Main

(±)3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA; ecstasy) was initially synthesized in 1914, and subsequent to a short period in the mid-1980s of experimental use as an adjunct in psychotherapy, MDMA became a common recreational drug of abuse (Green et al. 1995). As the popularity of MDMA has increased, psychiatric complications have emerged associated with repeated drug use. Although a diversity of MDMA-associated psychiatric problems have been reported, a frequently identified feature is the induction of anxiety and paranoia, sometimes culminating in panic attacks and paranoid psychosis (McCann and Ricaurte 1991; Creighton et al. 1991; Benzazzi and Mazzoli 1991; Whitaker-Azmitia and Aronson 1989). Although rigorous demographic analysis of the association between these relatively common psychiatric disorders and repeated MDMA use remains to be conducted (Green et al. 1995), the progressive emergence of anxiety and paranoia is reminiscent of the induction of similar psychopathologies in a subpopulation of methamphetamine and cocaine addicts (Ellinwood 1967; Post 1975; Brady et al. 1991; Satel et al. 1991).

Whereas there is some overlap in the behavioral pharmacology of MDMA and other commonly abused amphetamine derivatives, there also exist clear differences. General overlap can be found in the induction of behavioral excitation (Gold and Koob 1989; Spanos and Yamamoto 1989; Callaway et al. 1991; Callaway and Geyer 1992; McNamara et al. 1995; Dafters 1995) and conditioned place preference (Bilsky et al. 1990; Marona-Lewicka et al. 1996). Furthermore, the self-administration of MDMA has been reported in animals first trained to self-administer cocaine (Lamb and Griffiths 1987; Beardsley et al. 1986). Neurochemical overlap is also present because systemic, intracranial, or in vitro administration of MDMA produces a dose-dependent elevation of extracellular serotonin and dopamine in the striatum and nucleus accumbens (Yamamoto and Spanos 1988; Fitzgerald and Reid 1990; Gough et al. 1991; Nash and Brodkin 1991; White et al. 1994). In contrast to this general overlap, pharmacological studies reveal a critical involvement of serotonin transmission in the behavioral and neurochemical effects of MDMA (Geyer 1996), but not amphetamine (Cunningham et al. 1992; Seiden et al. 1993). Predominant involvement of serotonin in the effects of MDMA versus amphetamine is revealed by the facts that: (1) the behavioral profile of MDMA differs from that of amphetamine and is markedly attenuated by inhibiting serotonin synthesis and 5-HT1 receptors (Callaway et al. 1990, 1992); (2) the increase in dopamine release is partly blocked by inhibiting the serotonin transporter or blocking 5-HT2 receptors (Gudelsky and Nash 1996); (3) humans and rats readily distinguish MDMA from amphetamine, and in humans the pattern of abuse differs between MDMA and amphetamine (Geyer 1996; Green et al. 1995; Steele et al. 1994); and (4) repeated MDMA administration produces an enduring depletion of forebrain serotonin in rats and primates that is associated with the selective destruction of fine diameter fibers arising from the dorsal raphe (O'Hearn et al. 1988; Wilson et al. 1989).

Whereas the involvement of serotonin clearly distinguishes the acute effects of MDMA, it is possible that repeated administration of MDMA produces enduring neuroadaptations in the brain that overlap with other commonly abused amphetamines, and may contribute for the induction of paranoia by all drugs in this class (see above). Amphetamine-induced psychopathology has been modeled in rodents as the progressive sensitization of locomotor and stereotyped behavior elicited by repeated drug administration (Segal and Schuckit 1983; Robinson and Becker 1986). A consistent neurochemical feature of enduring behavioral sensitization to amphetamine and cocaine is an increased capacity of these drugs to release dopamine in the nucleus accumbens and striatum (Kolta et al. 1985; Robinson et al. 1988; Pettit et al. 1990; Kalivas and Duffy 1993; Wolf et al. 1993; Paulson and Robinson 1995; Heidbreder et al. 1996). This rat model of psychostimulant psychosis was used to determine whether repeated MDMA administration induces behavioral sensitization and augmented dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens.

