrations increased when monkeys exhibiting this behavior were fed the TRP supplements. Comparable TRP supplements had no apparent behavioral or biochemical effects on central serotonin turnover rate in nonSIB monkeys.