METHODS

Animal Housing and Surgery

Male Sprague-Dawley rats (Simonsen Laboratories, Gilroy, CA) were housed in groups of four with food and water made available ad libitum. A 12/12–h light/dark cycle was used with the lights on at 6:30 A.M. Rats used for the microdialysis experiment were anesthetized with Equithesin (3.0 ml/kg, IP), and a guide cannula (14 mm of 20 ga stainless steel tubing) was stereotaxically implanted 20 ga over the nucleus accumbens (A/P 9.0 mm; M/L 1.5 mm; D/V −0.5 mm; relative to the interaural line) according to the atlas of Pelligrino et al. (1979). The guide cannula was cemented in place by affixing dental acrylic to three stainless steel screws that were tapped into the skull. After surgery the rats were individually housed for the duration of the experiments.

Daily MDMA Treatment and Behavioral Measures

The treatment regimens used are outlined in Table 1. All rats were placed in the photocell apparatus 24 h prior to beginning repeated (±)3,4-MDMA (a gift from NIDA) or saline injections (day 1). After a 1-h adaptation on day 1, rats were injected with saline (1.0 ml/kg, SC) and returned to the photocell cage where photocell beam breaks were quantified for 60 min. The next day (day 2), the subjects were divided into two MDMA and a saline treatment group. After the adaptation period, the subjects were administered either saline or MDMA (5 mg/kg, SC), and motor activity was monitored for an additional 2 h. Over the next 4 days (days 3–6), rats were given either saline (1.0 ml/kg, SC, twice daily) or a low (5 mg/kg, SC, once daily) or high (20 mg/kg, SC, twice daily) MDMA treatment regimen in the home cage. One day after the last repeated injection (day 7), the rats were adapted to the photocell boxes for 60 min, followed by an injection of saline and motor activity was quantified for an additional 60 min. The next day (day 8), the rats were again adapted to the photocell cage for 60 min and the rats pretreated with saline were injected with saline, whereas both of the MDMA pretreatment groups were injected with MDMA (5 mg/kg, SC). Motor activity was monitored for an additional 120 min after the saline or MDMA injection.

Table 1 Treatment Regimens

Eleven days after the last injection of MDMA or saline (day 18), rats were returned to the photocell chamber. After a 60-min adaptation, the rats were administered saline and motor activity was monitored for 60 min. The next day (day 19), the rats were returned to the photocell cage and all animals were administered MDMA (5 mg/kg, SC). Seven days later (day 26), the rats were again adapted to the photocell cage for 60 min then administered saline (1 ml/kg, IP). The next day (day 27), after a 60-min adaptation, all animals were administered cocaine (15 mg/kg, IP) and photocell counts monitored for an additional 120 min. On day 34, the rats were decapitated and brain tissue harvested for measurement of monoamines (see below).

The photocell apparatus used consisted of 16 photocell cages (Omnitech Inc., Columbus, OH), each located in a separate sound attenuated chamber with individual light and air sources. Each apparatus contained 16 infrared photocells located 2 cm off the cage floor to measure horizontal activity (e.g., a combination of locomotion and stereotypy) and 8 photocells located 6 cm off the floor to estimate vertical activity (e.g., rearing).

Microdialysis

The dialysis probes were constructed as described by Robinson and Wishaw (1988), with 2.0 to 2.5 mm of active dialysis membrane exposed at the tip. The probes were inserted through the guide cannula into the nucleus accumbens the night prior to the experiment. The next day, dialysis buffer (2.7 mmol/L KCl, 140 mmol/L NaCl, 1.2 mmol/L CaCl2, 1.2 mmol/L MgCl2, 5.0 mmol/L d-glucose, plus 0.2 mmol/L phosphate-buffered saline to give a pH value of 7.4 and a final sodium concentration of 140.7 mmol/L) was advanced through the probe at a rate of 1.9 ml/min via a syringe pump (Harvard Instruments, Boston, MA) for 2 h.

Rats were treated on days 1 through 8 as described above (See Table 1) with repeated saline or MDMA (20 mg/kg, SC), and 11 days later (day 18) a microdialysis probe was inserted through the guide cannula into the nucleus accumbens. Approximately 16 h later (day 19), 20-min baseline samples were collected for 100 min, followed by an injection of saline (1.0 ml/kg, SC) and 80 min later by an injection of MDMA (5 mg/kg, SC). During both baseline and drug administration, samples were taken every 20 min, and the experiment was conducted in a photocell cage to permit the simultaneous monitoring of motor activity (Omnitech Electronics, Columbus, OH). At the end of the experiment, the dialysis probe was removed, and the animal was administered an overdose of pentobarbital (>100 mg/kg, IP).

Measurement of Monoamines

The dialysis samples were collected into microfuge tubes in 20 μl of mobile phase (0.1 mol/L citric acid, 75 mmol/L Na2HPO4, 0.6–1.0 mmol/L heptane sulfonic acid, 0.1 mmol/L EDTA, 13% methanol, V/V, pH = 3.8–4.2) plus 2.0 pmol dihdroxybenzylamine as the internal standard, and placed in a freezer (−70°C) until analyzed for dopamine content. The samples were placed in a refrigerated autosampler (Gilson Medical Supplies, Middleton, WI) and dopamine content measured using high-performance liquid chromatography (HPLC) with electrochemical detection. The dopamine was separated using a 25 cm C-18 reversed-phase column (Bioanalytical Systems, West Lafayette, IN) and oxidized/reduced using coulometric detection (ESA Inc., Bedford, MA). Three electrodes were used: a pre-injection port guard cell (0.4 V) to oxidize the mobile phase, an oxidation analytical electrode (0.3 V), and a reduction analytical electrode (−0.2 V). Peaks were recorded on a chart recorder and compared to an external standard curve (10 to 1000 fmol).

Tissue levels of monoamines were determined using HPLC with electrochemical detection. One week after the final cocaine injection (day 34), the rats were decapitated and the prefrontal cortex, core and shell of the nucleus accumbens, and dorsolateral striatum were dissected on an ice-cooled glass plate. A coronal slab of tissue was obtained by dropping a razor blade 0.5 mm rostral to the olfactory tubercle and making a parallel cut 1.5 mm caudal. The prefrontal cortex was hand dissected (Deutch et al. 1985), and the other brain regions obtained using a 16-gauge punch. Immediately after dissection, bilateral tissue pieces were placed in 0.5 ml of mobile phase (0.1 mol/L trichloroacetic acid, 0.01 mol/L sodium acetate, 0.1 moltc/L EDTA and 18% methanol, pH = 3.85) containing 2 × 10−7 mol/L isoproterenol as an internal standard, sonicated and centrifuged (13,000 × g) for 2 min. The resulting pellet was assayed for protein content using Folin reagent, and 50 to 150 μl of the supernatant injected into an HPLC system. The monoamines were separated using an ODS C-18 reversed-phase column (Bioanalytical, Lafayette, IN) and detected at 0.7 V by a glassy carbon electrode. Peak height was measured against a standard curve (1 to 100 pmol), and the final data expressed as pmol per mg protein.

Histology and Statistics

Rats used for microdialysis were perfused intracardially with phosphate-buffered saline (60 ml) followed by 10% formalin (60 ml). The brains were removed, stored in 10% formalin for at least 1 week, and coronal sections (100 μm thick) were taken at the level of the nucleus accumbens with a vibratome. The sections were mounted on gelatin-coated slides and stained with Cresyl violet. Probe placements were determined according to the atlas of Paxinos and Watson (1986) by an individual unaware of the rats’ neurochemical response. The data were evaluated using a one- or two-way ANOVA. Post hoc comparisons between multiple groups were made using a least significant difference test described by Milliken and Johnson (1984), and multiple comparisons to a single treatment were made using a Dunnett's test.

RESULTS

Behavioral Sensitization to Repeated MDMA Administration

Figure 1 shows that acute injection of MDMA (5.0 mg/kg, SC) on day 2 produces an increase in horizontal photocell counts, distance traveled, and stereotypy counts compared with the saline injection made on day 1. In contrast, there was no statistically consistent increase in vertical photocell counts after acute MDMA administration (data not shown).

Figure 1
figure 1

Effect of repeated MDMA on saline- and MDMA-induced motor activity. Rats were injected with saline (1 ml/kg, SC) or MDMA (5 mg/kg, SC) on the days indicated in parentheses (see Table 1). On days 3–6, subjects received injections of saline or 5 or 20 mg/kg, SC, of MDMA (MDMA-5 or MDMA-20). The bar graphs in panels A, C, and D illustrate the mean ± SEM total photocell counts, distance traveled (cm), and stereotypy over the 120 min after injecting saline or MDMA. The line graph in panel B shows the time course of horizontal photocell counts during the adaptation period and during the 120 min after injecting MDMA on day 19. The data were statistically evaluated using a two-way ANOVA with repeated measures over day of injection or time (Panel B). Panel A: treatmentF(2,17) = 9.03, p = .002; dayF(7,119) = 74.00, p < .001; interactionF(14,119) = 4.52, p < .001. Panel B: treatmentF(2,17) = 7.96, p = .004; timeF(11,187) = 19.43, p < .001; interactionF(22,187) = 3.739, p < .001. Panel C: treatmentF(2,17) = 7.60, p = .004; dayF(7,119) = 63.50, p < .001; interactionF(14,119 = 4.01, p < .001. Panel D: treatmentF(2,17) = 9.15, p = .002; dayF(7,119) = 105.43, p < .001; interactionF(14,119) = 6.84, p < .001. *p < .05, compared with saline (1) within each treatment group, using a least significant difference test (Milliken and Johnson 1984). +p < .05, compared with MDMA (2) within each treatment group. @ p < .05, compared with the b.i.d. saline group on day 19.

The motor stimulant effect of MDMA (5.0 mg/kg, SC) was significantly augmented after repeated treatments with either 5.0 or 20.0 mg/kg, SC, of MDMA for 4 days. At 11 days after the last repeated injection of MDMA or saline (day 19), MDMA-induced elevations in horizontal photocell counts, distance traveled, and stereotypy were greater in the two MDMA pretreatment groups than in the saline-pretreated groups. Similarly, the response to MDMA on day 19 was greater than the effect of the first MDMA injection made on day 2. In contrast, when MDMA was administered 48 hr after discontinuing repeated treatments (day 8), the behavioral stimulant effect was significantly greater than the first MDMA administration (day 2) only in the group pretreated with daily injections of 5.0 mg/kg of MDMA. The augmentation was manifest in horizontal photocell counts and distance traveled, but not in the measure of stereotypy.

The time course data for horizontal photocell counts after MDMA administration on day 19 is also shown in Figure 1 MDMA produced more photocell counts in both MDMA pretreatment groups between 30 and 90 min after injection compared with the effect in the saline-pretreated group. At no time after injection did the response to MDMA differ between the two MDMA pretreatment groups.

Figure 2 shows that cross-sensitization with the motor stimulant effect of cocaine (15 mg/kg, IP) was produced in rats pretreated with repeated injections of 20.0 mg/kg, SC of MDMA, but not in the group pretreated with the lower dose of MDMA. The cross-sensitization was apparent for both horizontal activity and distance traveled, whereas the effect of cocaine on stereotypy was not significantly different between the MDMA and saline pretreatment groups. The time course data for cocaine-induced horizontal photocell counts reveals that the augmented response occurred during the first 30 min after cocaine administration.

Figure 2
figure 2

Cross-sensitization between repeated MDMA and cocaine. All rats used in the data shown in Figure 1 were administered cocaine (15 mg/kg, IP) 1 week after the last injection of MDMA (i.e., on day 27). The bar graph illustrates the mean ± SEM total photocell counts, distance traveled (cm), and stereotypy counts over the 120 min after injecting cocaine. The stereotypy counts were multiplied by 30 to permit illustration on the same scale as horizontal photocell counts and distance traveled. The line graph shows the time course of horizontal photocell counts during the adaptation period and the 120 min after injecting cocaine. The data were statistically evaluated using a one-way ANOVA for total behavior and a two-way ANOVA with repeated measures over time for the time course data. Total Behavior: horizontalF(2,21) = 3.79, p = .039, distanceF(2,21) = 3.42, p = .052; stereotypyF(2,21) = .79, p = .466. Timecourse: treatmentF(2,17) = 4.34, p = .027; timeF(11,187) = 58.38, p < .001; interactionF(22,187) = 4.983, p < .001, *p < .05 comparing saline with MDMA-5 or MDMA-20 using a Dunnett's test for the total behavioral data and a least significant difference test for the time course data.

Neurochemical Sensitization to Repeated MDMA Administration

Microdialysis was used to measure the extracellular content of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens at 2 weeks after discontinuing repeated MDMA injections (20 mg/kg, SC, b.i.d.). Neither the basal level of extracellular dopamine nor horizontal photocell counts differed between the repeated MDMA and saline pretreatment groups (Figure 3 ). Likewise, neither the behavioral or neurochemical effects of acute saline administration (1.0 ml/kg, SC) were distinguished between the two pretreatment groups. An acute injection of MDMA (5.0 mg/kg, SC) significantly increased motor activity in both groups, with the response in the MDMA pretreatment group being significantly greater than the control group between 60 and 120 min after injection. Similarly, the increase in extracellular dopamine by MDMA was greater in the nucleus accumbens of rats pretreated with repeated MDMA injections between 40 and 120 min after acute MDMA administration.

Figure 3
figure 3

Augmented increase in extracellular dopamine content in the nucleus accumbens of MDMA-sensitized subjects. Rats were pretreated with repeated saline (n = 7) or MDMA (n = 6; 20 mg/kg, SC) as described in the legend for Figure 1. Twelve days later, dialysis was conducted in the nucleus accumbens. Following five baseline samples, saline (open arrow; 1 ml/kg, SC) was administered and 80 min later MDMA was injected (closed arrow; 5 mg/kg, SC). Panel A shows the raw mean ± SEM extracellular dopamine levels (uncorrected for probe recovery), panel B shows the data in panel A normalized to the percent change from the average of the five baseline samples, and panel C shows the mean ± SEM horizontal photocell counts obtained simultaneously with the dialysis samples. The data were statistically evaluated using a two-way ANOVA with repeated measures over time. Panel A: treatmentF(1,11) = 1.18, p = .301; timeF(15, 165) = 6.18, p < .001; interactionF(15,165) = 2.58, p = .002. Panel B: treatmentF(1,11) = 1.66, p .225; timeF(15,165) = 12.36, p < .001; interactionF(15,165) = 1.98, p = .020. Panel C: treatmentF(1,11) = .80, p = .390; timeF(15,165) = 6.37, p < .001; interactionF(15,165) = 1.69, p = .058. *p < .05, comparing saline with MDMA at individual times, using a least significant difference test (Milliken and Johnson 1984).

Figure 4 shows the location of microdialysis probes within the nucleus accumbens of the animals used in the data shown in Figure 3. The probes were primarily in the medial core of the rostral half of the nucleus accumbens. However, portions of the active regions of the probes were variably located dorsal or ventral to the core in the ventral striatum or lateral limb of the shell of the nucleus accumbens, respectively.

Figure 4
figure 4

Location of dialysis probes in the nucleus accumbens from animals used in the data shown in Figure 3. The solid lines on the left refer to probe location in the rats pretreated with repeated MDMA injections (n = 6), and the dashed lines on the right indicate the location of probes in the repeated saline treatment group (n = 7). The numbers indicated mm rostral to interaural zero in the atlas of Milliken and Johnson 1984. Nc = core of the nucleus accumbens; Ns = shell of the nucleus accumbens.

Effect of Repeated MDMA on Monamine Tissue Levels

Table 2 shows the tissue levels of dopamine, serotonin, and norepinephrine in the brains of the rats 1 or 2 weeks after completing the experiments shown in Figure 1 and Figure 2. The highest daily dosing regimen of MDMA produced a significant reduction in the level of serotonin in all brain nuclei examined, including the prefrontal cortex, shell and core of the nucleus accumbens, and the dorsalateral striatum. A depletion in serotonin was not elicited in any brain region by pretreatment with the lower dose of MDMA. In contrast, in the low dose MDMA group, the level of serotonin was elevated in the prefrontal cortex and shell of the nucleus accumbens. Similarly, the levels of extracellular dopamine were significantly elevated in the shell and core of the nucleus accumbens by the low dose regimen of MDMA. The level of dopamine was also increased in the core after b.i.d. injections of the highest dose of MDMA. No alteration was measured in the levels of norepinephrine in either the prefrontal cortex or shell.

Table 2 Effect of Repeated MDMA Treatment on the Levels of Monoamines in the Brain

DISCUSSION

This study demonstrates that, akin to other commonly abused amphetamine derivatives and cocaine (Robinson and Becker 1986; Kalivas and Stewart 1991), repeated administration of MDMA produces sensitization to the behavioral stimulant effects of an acute MDMA challenge. Also similar to other amphetamines (Robinson et al. 1988; Akimoto et al. 1990), the augmented behavioral response was accompanied by an increased capacity of MDMA to elevate extracellular dopamine content in the nucleus accumbens. The concordance between MDMA and other amphetamine derivatives and cocaine regarding the induction of behavioral and neurochemical sensitization in a rat model of amphetamine psychosis supports a hypothesis that the emergence of anxiety and psychosis associated with abuse of these drugs may arise, in part, from similar neuroadaptations. This possibility is further buttressed by the demonstration of behavioral cross-sensitization between MDMA and cocaine.

Behavioral Sensitization to MDMA

Although the automated measures of locomotion and stereotypy were augmented by repeated MDMA treatment, it is important to note that these methods do not provide detailed characterization of the behavioral profile. Indeed, the profile of augmented behaviors to other amphetamine-like stimulants depends upon which drug is used, the dose used, and reveals a high degree of individual variability (Segal and Kuczenski 1987; Kalivas et al. 1988). Thus, detailed rating of behavior would likely reveal distinctions between MDMA-induced behavioral sensitization and that elicited by repeated administration of other amphetamine-like drugs.

Although consistent with two previous reports (Spanos and Yamamoto 1989; Dafters 1995), the development of behavioral sensitization after repeated MDMA administration was not observed in three studies (Gold and Koob 1989; Callaway and Geyer 1992; McNamara et al. 1995). There are two distinctions between these studies that may have contributed to the different results. First, many studies have shown that behavioral sensitization to amphetamine derivatives and cocaine is most robust if animals are tested following an extended withdrawal period after discontinuing repeated drug administration (Kolta et al. 1985; Antelman 1988; Kalivas and Duffy 1993; Heidbreder et al. 1996). The delayed sensitization is amplified if higher dose or more frequent repeated drug injections are made (Paulson et al. 1991; Kalivas and Duffy 1993; Paulson and Robinson 1995). These time and dose relationships are reflected in the present study where at 48 h after discontinuing repeated MDMA treatment there was no behavioral sensitization in the group pretreated with the highest repeated MDMA regimen, but the group treated with the lower dosage regimen showed significantly enhanced behavioral activity. Furthermore, after 11 days of withdrawal from the repeated MDMA injections, all treatment groups demonstrated robust behavioral sensitization to a MDMA challenge. Two of the three previous studies that did not reveal behavioral sensitization to repeated MDMA examined animals only at 24 h after discontinuing the repeated treatment regimen (Gold and Koob 1989; McNamara et al. 1995). However, the study by Callaway and Geyer (1992) evaluated animals at both 36 h and 21 days after the last repeated MDMA injection and observed tolerance and no alteration, respectively, in the motor response to an acute MDMA injection. It is possible that the behavioral sensitization we observed at 11 days after the last daily MDMA injection may dissipate by 21 days. However, this seems unlikely because behavioral cross-sensitization to cocaine was observed at 19 days after discontinuing daily MDMA.

A second explanation may be related to the fact that learned associations made between the injection of amphetamine derivatives and environmental stimuli increase the expression of behavioral sensitization (Stewart and Vezina 1988). In the Callaway and Geyer (1992) study, all repeated injections of MDMA were made in an environment distinct from the test environment. In contrast, in studies showing behavioral sensitization to MDMA the animals had at least one previous exposure to MDMA in the test apparatus prior to evaluating the behavioral response to MDMA after discontinuing the repeated treatments (Spanos and Yamamoto 1989; Dafters 1995). Supporting a hypothesis that learned associations may be important in the expression of sensitization to MDMA, Gold and Koob (1989) observed that daily administration of MDMA in the test environment resulted in the production of conditioned locomotor activity to a subsequent saline injection.

The fact that behavioral augmentation was present in rats sustaining partial serotonin depletions, as well as in rats treated with a MDMA dosage regimen that was not neurotoxic indicates that serotonin depletion is not necessary for the induction of behavioral sensitization. Interestingly, both groups of rats pretreated with repeated MDMA injections showed an increase in the tissue content of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens (see Table 2). Similar increases in dopamine tissue content have been reported previously (McNamara et al. 1995) and may contribute to the augmented increase in extracellular dopamine seen in the nucleus accumbens in response to an acute MDMA injection (see below).

Neurochemical Sensitization to MDMA

Previous in vivo and in vitro studies reveal that the acute administration of MDMA increases dopamine release (Johnson et al. 1986; Yamamoto and Spanos 1988; Gough et al. 1991; White et al. 1994). The augmented increase in extracellular dopamine observed in the nucleus accumbens after an MDMA injection in rats pretreated with repeated MDMA injections is akin to what has been previously shown for other amphetamine derivatives and cocaine (Robinson et al. 1988; Pettit et al. 1990; Kalivas and Duffy 1993; Wolf et al. 1993; Paulson and Robinson 1995; Heidbreder et al. 1996). Contrasting this resemblance between repeated MDMA and other commonly abused amphetamine derivatives, the acute increase in extracellular dopamine by MDMA is attenuated by blocking serotonin transporters with serotonin uptake inhibitors or pretreating animals with 5-HT2 receptor antagonists (Yamamoto et al. 1995; Schmidt et al. 1994; Gudelsky and Nash 1996).

Considering the importance of serotonin in the acute effects of MDMA and the well-established toxic effect on serotonin-containing axons (O'Hearn et al. 1988; Wilson et al. 1989), one might expect that MDMA-induced increased dopamine transmission would be reduced after repeated MDMA injections. However, the extent of serotonin depletion is apparently not sufficient to impact the expression of sensitization, because Table 2 shows that both a neurotoxic (20 mg/kg, SC) and nonneurotoxic (5 mg/kg, SC) repeated MDMA dosing regimen elicited behavioral sensitization. In support of this conclusion, Gartside et al. (1996) observed that in spite of significant tissue depletion of serotonin, a neurotoxic regimen of MDMA did not alter basal extracellular levels of serotonin in the hippocampus or frontal cortex. Although this indicates that sufficient serotonin transmission may remain to support sensitized behavioral activity and dopamine release, fenfluramine-evoked increases in extracellular serotonin are blunted in rats pretreated with a neurotoxic regimen of MDMA (Series et al. 1994). Alternatively, it is possible that the actions of MDMA in the nucleus accumbens may be relatively less dependent upon serotonin transmission, especially after repeated MDMA administration. Callaway and Geyer (1992) found that MDMA microinjection into the nucleus accumbens elicits an increase in motor activity that is not blocked by fluoxetine, which led these authors to conclude that “the behavioral effects of S-MDMA in the nucleus accumbens may result from the catecholamine-releasing properties that S-MDMA shares with S-amphetamine.” Taken together with the present findings, it is possible that the augmented capacity of systemic MDMA to elevate extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens may, in part, contribute to the sensitized behavioral response elicited by repeated MDMA treatment. In future studies, it will be important to determine if, in contrast to the acute response, the sensitized behavioral response to MDMA is blocked by dopamine receptor antagonists.

Cross-Sensitization with Cocaine

The behavioral stimulant effect of cocaine was augmented in the group of rats that were pretreated with the highest dose of repeated MDMA. This is akin to the behavioral cross-sensitization previously reported between amphetamine and repeated MDMA injections (Callaway and Geyer 1992). The cross-sensitization among repeated MDMA, amphetamine, and cocaine highlights possible overlap in the neural adaptations mediating behavioral sensitization. Indeed, akin to MDMA, behavioral sensitization to cocaine is generally associated with augmented dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens (Pettit et al. 1990; Kalivas and Duffy 1993; Heidbreder et al. 1996). However, in contrast to this similarity, repeated cocaine is associated with enhanced responsiveness of spiny cells in the nucleus accumbens to iontophoretic application of dopamine D1 receptor agonist (Henry and White 1991), whereas repeated MDMA produces an enduring reduction in the response to D1 stimulation (White et al. 1996). Thus, although enhanced presynaptic transmission is a common neural adaptation produced by repeated cocaine, amphetamine, and MDMA, the effect of repeated drug treatment on dopamine postsynaptic responsiveness to dopamine is not a shared adaptation.

CONCLUSIONS

These data show that behavioral sensitization, as well as augmented dopamine transmission in the nucleus accumbens is produced by repeated MDMA administration in rats. This is akin to what is evoked by repeated injection of other amphetamine derivatives and cocaine and demonstrates the possibility of parallel neuroadaptations following the abuse of MDMA and amphetamines. Because the induction of behavioral and neurochemical sensitization in rodents is an animal model of amphetamine-induced psychosis and paranoia, the capacity of MDMA to elicit sensitization in rats supports the anecdotal literature (for review, see Green et al. 1995) that has documented the development of paranoia and psychosis in a subpopulation of individuals abusing MDMA.